首页 > 最新文献

Cognition最新文献

英文 中文
Perceptual plausibility of exaggerated realistic motion 夸张逼真运动的感知可信度
IF 2.8 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-07-16 DOI: 10.1016/j.cognition.2024.105880

The informal heuristic practices of the fine arts have much to offer to our understanding of the appearance of phenomenological reality. One interesting example is the use of exaggeration to enhance the illusion of liveliness in both living and nonliving subjects. This further eases the uncomfortable sense that the motion is somehow uncanny — especially with inanimate objects. We performed a series of experiments to test the effects of exaggeration on the phenomenological perception of simple animated objects — bouncing balls. A physically plausible model of a bouncing ball was augmented with a frequently used form of exaggeration known as squash and stretch. Observers were shown a series of animated balls, depicted using systematic parameterizations of the exaggeration model, and asked to rate their plausibility. A range of rendering styles provided varying levels of information as to the type of ball. In all cases, balls with small amounts of exaggeration were seen as plausible as those without any exaggeration (e.g., with veridical motion). Furthermore, when the type of ball was not specified, observers tolerated a large amount of exaggeration before judging them as implausible. When the type of ball was indicated, observers narrowed the range of acceptable exaggeration somewhat but still tolerated exaggeration well beyond that which would be physically possible. We contend that, in this case, exaggeration acts to bridge the so-called uncanny valley for artificial depictions of physical reality.

美术中非正式的启发式实践对我们理解现象学现实的表象有很大帮助。一个有趣的例子是,无论是有生命的物体还是非生命的物体,都会使用夸张的手法来增强活生生的错觉。这进一步缓解了人们对运动的不舒适感,尤其是对无生命的物体。我们进行了一系列实验,测试夸张对简单动画物体--弹跳球--的现象感知的影响。一个物理上可信的弹跳球模型被添加了一种常用的夸张形式,即挤压和拉伸。研究人员向观察者展示了一系列使用夸张模型的系统参数描述的动画球,并要求观察者对这些球的可信度进行评分。一系列的渲染风格提供了不同程度的球的类型信息。在所有情况下,有少量夸张的球与没有任何夸张的球(如真实运动的球)一样被认为是可信的。此外,当没有指明球的类型时,观察者会容忍大量的夸张,然后再判断它们不可信。当标明球的类型时,观察者会在一定程度上缩小可接受的夸张范围,但仍能容忍远远超出物理上可能的夸张。我们认为,在这种情况下,夸张在物理现实的人工描述中起到了弥合所谓 "不可思议谷 "的作用。
{"title":"Perceptual plausibility of exaggerated realistic motion","authors":"","doi":"10.1016/j.cognition.2024.105880","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.cognition.2024.105880","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>The informal heuristic practices of the fine arts have much to offer to our understanding of the appearance of phenomenological reality. One interesting example is the use of <em>exaggeration</em> to enhance the illusion of liveliness in both living and nonliving subjects. This further eases the uncomfortable sense that the motion is somehow uncanny — especially with inanimate objects. We performed a series of experiments to test the effects of exaggeration on the phenomenological perception of simple animated objects — bouncing balls. A physically plausible model of a bouncing ball was augmented with a frequently used form of exaggeration known as <em>squash and stretch</em>. Observers were shown a series of animated balls, depicted using systematic parameterizations of the exaggeration model, and asked to rate their plausibility. A range of rendering styles provided varying levels of information as to the type of ball. In all cases, balls with small amounts of exaggeration were seen as plausible as those without any exaggeration (e.g., with veridical motion). Furthermore, when the type of ball was not specified, observers tolerated a large amount of exaggeration before judging them as implausible. When the type of ball was indicated, observers narrowed the range of acceptable exaggeration somewhat but still tolerated exaggeration well beyond that which would be physically possible. We contend that, in this case, exaggeration acts to bridge the so-called <em>uncanny valley</em> for artificial depictions of physical reality.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":48455,"journal":{"name":"Cognition","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.8,"publicationDate":"2024-07-16","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0010027724001665/pdfft?md5=ee005b9ef1d1b7458e65040637eea50c&pid=1-s2.0-S0010027724001665-main.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141630284","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Hijacking limitations of working memory load to test for composition in language 利用工作记忆负荷的局限性测试语言的构成
IF 2.8 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-07-16 DOI: 10.1016/j.cognition.2024.105875

Although language depends on storage and composition, just what is stored or (de)composed remains unclear. We leveraged working memory load limitations to test for composition, hypothesizing that decomposed forms should particularly tax working memory. We focused on a well-studied paradigm, English inflectional morphology. We predicted that (compositional) regulars should be harder to maintain in working memory than (non-compositional) irregulars, using a 3-back production task. Frequency, phonology, orthography, and other potentially confounding factors were controlled for. Compared to irregulars, regulars and their accompanying −s/−ing-affixed filler items yielded more errors. Underscoring the decomposition of only regulars, regulars yielded more bare-stem (e.g., walk) and stem affixation errors (walks/walking) than irregulars, whereas irregulars yielded more past-tense-form affixation errors (broughts/tolded). In line with previous evidence that regulars can be stored under certain conditions, the regular-irregular difference held specifically for phonologically consistent (not inconsistent) regulars, in particular for both low and high frequency consistent regulars in males, but only for low frequency consistent regulars in females. Sensitivity analyses suggested the findings were robust. The study further elucidates the computation of inflected forms, and introduces a simple diagnostic for linguistic composition.

虽然语言依赖于存储和构成,但究竟存储或(去)构成了什么仍不清楚。我们利用工作记忆负荷的限制来测试构成,并假设分解形式会对工作记忆造成特别的负担。我们重点研究了一个研究得很好的范例--英语词形变化。我们预测,与(非组合)不规则词相比,(组合)规则词更难在工作记忆中保持。我们对频率、语音、正字法和其他潜在的干扰因素进行了控制。与不规则词相比,规则词及其伴随的-s/-后缀填充项产生的错误更多。与不规则词相比,规则词产生了更多的裸词干(如:walk)和词干词缀错误(walks/walking),而不规则词则产生了更多的过去式词缀错误(broughts/tolded)。与之前的证据一致,正则表达式在特定条件下可以被储存,正则表达式与不规则表达式之间的差异特别适用于语音一致(而非不一致)的正则表达式,尤其适用于男性的低频和高频一致的正则表达式,但只适用于女性的低频一致的正则表达式。敏感性分析表明研究结果是可靠的。该研究进一步阐明了转折形式的计算方法,并为语言构成引入了一种简单的诊断方法。
{"title":"Hijacking limitations of working memory load to test for composition in language","authors":"","doi":"10.1016/j.cognition.2024.105875","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.cognition.2024.105875","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Although language depends on storage and composition, just what is stored or (de)composed remains unclear. We leveraged working memory load limitations to test for composition, hypothesizing that decomposed forms should particularly tax working memory. We focused on a well-studied paradigm, English inflectional morphology. We predicted that (compositional) regulars should be harder to maintain in working memory than (non-compositional) irregulars, using a 3-back production task. Frequency, phonology, orthography, and other potentially confounding factors were controlled for. Compared to irregulars, regulars and their accompanying −<em>s</em>/<em>−ing</em>-affixed filler items yielded more errors. Underscoring the decomposition of only regulars, regulars yielded more bare-stem (e.g., <em>walk</em>) and stem affixation errors (<em>walks</em>/<em>walking</em>) than irregulars, whereas irregulars yielded more past-tense-form affixation errors (<em>broughts</em>/<em>tolded</em>). In line with previous evidence that regulars can be stored under certain conditions, the regular-irregular difference held specifically for phonologically consistent (not inconsistent) regulars, in particular for both low and high frequency consistent regulars in males, but only for low frequency consistent regulars in females. Sensitivity analyses suggested the findings were robust. The study further elucidates the computation of inflected forms, and introduces a simple diagnostic for linguistic composition.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":48455,"journal":{"name":"Cognition","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.8,"publicationDate":"2024-07-16","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141622933","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Comparing human evaluations of eyewitness statements to a machine learning classifier under pristine and suboptimal lineup administration procedures 在原始和次优列队管理程序下,将目击者陈述的人工评估与机器学习分类器进行比较
IF 2.8 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-07-14 DOI: 10.1016/j.cognition.2024.105876
Jesse H. Grabman , Ian G. Dobbins , Chad S. Dodson

Recent work highlights the ability of verbal machine learning classifiers to distinguish between accurate and inaccurate recognition memory decisions (Dobbins, 2022; Dobbins & Kantner, 2019; Seale-Carlisle, Grabman, & Dodson, 2022). Given the surge of interest in these modeling techniques, there is an urgent need to investigate verbal classifiers' limitations – particularly in applied contexts such as when police collect eyewitness's confidence statements. We find that confirmatory feedback (e.g., “This study now has a total of 87 participants, 84 of them made the same decision as you!”) weakens the relationship between identification accuracy and verbal classifier scores to a similar degree as mock witnesses' numeric confidence judgments (Experiment 1). Crucially, for the first time, we compare the discriminative value of verbal classifier scores to the ratings of human evaluators who assessed the identical verbal confidence statements (Experiment 2). Our results suggest that human evaluators outperform the classifier when mock witnesses received no feedback; however, the classifier matches (or exceeds) the performance of human evaluators when mock witnesses received confirmatory feedback. Providing lineup information to human evaluators resulted in a worse ability to distinguish between correct and filler identifications, suggesting that this particular information may encourage the use of inappropriate heuristics when rendering accuracy judgments. Overall, these results suggest that the utility of verbal classifiers may be enhanced when contextual effects (e.g., lineup presence) impair human estimates of others' performance, but that translating witnesses' statements into classifier scores will not fix the problems of an improperly conducted lineup procedure.

最近的研究突显了语言机器学习分类器区分准确和不准确识别记忆决策的能力(Dobbins,2022;Dobbins & Kantner,2019;Seale-Carlisle, Grabman, & Dodson,2022)。鉴于人们对这些建模技术的兴趣激增,迫切需要研究语言分类器的局限性--尤其是在应用环境中,如警方收集目击证人的置信陈述时。我们发现,确认性反馈(例如,"本研究现在共有 87 名参与者,其中 84 人做出了与您相同的决定!")会削弱识别准确率与口头分类器分数之间的关系,其程度与模拟证人的数字信心判断相似(实验 1)。最重要的是,我们首次将口头分类器分数的鉴别价值与人类评估者对相同口头置信度陈述的评分进行了比较(实验 2)。我们的结果表明,当模拟证人没有收到任何反馈时,人类评估者的表现优于分类器;然而,当模拟证人收到确认反馈时,分类器的表现与人类评估者的表现相当(甚至超过)。向人类评估员提供列队信息会导致他们区分正确指认和填充指认的能力下降,这表明在做出准确性判断时,这种特定信息可能会鼓励使用不恰当的启发式方法。总之,这些结果表明,当环境影响(如列队在场)损害了人类对他人表现的估计时,口头分类器的效用可能会增强,但将证人的陈述转化为分类器分数并不能解决列队程序不当的问题。
{"title":"Comparing human evaluations of eyewitness statements to a machine learning classifier under pristine and suboptimal lineup administration procedures","authors":"Jesse H. Grabman ,&nbsp;Ian G. Dobbins ,&nbsp;Chad S. Dodson","doi":"10.1016/j.cognition.2024.105876","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2024.105876","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Recent work highlights the ability of verbal machine learning classifiers to distinguish between accurate and inaccurate recognition memory decisions (Dobbins, 2022; Dobbins &amp; Kantner, 2019; Seale-Carlisle, Grabman, &amp; Dodson, 2022). Given the surge of interest in these modeling techniques, there is an urgent need to investigate verbal classifiers' limitations – particularly in applied contexts such as when police collect eyewitness's confidence statements. We find that confirmatory feedback (e.g., “This study now has a total of 87 participants, 84 of them made the same decision as you!”) weakens the relationship between identification accuracy and verbal classifier scores to a similar degree as mock witnesses' numeric confidence judgments (Experiment 1). Crucially, for the first time, we compare the discriminative value of verbal classifier scores to the ratings of human evaluators who assessed the identical verbal confidence statements (Experiment 2). Our results suggest that human evaluators outperform the classifier when mock witnesses received no feedback; however, the classifier matches (or exceeds) the performance of human evaluators when mock witnesses received confirmatory feedback. Providing lineup information to human evaluators resulted in a <em>worse</em> ability to distinguish between correct and filler identifications, suggesting that this particular information may encourage the use of inappropriate heuristics when rendering accuracy judgments. Overall, these results suggest that the utility of verbal classifiers may be enhanced when contextual effects (e.g., lineup presence) impair human estimates of others' performance, but that translating witnesses' statements into classifier scores will not fix the problems of an improperly conducted lineup procedure.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":48455,"journal":{"name":"Cognition","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.8,"publicationDate":"2024-07-14","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141605419","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Applying process dissociation to self-sacrificial moral dilemmas: Extending the dual-process model 将过程分离应用于自我牺牲的道德困境:扩展双重过程模型。
IF 2.8 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-07-09 DOI: 10.1016/j.cognition.2024.105873
David Simpson, Kyle Nash

There is considerable evidence linking cognitive reflection with utilitarian judgments in dilemmas that involve sacrificing someone else for the greater good. However, the evidence is mixed on the question of whether cognitive reflection is associated with utilitarian judgments in self-sacrificial dilemmas. We employed process dissociation to extract a self-sacrificial utilitarian (SU) parameter, an altruism (A) parameter, an other-sacrificial (OU) utilitarian parameter, and a deontology (D) parameter. In Study 1, the cognitive reflection test (CRT) positively correlated with both SU and OU (replicated in Studies 2 and 4, pre-registered). In Study 2, we found that instructing participants to rely on reason increased SU and OU (replicated in Study 4, pre-registered). In Study 3, we found that SU and OU positively correlated with giving in the single-game version of the public goods game (replicated in Study 4, pre-registered), which provides behavioral validation that they are genuine moral tendencies. Together, these studies constitute strong cumulative evidence that SU and OU are both valid measures that are associated with reliance on cognitive reflection.

有相当多的证据表明,在涉及为更大利益而牺牲他人的困境中,认知反思与功利判断有关。然而,对于认知反思是否与自我牺牲困境中的功利判断相关这一问题,证据却不尽相同。我们采用过程分离法提取了自我牺牲功利主义(SU)参数、利他主义(A)参数、他者牺牲功利主义(OU)参数和去道德主义(D)参数。在研究 1 中,认知反思测试(CRT)与 SU 和 OU 均呈正相关(在研究 2 和研究 4 中复制,预注册)。在研究 2 中,我们发现指导参与者依靠理性会增加 SU 和 OU(在研究 4 中重复,预先注册)。在研究 3 中,我们发现 SU 和 OU 与单局版公益游戏中的捐赠正相关(在研究 4 中重复,预先注册),这为它们是真正的道德倾向提供了行为验证。这些研究共同构成了有力的累积证据,证明 "自我评价 "和 "他人评价 "都是与依赖认知反思相关的有效测量指标。
{"title":"Applying process dissociation to self-sacrificial moral dilemmas: Extending the dual-process model","authors":"David Simpson,&nbsp;Kyle Nash","doi":"10.1016/j.cognition.2024.105873","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.cognition.2024.105873","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>There is considerable evidence linking cognitive reflection with utilitarian judgments in dilemmas that involve sacrificing someone else for the greater good. However, the evidence is mixed on the question of whether cognitive reflection is associated with utilitarian judgments in self-sacrificial dilemmas. We employed process dissociation to extract a self-sacrificial utilitarian (SU) parameter, an altruism (A) parameter, an other-sacrificial (OU) utilitarian parameter, and a deontology (D) parameter. In Study 1, the cognitive reflection test (CRT) positively correlated with both SU and OU (replicated in Studies 2 and 4, pre-registered). In Study 2, we found that instructing participants to rely on reason increased SU and OU (replicated in Study 4, pre-registered). In Study 3, we found that SU and OU positively correlated with giving in the single-game version of the public goods game (replicated in Study 4, pre-registered), which provides behavioral validation that they are genuine moral tendencies. Together, these studies constitute strong cumulative evidence that SU and OU are both valid measures that are associated with reliance on cognitive reflection.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":48455,"journal":{"name":"Cognition","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.8,"publicationDate":"2024-07-09","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0010027724001598/pdfft?md5=e86318dd350dc083310182e27e322242&pid=1-s2.0-S0010027724001598-main.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141581168","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Language-general versus language-specific processes in bilingual voice learning 双语语音学习中的语言一般过程与语言特定过程
IF 2.8 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-07-05 DOI: 10.1016/j.cognition.2024.105866
Line Lloy, Khushi Nilesh Patil, Khia A. Johnson, Molly Babel

Language experience confers a benefit to voice learning, a concept described in the literature as the language familiarity effect (LFE). What experiences are necessary for the LFE to be conferred is less clear. We contribute empirically and theoretically to this debate by examining within and across language voice learning with Cantonese-English bilingual voices in a talker-voice association paradigm. Listeners were trained in Cantonese or English and assessed on their abilities to generalize voice learning at test on Cantonese and English utterances. By testing listeners from four language backgrounds – English Monolingual, Cantonese-English Multilingual, Tone Multilingual, and Non-tone Multilingual groups – we assess whether the LFE and group-level differences in voice learning are due to varying abilities (1) in accessing the relative acoustic-phonetic features that distinguish a voice, (2) learning at a given rate, or (3) generalizing learning of talker-voice associations to novel same-language and different-language utterances. The specific four language background groups allow us to investigate the roles of language-specific familiarity, tone language experience, and generic multilingual experience in voice learning. Differences in performance across listener groups shows evidence in support of the LFE and the role of two mechanisms for voice learning: the extraction and association of talker-specific, language-general information that is more robustly generalized across languages, and talker-specific, language-specific information that may be more readily accessible and learnable, but due to its language-specific nature, is less able to be extended to another language.

语言经验有利于语音学习,这一概念在文献中被称为语言熟悉效应(LFE)。目前还不太清楚哪些经验是产生 LFE 的必要条件。我们通过在说话者-声音联想范式中使用粤英双语语音对语言内和语言间的语音学习进行了研究,从而在经验和理论上为这一争论做出了贡献。听者接受了粤语或英语训练,并在粤语和英语语篇测试中对其语音学习归纳能力进行了评估。通过测试四种语言背景的听者--单语英语组、粤英多语种组、音调多语种组和非音调多语种组--我们评估了语音学习中的低频和组间差异是否是由于以下能力的不同造成的:(1)获取区分语音的相对声学-音学特征的能力;(2)以特定速度学习的能力;或(3)将谈话者-语音联想的学习推广到新的同语和异语语料中的能力。通过特定的四种语言背景组,我们可以研究特定语言的熟悉程度、语调语言经验和通用多语言经验在语音学习中的作用。不同听者群体的表现差异表明,有证据支持 LFE 以及两种语音学习机制的作用:一种是提取和联想说话者特定的、语言通用的信息,这种信息在不同语言之间具有更强的通用性;另一种是提取和联想说话者特定的、语言特定的信息,这种信息可能更容易获得和学习,但由于其语言特定性,较难扩展到另一种语言。
{"title":"Language-general versus language-specific processes in bilingual voice learning","authors":"Line Lloy,&nbsp;Khushi Nilesh Patil,&nbsp;Khia A. Johnson,&nbsp;Molly Babel","doi":"10.1016/j.cognition.2024.105866","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2024.105866","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Language experience confers a benefit to voice learning, a concept described in the literature as the language familiarity effect (LFE). What experiences are necessary for the LFE to be conferred is less clear. We contribute empirically and theoretically to this debate by examining within and across language voice learning with Cantonese-English bilingual voices in a talker-voice association paradigm. Listeners were trained in Cantonese or English and assessed on their abilities to generalize voice learning at test on Cantonese and English utterances. By testing listeners from four language backgrounds – English Monolingual, Cantonese-English Multilingual, Tone Multilingual, and Non-tone Multilingual groups – we assess whether the LFE and group-level differences in voice learning are due to varying abilities (1) in accessing the relative acoustic-phonetic features that distinguish a voice, (2) learning at a given rate, or (3) generalizing learning of talker-voice associations to novel same-language and different-language utterances. The specific four language background groups allow us to investigate the roles of language-specific familiarity, tone language experience, and generic multilingual experience in voice learning. Differences in performance across listener groups shows evidence in support of the LFE and the role of two mechanisms for voice learning: the extraction and association of talker-specific, language-general information that is more robustly generalized across languages, and talker-specific, language-specific information that may be more readily accessible and learnable, but due to its language-specific nature, is less able to be extended to another language.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":48455,"journal":{"name":"Cognition","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.8,"publicationDate":"2024-07-05","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0010027724001525/pdfft?md5=46ccac7aa9a26b0262574c2dce610408&pid=1-s2.0-S0010027724001525-main.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141539249","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Motion extrapolation in sport expertise: Representational momentum and representational gravity in volleyball athletes 运动专业知识中的运动外推法:排球运动员的表象动量和表象重力
IF 2.8 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-07-05 DOI: 10.1016/j.cognition.2024.105869
Mariana M. Rodrigues , Nuno De Sá Teixeira

When people indicate the vanishing location of a moving target that suddenly disappears, systematic errors forward (in the direction of motion) and downward (in the direction of gravity) emerge. These spatial displacements were coined, respectively, Representational Momentum and Representational Gravity, and are believed to reflect internalized ecologically relevant physical invariants useful for the anticipation of future states of an event. Previous research has shown that sports athletes exhibit increased Representational Momentum, indicating enhanced motion extrapolation and anticipation, albeit it is still not clear up to what degree this effect is specific for the expertise context or if it generalizes to other dynamic events. Furthermore, the influence of expertise on Representational Gravity, particularly in contexts where anticipation of vertically moving objects is crucial, remains understudied. This study aimed to address these gaps by focusing on Volleyball as a context of expertise due to the prevalence of fast vertically moving balls. Volleyball athletes and non-athletes indicated the perceived offset location of a smoothly moving target, which moved at a constant speed or was subjected to acceleration/deceleration, embedded either in a Volleyball or neutral context. Outcomes revealed that for the Volleyball context, athletes, but not non-athletes, revealed a significant trend to misperceive targets moving along the left diagonal to be further displaced forward beyond what would be expected due to Representational Momentum alone. This finding is discussed in relation to the natural statistics of Volleyball games, where crossed ball trajectories, particularly by the outside hitter, are more prevalent, fast, and offensive, requiring better anticipation to be efficiently dealt with.

当人们指出一个突然消失的移动目标的消失位置时,会出现向前(运动方向)和向下(重力方向)的系统误差。这些空间位移分别被称为 "表象动量 "和 "表象重力",被认为反映了内化的、与生态相关的物理不变性,有助于预测事件的未来状态。以往的研究表明,体育运动员的表征动量增加,表明运动外推和预测能力增强,但这种影响在多大程度上是针对专业背景的,或者是否能推广到其他动态事件,目前尚不清楚。此外,对于专业知识对表象重力的影响,尤其是在对垂直运动物体的预测至关重要的情况下,研究仍然不足。本研究旨在弥补这些不足,研究重点是排球,因为排球是一种快速垂直运动的球类运动。排球运动员和非运动员指出了一个平滑移动目标的感知偏移位置,该目标以恒定速度移动或受到加速/减速影响,嵌入排球或中性环境中。结果显示,在排球情境中,运动员(而非非运动员)对沿左侧对角线移动的目标有明显的误解趋势,认为其向前移动的距离超出了仅凭表象动量所能预期的距离。这一发现与排球比赛的自然统计有关,在排球比赛中,交叉球轨迹,尤其是外线击球手的交叉球轨迹,更加普遍、快速和具有攻击性,需要更好的预测才能有效处理。
{"title":"Motion extrapolation in sport expertise: Representational momentum and representational gravity in volleyball athletes","authors":"Mariana M. Rodrigues ,&nbsp;Nuno De Sá Teixeira","doi":"10.1016/j.cognition.2024.105869","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2024.105869","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>When people indicate the vanishing location of a moving target that suddenly disappears, systematic errors forward (in the direction of motion) and downward (in the direction of gravity) emerge. These spatial displacements were coined, respectively, Representational Momentum and Representational Gravity, and are believed to reflect internalized ecologically relevant physical invariants useful for the anticipation of future states of an event. Previous research has shown that sports athletes exhibit increased Representational Momentum, indicating enhanced motion extrapolation and anticipation, albeit it is still not clear up to what degree this effect is specific for the expertise context or if it generalizes to other dynamic events. Furthermore, the influence of expertise on Representational Gravity, particularly in contexts where anticipation of vertically moving objects is crucial, remains understudied. This study aimed to address these gaps by focusing on Volleyball as a context of expertise due to the prevalence of fast vertically moving balls. Volleyball athletes and non-athletes indicated the perceived offset location of a smoothly moving target, which moved at a constant speed or was subjected to acceleration/deceleration, embedded either in a Volleyball or neutral context. Outcomes revealed that for the Volleyball context, athletes, but not non-athletes, revealed a significant trend to misperceive targets moving along the left diagonal to be further displaced forward beyond what would be expected due to Representational Momentum alone. This finding is discussed in relation to the natural statistics of Volleyball games, where crossed ball trajectories, particularly by the outside hitter, are more prevalent, fast, and offensive, requiring better anticipation to be efficiently dealt with.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":48455,"journal":{"name":"Cognition","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.8,"publicationDate":"2024-07-05","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0010027724001550/pdfft?md5=eb6619c90f5b6b530540c360a4a89ced&pid=1-s2.0-S0010027724001550-main.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141539248","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Mechanisms of visual working memory processing task-irrelevant information retrieved from visual long-term memory 视觉工作记忆处理从视觉长时记忆中检索的任务相关信息的机制
IF 2.8 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-07-04 DOI: 10.1016/j.cognition.2024.105871
Yanliang Sun , Lixue Wang , Wenhao Yu , Xue Yang , Jiaru Song , Shouxin Li

Visual working memory (VWM) can selectively filter task-irrelevant information from incoming visual stimuli. However, whether a similar filtering process applies to task-irrelevant information retrieved from visual long-term memory (VLTM) remains elusive. We assume a “resource-limited retrieval mechanism” in VWM in charge of the retrieval of irrelevant VLTM information. To make a comprehensive understanding of this mechanism, we conducted three experiments using both a VLTM learning task and a VWM task combined with pupillometry. The presence of a significant pupil light response (PLR) served as empirical evidence that VLTM information can indeed make its way into VWM. Notably, task-relevant VLTM information induced a sustained PLR, contrasting with the transient PLR observed for task-irrelevant VLTM information. Importantly, the transience of the PLR occurred under conditions of low VWM load, but this effect was absent under conditions of high load. Collectively, these results show that task-irrelevant VLTM information can enter VWM and then fade away only under conditions of low VWM load. This dynamic underscores the resource-limited retrieval mechanism within VWM, exerting control over the entry of VLTM information.

视觉工作记忆(VWM)可以从输入的视觉刺激中选择性地过滤与任务无关的信息。然而,从视觉长时记忆(VLTM)中检索出的与任务无关的信息是否也有类似的过滤过程,这一点仍然令人费解。我们假设视觉长时记忆中有一个 "资源有限检索机制",负责检索视觉长时记忆中的无关信息。为了全面了解这一机制,我们进行了三项实验,分别使用了VLTM学习任务和结合瞳孔测量法的VWM任务。明显的瞳孔光反应(PLR)的存在是 VLTM 信息确实能进入 VWM 的实证证据。值得注意的是,与任务相关的 VLTM 信息会引起持续的瞳孔光反应,而与任务无关的 VLTM 信息则会引起短暂的瞳孔光反应。重要的是,PLR 的瞬时性发生在 VWM 负荷较低的条件下,而在负载较高的条件下则不存在这种效应。总之,这些结果表明,与任务无关的 VLTM 信息只有在 VWM 负荷较低的情况下才能进入 VWM,然后逐渐消失。这种动态变化凸显了 VWM 中的资源有限检索机制对 VLTM 信息进入的控制。
{"title":"Mechanisms of visual working memory processing task-irrelevant information retrieved from visual long-term memory","authors":"Yanliang Sun ,&nbsp;Lixue Wang ,&nbsp;Wenhao Yu ,&nbsp;Xue Yang ,&nbsp;Jiaru Song ,&nbsp;Shouxin Li","doi":"10.1016/j.cognition.2024.105871","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.cognition.2024.105871","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Visual working memory (VWM) can selectively filter task-irrelevant information from incoming visual stimuli. However, whether a similar filtering process applies to task-irrelevant information retrieved from visual long-term memory (VLTM) remains elusive. We assume a “resource-limited retrieval mechanism” in VWM in charge of the retrieval of irrelevant VLTM information. To make a comprehensive understanding of this mechanism, we conducted three experiments using both a VLTM learning task and a VWM task combined with pupillometry. The presence of a significant pupil light response (PLR) served as empirical evidence that VLTM information can indeed make its way into VWM. Notably, task-relevant VLTM information induced a sustained PLR, contrasting with the transient PLR observed for task-irrelevant VLTM information. Importantly, the transience of the PLR occurred under conditions of low VWM load, but this effect was absent under conditions of high load. Collectively, these results show that task-irrelevant VLTM information can enter VWM and then fade away only under conditions of low VWM load. This dynamic underscores the resource-limited retrieval mechanism within VWM, exerting control over the entry of VLTM information.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":48455,"journal":{"name":"Cognition","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.8,"publicationDate":"2024-07-04","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141538706","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Forever young: The end of history illusion in children 永远年轻儿童历史幻觉的终结
IF 2.8 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-07-02 DOI: 10.1016/j.cognition.2024.105867
Alexa Sacchi, Jessica Sah, Melissa Finlay, Christina Starmans

The “end of history” illusion in adults (Quoidbach et al., 2013) is an asymmetrical pattern in which people accept that they've changed in the past but don't believe they will change in the future. We explore here whether the same psychological forces that cause the illusion in adults exist in the minds of children. Two studies with 4- to 11-year-olds (N = 256) suggest that they do, even in a within-subject design where the same child is asked questions about the past and the future. A third study (N = 83) finds that this illusion does not persist when children are asked about other people. These studies suggest that even young children believe that although they used to be different in the past, from this point on, they will remain forever young.

成年人的 "历史终结 "错觉(Qoidbach 等人,2013 年)是一种不对称的模式,在这种模式中,人们接受自己在过去发生了改变,但不相信自己在未来会发生改变。在此,我们将探讨导致成人产生错觉的心理力量是否也存在于儿童的头脑中。两项针对 4 至 11 岁儿童(人数=256)的研究表明,即使是在同一研究对象中,同一个儿童被问及有关过去和未来的问题,也会产生同样的错觉。第三项研究(N = 83)发现,当儿童被问及其他人时,这种错觉并不会持续存在。这些研究表明,即使年幼的儿童也会相信,虽然他们过去与现在不同,但从现在开始,他们将永远年轻。
{"title":"Forever young: The end of history illusion in children","authors":"Alexa Sacchi,&nbsp;Jessica Sah,&nbsp;Melissa Finlay,&nbsp;Christina Starmans","doi":"10.1016/j.cognition.2024.105867","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2024.105867","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>The “end of history” illusion in adults (Quoidbach et al., 2013) is an asymmetrical pattern in which people accept that they've changed in the past but don't believe they will change in the future. We explore here whether the same psychological forces that cause the illusion in adults exist in the minds of children. Two studies with 4- to 11-year-olds (<em>N</em> = 256) suggest that they do, even in a within-subject design where the same child is asked questions about the past and the future. A third study (<em>N</em> = 83) finds that this illusion does not persist when children are asked about other people. These studies suggest that even young children believe that although they used to be different in the past, from this point on, they will remain forever young.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":48455,"journal":{"name":"Cognition","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.8,"publicationDate":"2024-07-02","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141484010","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Asymmetries in encoding event roles: Evidence from language and cognition 事件角色编码的不对称性:来自语言和认知的证据
IF 2.8 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-07-02 DOI: 10.1016/j.cognition.2024.105868
Ercenur Ünal , Frances Wilson , John Trueswell , Anna Papafragou

It has long been hypothesized that the linguistic structure of events, including event participants and their relative prominence, draws on the non-linguistic nature of events and the roles that these events license. However, the precise relation between the prominence of event participants in language and cognition has not been tested experimentally in a systematic way. Here we address this gap. In four experiments, we investigate the relative prominence of (animate) Agents, Patients, Goals and Instruments in the linguistic encoding of complex events and the prominence of these event roles in cognition as measured by visual search and change blindness tasks. The relative prominence of these event roles was largely similar—though not identical—across linguistic and non-linguistic measures. Across linguistic and non-linguistic tasks, Patients were more salient than Goals, which were more salient than Instruments. (Animate) Agents were more salient than Patients in linguistic descriptions and visual search; however, this asymmetrical pattern did not emerge in change detection. Overall, our results reveal homologies between the linguistic and non-linguistic prominence of individual event participants, thereby lending support to the claim that the linguistic structure of events builds on underlying conceptual event representations. We discuss implications of these findings for linguistic theory and theories of event cognition.

长期以来,人们一直假设,事件的语言结构(包括事件参与者及其相对突出地位)借鉴了事件的非语言性质以及这些事件所许可的角色。然而,事件参与者在语言和认知中的突出地位之间的确切关系尚未得到系统的实验检验。在此,我们将弥补这一空白。在四个实验中,我们研究了(有生命的)代理、患者、目标和工具在复杂事件的语言编码中的相对突出地位,以及这些事件角色在认知中的突出地位,并通过视觉搜索和变化盲任务进行了测量。在语言和非语言测量中,这些事件角色的相对重要性大体相似,但并不完全相同。在语言和非语言任务中,"患者 "比 "目标 "更突出,而 "目标 "比 "工具 "更突出。(在语言描述和视觉搜索中,"(有生命的)代理 "比 "患者 "更突出;然而,这种不对称模式并没有出现在变化检测中。总之,我们的研究结果揭示了单个事件参与者的语言和非语言显著性之间的同源性,从而支持了事件的语言结构建立在潜在概念事件表征基础之上的说法。我们将讨论这些发现对语言理论和事件认知理论的影响。
{"title":"Asymmetries in encoding event roles: Evidence from language and cognition","authors":"Ercenur Ünal ,&nbsp;Frances Wilson ,&nbsp;John Trueswell ,&nbsp;Anna Papafragou","doi":"10.1016/j.cognition.2024.105868","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.cognition.2024.105868","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>It has long been hypothesized that the linguistic structure of events, including event participants and their relative prominence, draws on the non-linguistic nature of events and the roles that these events license. However, the precise relation between the prominence of event participants in language and cognition has not been tested experimentally in a systematic way. Here we address this gap. In four experiments, we investigate the relative prominence of (animate) Agents, Patients, Goals and Instruments in the linguistic encoding of complex events and the prominence of these event roles in cognition as measured by visual search and change blindness tasks. The relative prominence of these event roles was largely similar—though not identical—across linguistic and non-linguistic measures. Across linguistic and non-linguistic tasks, Patients were more salient than Goals, which were more salient than Instruments. (Animate) Agents were more salient than Patients in linguistic descriptions and visual search; however, this asymmetrical pattern did not emerge in change detection. Overall, our results reveal homologies between the linguistic and non-linguistic prominence of individual event participants, thereby lending support to the claim that the linguistic structure of events builds on underlying conceptual event representations. We discuss implications of these findings for linguistic theory and theories of event cognition.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":48455,"journal":{"name":"Cognition","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.8,"publicationDate":"2024-07-02","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141499333","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Children's representation of coincidence 儿童对巧合的表述。
IF 2.8 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-06-27 DOI: 10.1016/j.cognition.2024.105854
Qiong Cao, Lisa Feigenson

People relish thinking about coincidences—we puzzle over their meanings and delight in conveying our experiences of them to others. But whereas some research has begun to explore how coincidences are represented by adults, little is known about the early development of these representations. Here we explored factors influencing coincidence representations in both adults and children. Across two experiments, participants read stories describing co-occurring events and then judged whether these constituted coincidences. In Experiment 1 we found that adults' coincidence judgments were highly sensitive to the presence or absence of plausible explanations: as expected, adults were more likely to judge co-occurrences as a coincidence when no explanation was available. Importantly, their coincidence judgments were also modulated by the number of events that co-occurred. Adults tended to reject scenarios involving too many co-occurring events as coincidences regardless of whether an explanation was present, suggesting that observing suspiciously many co-occurrences triggered them to infer their own underlying explanation (and thus blocking the events' interpretation as a coincidence). In Experiment 2 we found that 4- to 10-year-old children also represent coincidences, and identify them via the absence of plausible explanations. Older children, like adults, rejected suspiciously large numbers of co-occurring events as coincidental, whereas younger children did not exhibit this sensitivity. Overall, these results suggest that representation of coincidence is available from early in life, but undergoes developmental change during the early school-age years.

人们乐于思考巧合--我们困惑于巧合的含义,并乐于向他人传达我们对巧合的体验。然而,尽管一些研究已经开始探索成人如何表征巧合,但人们对这些表征的早期发展却知之甚少。在这里,我们探讨了影响成人和儿童巧合表征的因素。在两个实验中,参与者阅读描述共同发生事件的故事,然后判断这些故事是否构成巧合。在实验 1 中,我们发现成人的巧合判断对是否存在合理解释高度敏感:正如预期的那样,在没有解释的情况下,成人更倾向于将共同发生的事件判断为巧合。重要的是,他们对巧合的判断还受到共同发生事件数量的影响。无论是否存在解释,成人都倾向于拒绝将涉及过多共同发生事件的情景视为巧合,这表明观察到可疑的过多共同发生事件会触发他们推断自己的潜在解释(从而阻止将事件解释为巧合)。在实验 2 中,我们发现 4 至 10 岁的儿童也会表示巧合,并通过缺乏合理解释来识别巧合。年龄较大的儿童和成人一样,会将大量可疑的同时发生的事件视为巧合而加以拒绝,而年龄较小的儿童则没有表现出这种敏感性。总之,这些结果表明,巧合的表征从生命早期就开始存在,但在学龄早期会发生发展变化。
{"title":"Children's representation of coincidence","authors":"Qiong Cao,&nbsp;Lisa Feigenson","doi":"10.1016/j.cognition.2024.105854","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.cognition.2024.105854","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>People relish thinking about coincidences—we puzzle over their meanings and delight in conveying our experiences of them to others. But whereas some research has begun to explore how coincidences are represented by adults, little is known about the early development of these representations. Here we explored factors influencing coincidence representations in both adults and children. Across two experiments, participants read stories describing co-occurring events and then judged whether these constituted coincidences. In Experiment 1 we found that adults' coincidence judgments were highly sensitive to the presence or absence of plausible explanations: as expected, adults were more likely to judge co-occurrences as a coincidence when no explanation was available. Importantly, their coincidence judgments were also modulated by the number of events that co-occurred. Adults tended to reject scenarios involving too many co-occurring events as coincidences regardless of whether an explanation was present, suggesting that observing suspiciously many co-occurrences triggered them to infer their own underlying explanation (and thus blocking the events' interpretation as a coincidence). In Experiment 2 we found that 4- to 10-year-old children also represent coincidences, and identify them via the absence of plausible explanations. Older children, like adults, rejected suspiciously large numbers of co-occurring events as coincidental, whereas younger children did not exhibit this sensitivity. Overall, these results suggest that representation of coincidence is available from early in life, but undergoes developmental change during the early school-age years.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":48455,"journal":{"name":"Cognition","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.8,"publicationDate":"2024-06-27","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141471643","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Cognition
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1