首页 > 最新文献

Perception最新文献

英文 中文
Book review: Physiological Influences of Music in Perception and Action by Shannon E. Wright, Valentin Bégel, and Caroline Palmer 书评:《音乐在感知和行动中的生理影响》,作者:香农·e·赖特、瓦伦丁·巴格尔和卡罗琳·帕尔默
4区 心理学 Q3 OPHTHALMOLOGY Pub Date : 2023-10-19 DOI: 10.1177/03010066231208913
Kentaro Ono
{"title":"Book review: <i>Physiological Influences of Music in Perception and Action</i> by Shannon E. Wright, Valentin Bégel, and Caroline Palmer","authors":"Kentaro Ono","doi":"10.1177/03010066231208913","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1177/03010066231208913","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":49708,"journal":{"name":"Perception","volume":"25 1","pages":"0"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2023-10-19","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"135780392","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Obituary: Vincent Hayward (1955-2023). 讣告:文森特·海沃德(1955-2023)。
IF 1.7 4区 心理学 Q3 OPHTHALMOLOGY Pub Date : 2023-10-01 Epub Date: 2023-09-07 DOI: 10.1177/03010066231198763
Mounia Ziat
On May 10th, 2023, the world lost a remarkable scientist and polymath, Vincent Hayward (1955–2023). His departure leaves a void in the scientific community, and we will deeply miss his intellectual brilliance, eclectic personality, profound humility, and legendary laughter. Vincent was a haptician, captivated by our ability to perceive the world through the sense of touch. His research was a unique blend of engineering, psychology, psychophysics, neuroscience, and philosophy, focused in large part on the perceptual skin and human behavior. Vincent’s academic journey began as an undergraduate at Ecole Centrale of Nantes in France, where he earned his engineering degree in 1978. Subsequently, he pursued his PhD at LIMSI (“Laboratoire d’informatique pour la mécanique et les sciences de l’ingénieur” in Orsay, France) in 1981, focusing on real-time optimization computer programs for robotics control. Afterward, he secured two positions at Purdue University in the United States, first as a Postdoctoral Fellow and later as a Visiting Assistant Professor (1981–1983), collaborating with Professor Richard P. Paul on developing the first control library for advanced industrial robots and exploring force feedback integration. In 1983, he returned to France as a Research Officer (Chargé de Recherches) at the French National Centre for Scientific Research, working on trajectory planning and spatial reasoning. In 1987, Vincent joined the Department of Electrical and Computer Engineering at McGill University in Canada as an assistant, associate (1994), and then full professor (2006). From 2001 to 2004, he served as the Director of the McGill Center for Intelligent Machines. During his early years at McGill, his research centered on robot programming and control, 3-D imaging, computational geometry, spatial reasoning, computational architectures, and space and remote applications of robotics and telerobotics. His interests shifted, between 1992 and 1999, from robotics to the sense of touch, leading him to delve deeply into multidisciplinary research by integrating biology, psychophysics, neuroscience, philosophy, and design. At McGill University, Vincent earned international recognition for his groundbreaking work in robotics and haptics. In 2008, Vincent returned to France, where he held an international chair at ISIR (“Institut des Systèmes Intelligents et de Robotique” in Paris) linked to the Pierre and Marie Curie University. He became one of the first French recipients of the European Research Council (ERC) advanced grant in 2009, followed by the prestigious ERC Proof of Concept Grant in 2014. Supported by a Leverhulme Trust fellowship, Vincent took a leave of absence between 2017 and 2018 to serve as a Professor of Tactile Perception and Technology at the School of Advanced Studies, University of London, UK. Simultaneously, Vincent cofounded Actronika, a Paris-based start-up dedicated to haptic technology. From 2016, he was passionately investing his time i
{"title":"Obituary: Vincent Hayward (1955-2023).","authors":"Mounia Ziat","doi":"10.1177/03010066231198763","DOIUrl":"10.1177/03010066231198763","url":null,"abstract":"On May 10th, 2023, the world lost a remarkable scientist and polymath, Vincent Hayward (1955–2023). His departure leaves a void in the scientific community, and we will deeply miss his intellectual brilliance, eclectic personality, profound humility, and legendary laughter. Vincent was a haptician, captivated by our ability to perceive the world through the sense of touch. His research was a unique blend of engineering, psychology, psychophysics, neuroscience, and philosophy, focused in large part on the perceptual skin and human behavior. Vincent’s academic journey began as an undergraduate at Ecole Centrale of Nantes in France, where he earned his engineering degree in 1978. Subsequently, he pursued his PhD at LIMSI (“Laboratoire d’informatique pour la mécanique et les sciences de l’ingénieur” in Orsay, France) in 1981, focusing on real-time optimization computer programs for robotics control. Afterward, he secured two positions at Purdue University in the United States, first as a Postdoctoral Fellow and later as a Visiting Assistant Professor (1981–1983), collaborating with Professor Richard P. Paul on developing the first control library for advanced industrial robots and exploring force feedback integration. In 1983, he returned to France as a Research Officer (Chargé de Recherches) at the French National Centre for Scientific Research, working on trajectory planning and spatial reasoning. In 1987, Vincent joined the Department of Electrical and Computer Engineering at McGill University in Canada as an assistant, associate (1994), and then full professor (2006). From 2001 to 2004, he served as the Director of the McGill Center for Intelligent Machines. During his early years at McGill, his research centered on robot programming and control, 3-D imaging, computational geometry, spatial reasoning, computational architectures, and space and remote applications of robotics and telerobotics. His interests shifted, between 1992 and 1999, from robotics to the sense of touch, leading him to delve deeply into multidisciplinary research by integrating biology, psychophysics, neuroscience, philosophy, and design. At McGill University, Vincent earned international recognition for his groundbreaking work in robotics and haptics. In 2008, Vincent returned to France, where he held an international chair at ISIR (“Institut des Systèmes Intelligents et de Robotique” in Paris) linked to the Pierre and Marie Curie University. He became one of the first French recipients of the European Research Council (ERC) advanced grant in 2009, followed by the prestigious ERC Proof of Concept Grant in 2014. Supported by a Leverhulme Trust fellowship, Vincent took a leave of absence between 2017 and 2018 to serve as a Professor of Tactile Perception and Technology at the School of Advanced Studies, University of London, UK. Simultaneously, Vincent cofounded Actronika, a Paris-based start-up dedicated to haptic technology. From 2016, he was passionately investing his time i","PeriodicalId":49708,"journal":{"name":"Perception","volume":"52 10","pages":"752-756"},"PeriodicalIF":1.7,"publicationDate":"2023-10-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10305797","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Detection cost: A nonnegligible factor contributing to inhibition of return in the discrimination task under the cue-target paradigm. 检测成本:在线索-目标范式下,在辨别任务中,抑制返回是一个不可忽略的因素。
IF 1.7 4区 心理学 Q3 OPHTHALMOLOGY Pub Date : 2023-10-01 Epub Date: 2023-08-01 DOI: 10.1177/03010066231190216
Ming Zhang, Guangyao Zu, Aijun Wang

The three-factor model argues that the spatial orienting benefit triggered by the cue, the spatial selection benefit of cue-target matching, and the detection cost of distinguishing the cue from the target contribute to the measured inhibition of return (IOR) effect. According to the three-factor model, the spatial selection benefit dominates the occurrence of the IOR effect in the discrimination task, while the detection cost is negligible. The present study verified the three-factor model in the discrimination task under the cue-target paradigm by manipulating the spatial location and nonspatial feature consistency of the cue and the target as well as the promotion or hindrance of attentional disengagement from the cued location with a central reorienting cue. The results indicated that the three factors of the three-factor model contributed to the measured IOR effect in the discrimination task. Interestingly, the IOR effect was stable when the cue and target were perfectly repeated and attention was maintained at the cued location, implying that detection cost was not a negligible factor. The current study supported the contribution of all three factors in the three-factor model to the measured IOR effect; however, we argue that the role of detection cost in the discrimination task under different paradigms should be further refined.

三因素模型认为,线索触发的空间定向效益、线索-目标匹配的空间选择效益以及区分线索和目标的检测成本有助于测量的返回抑制(IOR)效应。根据三因素模型,在判别任务中,空间选择效益主导IOR效应的发生,而检测成本可以忽略不计。本研究通过操纵线索和目标的空间位置和非空间特征一致性,以及利用中心重定向线索促进或阻碍注意力从线索位置脱离,验证了线索-目标范式下辨别任务中的三因素模型。结果表明,三因素模型中的三个因素对判别任务中测量的IOR效应有贡献。有趣的是,当线索和目标完美重复,注意力保持在提示位置时,IOR效应是稳定的,这意味着检测成本不是一个可以忽略的因素。目前的研究支持三因素模型中所有三个因素对测量IOR效应的贡献;然而,我们认为,在不同范式下,检测成本在判别任务中的作用应该进一步细化。
{"title":"Detection cost: A nonnegligible factor contributing to inhibition of return in the discrimination task under the cue-target paradigm.","authors":"Ming Zhang,&nbsp;Guangyao Zu,&nbsp;Aijun Wang","doi":"10.1177/03010066231190216","DOIUrl":"10.1177/03010066231190216","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The three-factor model argues that the spatial orienting benefit triggered by the cue, the spatial selection benefit of cue-target matching, and the detection cost of distinguishing the cue from the target contribute to the measured inhibition of return (IOR) effect. According to the three-factor model, the spatial selection benefit dominates the occurrence of the IOR effect in the discrimination task, while the detection cost is negligible. The present study verified the three-factor model in the discrimination task under the cue-target paradigm by manipulating the spatial location and nonspatial feature consistency of the cue and the target as well as the promotion or hindrance of attentional disengagement from the cued location with a central reorienting cue. The results indicated that the three factors of the three-factor model contributed to the measured IOR effect in the discrimination task. Interestingly, the IOR effect was stable when the cue and target were perfectly repeated and attention was maintained at the cued location, implying that detection cost was not a negligible factor. The current study supported the contribution of all three factors in the three-factor model to the measured IOR effect; however, we argue that the role of detection cost in the discrimination task under different paradigms should be further refined.</p>","PeriodicalId":49708,"journal":{"name":"Perception","volume":"52 10","pages":"681-694"},"PeriodicalIF":1.7,"publicationDate":"2023-10-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10294222","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Emotion category-modulated interpretation bias in perceiving ambiguous facial expressions. 情绪范畴在感知模糊面部表情时调节了解释偏差。
IF 1.7 4区 心理学 Q3 OPHTHALMOLOGY Pub Date : 2023-10-01 Epub Date: 2023-07-10 DOI: 10.1177/03010066231186936
Emily Todd, Shaini Subendran, George Wright, Kun Guo

In contrast to prototypical facial expressions, we show less perceptual tolerance in perceiving vague expressions by demonstrating an interpretation bias, such as more frequent perception of anger or happiness when categorizing ambiguous expressions of angry and happy faces that are morphed in different proportions and displayed under high- or low-quality conditions. However, it remains unclear whether this interpretation bias is specific to emotion categories or reflects a general negativity versus positivity bias and whether the degree of this bias is affected by the valence or category of two morphed expressions. These questions were examined in two eye-tracking experiments by systematically manipulating expression ambiguity and image quality in fear- and sad-happiness faces (Experiment 1) and by directly comparing anger-, fear-, sadness-, and disgust-happiness expressions (Experiment 2). We found that increasing expression ambiguity and degrading image quality induced a general negativity versus positivity bias in expression categorization. The degree of negativity bias, the associated reaction time and face-viewing gaze allocation were further manipulated by different expression combinations. It seems that although we show a viewing condition-dependent bias in interpreting vague facial expressions that display valence-contradicting expressive cues, it appears that the perception of these ambiguous expressions is guided by a categorical process similar to that involved in perceiving prototypical expressions.

与原型面部表情相比,我们在感知模糊表情时表现出较少的感知容忍度,表现出解释偏见,例如在对以不同比例变形并在高质量或低质量条件下显示的愤怒和快乐面部的模糊表情进行分类时,更频繁地感知愤怒或快乐。然而,目前尚不清楚这种解释偏见是特定于情绪类别,还是反映了普遍的消极与积极偏见,以及这种偏见的程度是否受到两种变体表达的效价或类别的影响。在两个眼动追踪实验中,通过系统地操纵恐惧和悲伤幸福表情中的表情模糊性和图像质量(实验1),以及通过直接比较愤怒、恐惧、悲伤和厌恶幸福表情(实验2),对这些问题进行了检验。我们发现,表达歧义的增加和图像质量的降低导致了表达分类中普遍存在的消极与积极偏见。消极偏见的程度、相关的反应时间和面部注视的分配通过不同的表情组合进一步操纵。似乎,尽管我们在解释模糊的面部表情时表现出了依赖于观看条件的偏见,这些表情显示出与表达线索相矛盾的效价,但对这些模糊表情的感知似乎是由一个类似于感知原型表情的分类过程引导的。
{"title":"Emotion category-modulated interpretation bias in perceiving ambiguous facial expressions.","authors":"Emily Todd,&nbsp;Shaini Subendran,&nbsp;George Wright,&nbsp;Kun Guo","doi":"10.1177/03010066231186936","DOIUrl":"10.1177/03010066231186936","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>In contrast to prototypical facial expressions, we show less perceptual tolerance in perceiving vague expressions by demonstrating an interpretation bias, such as more frequent perception of anger or happiness when categorizing ambiguous expressions of angry and happy faces that are morphed in different proportions and displayed under high- or low-quality conditions. However, it remains unclear whether this interpretation bias is specific to emotion categories or reflects a general negativity versus positivity bias and whether the degree of this bias is affected by the valence or category of two morphed expressions. These questions were examined in two eye-tracking experiments by systematically manipulating expression ambiguity and image quality in fear- and sad-happiness faces (Experiment 1) and by directly comparing anger-, fear-, sadness-, and disgust-happiness expressions (Experiment 2). We found that increasing expression ambiguity and degrading image quality induced a general negativity versus positivity bias in expression categorization. The degree of negativity bias, the associated reaction time and face-viewing gaze allocation were further manipulated by different expression combinations. It seems that although we show a viewing condition-dependent bias in interpreting vague facial expressions that display valence-contradicting expressive cues, it appears that the perception of these ambiguous expressions is guided by a categorical process similar to that involved in perceiving prototypical expressions.</p>","PeriodicalId":49708,"journal":{"name":"Perception","volume":"52 10","pages":"695-711"},"PeriodicalIF":1.7,"publicationDate":"2023-10-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://ftp.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pub/pmc/oa_pdf/a9/f2/10.1177_03010066231186936.PMC10510303.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10292286","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Change detection versus change localization for faces, houses, and words. 人脸、房屋和单词的变化检测与变化定位。
IF 1.7 4区 心理学 Q3 OPHTHALMOLOGY Pub Date : 2023-10-01 Epub Date: 2023-08-08 DOI: 10.1177/03010066231191193
Paulo Ventura, Alexandre Pereira, Francisco Cruz, João Delgado, Bruno Faustino, José Carlos Guerreiro

Holistic processing aids in the discrimination of visually similar objects, but it may also come with a cost. Indeed holistic processing may improve the ability to detect changes to a face while impairing the ability to locate where the changes occur. We investigated the capacity to detect the occurrence of a change versus the capacity to detect the localization of a change for faces, houses, and words. Change detection was better than change localization for faces. Change localization outperformed change detection for houses. For words, there was no difference between detection and localization. We know from previous studies that words are processed holistically. However, being an object of visual expertise processed holistically, visual words are also a linguistic entity. Previously, the word composite effect was found for phonologically consistent words but not for phonologically inconsistent words. Being an object of visual expertise for which linguistic information is important, letter position information, is also crucial. Thus, the importance of localization of letters and features may augment the capacity to localize a change in words making the detection of a change and the detection of localization of a change equivalent.

整体处理有助于区分视觉上相似的物体,但也可能会带来成本。事实上,整体处理可以提高检测面部变化的能力,同时削弱定位变化发生位置的能力。我们调查了人脸、房屋和单词检测变化发生的能力与检测变化定位的能力。人脸的变化检测优于变化定位。房屋的变化定位优于变化检测。对于单词,检测和定位之间没有区别。我们从以前的研究中知道,单词是经过整体处理的。然而,作为视觉专业知识的整体处理对象,视觉单词也是一个语言实体。以前,单词复合效应被发现适用于语音一致的单词,但不适用于语音不一致的单词。作为视觉专业知识的对象,语言信息很重要,字母位置信息也很重要。因此,字母和特征的定位的重要性可以增强对单词中的变化进行定位的能力,使得对变化的检测和对变化的定位的检测等效。
{"title":"Change detection versus change localization for faces, houses, and words.","authors":"Paulo Ventura,&nbsp;Alexandre Pereira,&nbsp;Francisco Cruz,&nbsp;João Delgado,&nbsp;Bruno Faustino,&nbsp;José Carlos Guerreiro","doi":"10.1177/03010066231191193","DOIUrl":"10.1177/03010066231191193","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Holistic processing aids in the discrimination of visually similar objects, but it may also come with a cost. Indeed holistic processing may improve the ability to detect changes to a face while impairing the ability to locate where the changes occur. We investigated the capacity to detect the occurrence of a change versus the capacity to detect the localization of a change for faces, houses, and words. Change detection was better than change localization for faces. Change localization outperformed change detection for houses. For words, there was no difference between detection and localization. We know from previous studies that words are processed holistically. However, being an object of visual expertise processed holistically, visual words are also a linguistic entity. Previously, the word composite effect was found for phonologically consistent words but not for phonologically inconsistent words. Being an object of visual expertise for which linguistic information is important, letter position information, is also crucial. Thus, the importance of localization of letters and features may augment the capacity to localize a change in words making the detection of a change and the detection of localization of a change equivalent.</p>","PeriodicalId":49708,"journal":{"name":"Perception","volume":"52 10","pages":"739-751"},"PeriodicalIF":1.7,"publicationDate":"2023-10-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC10510304/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10294760","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Does a concurrent motor process influence representational momentum? 同时发生的运动过程会影响表征动量吗?
IF 1.7 4区 心理学 Q3 OPHTHALMOLOGY Pub Date : 2023-10-01 Epub Date: 2023-09-08 DOI: 10.1177/03010066231194955
Simon Merz, Jana Weiten, Timothy L Hubbard

Whether the direction of a hand motion that is congruent or incongruent with a concurrent target motion can influence representational momentum for that target was examined. Participants viewed a leftward or rightward moving target while moving their hand rightward, leftward, or not moving their hand. Prior studies of mental rotation found that congruency or incongruency of the direction of mental rotation and the direction of a concurrent physical rotation of a stimulus influenced mental rotation. As mental rotation and representational momentum each involve extrapolation of target motion, it could be predicted that congruency of the direction of hand motion and the direction of target motion might influence representational momentum of the target. Robust representational momentum occurred in all conditions, but there was no effect of congruency of hand motion and target motion, nor of the presence or absence of hand motion, on representational momentum. The results are consistent with a hypothesis that the generation of representational momentum involves sensory processes rather than motor processes.

研究了与同时进行的目标运动一致或不一致的手部运动的方向是否会影响该目标的表征动量。参与者观察向左或向右移动的目标,同时向右、向左或不移动手。先前对心理旋转的研究发现,心理旋转的方向与刺激的同时物理旋转的方向一致或不一致会影响心理旋转。由于心理旋转和表征动量都涉及目标运动的外推,可以预测手运动方向和目标运动方向的一致性可能会影响目标的表征动量。在所有条件下都会出现稳健的表征动量,但手部运动和目标运动的一致性以及手部运动的存在与否对表征动量没有影响。研究结果与一种假设一致,即表征动量的产生涉及感觉过程,而不是运动过程。
{"title":"Does a concurrent motor process influence representational momentum?","authors":"Simon Merz,&nbsp;Jana Weiten,&nbsp;Timothy L Hubbard","doi":"10.1177/03010066231194955","DOIUrl":"10.1177/03010066231194955","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Whether the direction of a hand motion that is congruent or incongruent with a concurrent target motion can influence representational momentum for that target was examined. Participants viewed a leftward or rightward moving target while moving their hand rightward, leftward, or not moving their hand. Prior studies of mental rotation found that congruency or incongruency of the direction of mental rotation and the direction of a concurrent physical rotation of a stimulus influenced mental rotation. As mental rotation and representational momentum each involve extrapolation of target motion, it could be predicted that congruency of the direction of hand motion and the direction of target motion might influence representational momentum of the target. Robust representational momentum occurred in all conditions, but there was no effect of congruency of hand motion and target motion, nor of the presence or absence of hand motion, on representational momentum. The results are consistent with a hypothesis that the generation of representational momentum involves sensory processes rather than motor processes.</p>","PeriodicalId":49708,"journal":{"name":"Perception","volume":"52 10","pages":"726-738"},"PeriodicalIF":1.7,"publicationDate":"2023-10-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10308592","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
Are the directions of both eyes integrated before or after the perception of direct gaze? Evidence from simulated mild strabismus. 在感知直接凝视之前或之后,双眼的方向是一致的吗?模拟轻度斜视的证据。
IF 1.7 4区 心理学 Q3 OPHTHALMOLOGY Pub Date : 2023-10-01 Epub Date: 2023-09-03 DOI: 10.1177/03010066231194216
Gernot Horstmann, Linda Linke

The cone of gaze is a looker's range of gaze directions that is accepted as direct by an observer. The present research asks how the condition of mild strabismus, that is, when the two eyes point in slightly different directions, influences the cone of gaze. Normally, both eyes are rotated in a coordinated manner such that both eyes are directed to the same fixation point. With strabismus, there are two fixation points, and, therefore, two directions into which the two eyes point. This raises the question of the direction and the shape (i.e., width) of the gaze cone. Two experiments are conducted with simulated mild strabismus. Three conditions are tested, the two strabismic conditions of esotropia, and exotropia and one orthotropic (nonstrabismic) condition. Results show that the direction of the gaze cone is roughly the average of the directions of the two eyes. Furthermore, the width of the gaze cone is not affected by simulated strabismus and is thus the same for the strabismic and the orthotropic conditions. The results imply a model where at first the direction of gaze based on both eyes is perceived, and where the gaze cone is implied on the basis of the combined gaze direction.

视锥是观测者直接接受的观测者的视线方向范围。目前的研究询问轻度斜视的情况,即当两只眼睛指向稍微不同的方向时,如何影响视锥。通常,两只眼睛以协调的方式旋转,使得两只眼睛指向同一固定点。斜视有两个注视点,因此,两只眼睛指向两个方向。这就提出了凝视锥的方向和形状(即宽度)的问题。用模拟轻度斜视进行了两个实验。测试了三种条件,内斜视和外斜视的两种斜视条件和一种正交异性(非斜视)条件。结果表明,凝视锥的方向大致是两只眼睛方向的平均值。此外,凝视锥的宽度不受模拟斜视的影响,因此在斜视和正交各向异性条件下是相同的。该结果暗示了一个模型,其中首先感知基于双眼的凝视方向,并且其中基于组合的凝视方向暗示凝视锥。
{"title":"Are the directions of both eyes integrated before or after the perception of direct gaze? Evidence from simulated mild strabismus.","authors":"Gernot Horstmann,&nbsp;Linda Linke","doi":"10.1177/03010066231194216","DOIUrl":"10.1177/03010066231194216","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The cone of gaze is a looker's range of gaze directions that is accepted as direct by an observer. The present research asks how the condition of mild strabismus, that is, when the two eyes point in slightly different directions, influences the cone of gaze. Normally, both eyes are rotated in a coordinated manner such that both eyes are directed to the same fixation point. With strabismus, there are two fixation points, and, therefore, two directions into which the two eyes point. This raises the question of the direction and the shape (i.e., width) of the gaze cone. Two experiments are conducted with simulated mild strabismus. Three conditions are tested, the two strabismic conditions of esotropia, and exotropia and one orthotropic (nonstrabismic) condition. Results show that the direction of the gaze cone is roughly the average of the directions of the two eyes. Furthermore, the width of the gaze cone is not affected by simulated strabismus and is thus the same for the strabismic and the orthotropic conditions. The results imply a model where at first the direction of gaze based on both eyes is perceived, and where the gaze cone is implied on the basis of the combined gaze direction.</p>","PeriodicalId":49708,"journal":{"name":"Perception","volume":"52 10","pages":"712-725"},"PeriodicalIF":1.7,"publicationDate":"2023-10-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10304586","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Book Review: Sensing in Social Interaction, the Taste for Cheese in Gourmet Shop by L. Mondada 书评:《社会互动中的感知,美食家店里的奶酪味道》,作者:L. Mondada
4区 心理学 Q3 OPHTHALMOLOGY Pub Date : 2023-09-28 DOI: 10.1177/03010066231204815
Isabelle Viaud-Delmon
{"title":"Book Review: <i>Sensing in Social Interaction, the Taste for Cheese in Gourmet Shop</i> by L. Mondada","authors":"Isabelle Viaud-Delmon","doi":"10.1177/03010066231204815","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1177/03010066231204815","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":49708,"journal":{"name":"Perception","volume":"20 1","pages":"0"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2023-09-28","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"135385669","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Book review: The Routledge International Handbook of Neuroaesthetics by Martin Skov and Marcos Nadal 书评:《劳特利奇国际神经美学手册》,作者:马丁·斯科夫和马科斯·纳达尔
4区 心理学 Q3 OPHTHALMOLOGY Pub Date : 2023-09-10 DOI: 10.1177/03010066231196840
Eugen Wassiliwizky
{"title":"Book review: <i>The Routledge International Handbook of Neuroaesthetics</i> by Martin Skov and Marcos Nadal","authors":"Eugen Wassiliwizky","doi":"10.1177/03010066231196840","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1177/03010066231196840","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":49708,"journal":{"name":"Perception","volume":"78 1","pages":"0"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2023-09-10","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"136073472","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Responses to Raven matrices: Governed by visual complexity and centrality. 对Raven矩阵的反应:由视觉复杂性和中心性控制。
IF 1.7 4区 心理学 Q3 OPHTHALMOLOGY Pub Date : 2023-09-01 DOI: 10.1177/03010066231178149
Joost C F de Winter, Dimitra Dodou, Yke Bauke Eisma

Raven matrices are widely considered a pure test of cognitive abilities. Previous research has examined the extent to which cognitive strategies are predictive of the number of correct responses to Raven items. This study examined whether response times can be explained directly from the centrality and visual complexity of the matrix cells (edge density and perceived complexity). A total of 159 participants completed a 12-item version of the Raven Advanced Progressive Matrices. In addition to item number (an index of item difficulty), the findings demonstrated a positive correlation between the visual complexity of Raven items and both the mean response time and the number of fixations on the matrix (a strong correlate of response time). Moreover, more centrally placed cells as well as more complex cells received more fixations. It is concluded that response times on Raven matrices are impacted by low-level stimulus attributes, namely, visual complexity and eccentricity.

Raven矩阵被广泛认为是一种纯粹的认知能力测试。先前的研究已经检验了认知策略在多大程度上预测了对Raven项目的正确反应的数量。本研究考察了响应时间是否可以直接从矩阵细胞的中心性和视觉复杂性(边缘密度和感知复杂性)来解释。共有159名参与者完成了一份由12个项目组成的瑞文高级递进矩阵。除了项目数量(项目难度指数)外,研究结果还表明,Raven项目的视觉复杂性与平均反应时间和注视矩阵的次数(反应时间的强相关性)呈正相关。此外,更集中的细胞和更复杂的细胞受到更多的注视。结果表明,在Raven矩阵上的响应时间受到低层次刺激属性的影响,即视觉复杂性和偏心率。
{"title":"Responses to Raven matrices: Governed by visual complexity and centrality.","authors":"Joost C F de Winter,&nbsp;Dimitra Dodou,&nbsp;Yke Bauke Eisma","doi":"10.1177/03010066231178149","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1177/03010066231178149","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Raven matrices are widely considered a pure test of cognitive abilities. Previous research has examined the extent to which cognitive strategies are predictive of the number of correct responses to Raven items. This study examined whether response times can be explained directly from the centrality and visual complexity of the matrix cells (edge density and perceived complexity). A total of 159 participants completed a 12-item version of the Raven Advanced Progressive Matrices. In addition to item number (an index of item difficulty), the findings demonstrated a positive correlation between the visual complexity of Raven items and both the mean response time and the number of fixations on the matrix (a strong correlate of response time). Moreover, more centrally placed cells as well as more complex cells received more fixations. It is concluded that response times on Raven matrices are impacted by low-level stimulus attributes, namely, visual complexity and eccentricity.</p>","PeriodicalId":49708,"journal":{"name":"Perception","volume":"52 9","pages":"645-661"},"PeriodicalIF":1.7,"publicationDate":"2023-09-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC10469510/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10191095","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Perception
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1