首页 > 最新文献

Journal of Vision最新文献

英文 中文
Concurrent perception of competing predictions: A "split-stimulus effect". 同时感知相互竞争的预测:分裂刺激效应
IF 2 4区 心理学 Q2 OPHTHALMOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-10-03 DOI: 10.1167/jov.24.11.5
Joseph Melling, William Turner, Hinze Hogendoorn

Visual illusions are systematic misperceptions that can help us glean the heuristics with which the brain constructs visual experience. In a recently discovered visual illusion (the "frame effect"), it has been shown that flashing a stimulus inside of a moving frame produces a large misperception of that stimulus's position. Across two experiments, we investigated a novel illusion (the "split stimulus effect") where the symmetrical motion of two overlaid frames produces two simultaneous positional misperceptions of a single stimulus. That is, one stimulus is presented but two are perceived. In both experiments, a single red dot was flashed when the moving frames reversed direction, and participants were asked to report how many dots they saw. Naïve participants sometimes reported seeing two dots when only one was presented, indicating spontaneous perception of the illusion. A Bayesian analysis of the population prevalence of this effect was conducted. The dependence of this effect on the frames' speed, the dot's opacity, spatial attention, as the presence/absence of pre-flash motion ("postdiction") was also investigated, and the features of this illusion were compared to similar motion position illusions within a predictive processing framework. In demonstrating this illusory "splitting" effect, this study is the first to show that it is possible to be simultaneously aware of two opposing perceptual predictions about a single object and provides evidence of the hyperpriors that limit and inform the structure of visual experience.

视错觉是一种系统性错觉,可以帮助我们了解大脑构建视觉经验的启发式方法。最近发现的一种视觉错觉("框架效应")表明,在一个移动的框架内闪烁一个刺激物,会对该刺激物的位置产生很大的错觉。在两个实验中,我们研究了一种新的错觉("分割刺激效应"),即两个重叠框架的对称运动会同时对一个刺激产生两种位置错觉。也就是说,呈现的是一个刺激,但感知到的却是两个刺激。在这两项实验中,当运动框架反向运动时,会闪现一个红点,参与者被要求报告他们看到了几个红点。天真的参与者有时会报告看到了两个点,而实际上只出现了一个点,这表明他们自发地感知到了幻觉。我们对这种效应在人群中的普遍性进行了贝叶斯分析。此外,还研究了这种效应对帧的速度、点的不透明度、空间注意力以及闪烁前运动("后预测")的存在/不存在的依赖性,并在预测处理框架内将这种错觉的特征与类似的运动位置错觉进行了比较。通过展示这种虚幻的 "分裂 "效应,本研究首次证明了同时意识到关于单一物体的两种相反的知觉预测是可能的,并提供了限制视觉经验结构并为其提供信息的超先验因素的证据。
{"title":"Concurrent perception of competing predictions: A \"split-stimulus effect\".","authors":"Joseph Melling, William Turner, Hinze Hogendoorn","doi":"10.1167/jov.24.11.5","DOIUrl":"10.1167/jov.24.11.5","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Visual illusions are systematic misperceptions that can help us glean the heuristics with which the brain constructs visual experience. In a recently discovered visual illusion (the \"frame effect\"), it has been shown that flashing a stimulus inside of a moving frame produces a large misperception of that stimulus's position. Across two experiments, we investigated a novel illusion (the \"split stimulus effect\") where the symmetrical motion of two overlaid frames produces two simultaneous positional misperceptions of a single stimulus. That is, one stimulus is presented but two are perceived. In both experiments, a single red dot was flashed when the moving frames reversed direction, and participants were asked to report how many dots they saw. Naïve participants sometimes reported seeing two dots when only one was presented, indicating spontaneous perception of the illusion. A Bayesian analysis of the population prevalence of this effect was conducted. The dependence of this effect on the frames' speed, the dot's opacity, spatial attention, as the presence/absence of pre-flash motion (\"postdiction\") was also investigated, and the features of this illusion were compared to similar motion position illusions within a predictive processing framework. In demonstrating this illusory \"splitting\" effect, this study is the first to show that it is possible to be simultaneously aware of two opposing perceptual predictions about a single object and provides evidence of the hyperpriors that limit and inform the structure of visual experience.</p>","PeriodicalId":49955,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Vision","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.0,"publicationDate":"2024-10-03","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11463704/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142394803","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Two faces of holistic face processing: Facilitation and interference underlying part-whole and composite effects. 整体人脸处理的两面性:部分-整体效应和复合效应背后的促进和干扰。
IF 2 4区 心理学 Q2 OPHTHALMOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-10-03 DOI: 10.1167/jov.24.11.13
Haiyang Jin, William G Hayward, Olivia S Cheung

Holistic processing, a strong tendency to process multiple features together, is regarded as a hallmark of face perception. Holistic effects can be revealed by several tasks, including the part-whole task, standard composite task, and complete composite task. Although holistic effects are readily observed using these tasks, the lack of correlations among these effects and the mixed findings across these tasks when examining the effects among various populations or manipulations pose questions about how these effects should be understood. We distinguished facilitation and interference effects within the holistic effects in the complete composite task and found that the holistic effect in the part-whole task appeared to be correlated with facilitation but not interference in the complete composite task, whereas the holistic effect in the standard composite task was correlated with interference but not facilitation in the complete composite task. These findings suggest that clarifying the roles of facilitation and interference is critical for understanding holistic face processing.

整体处理,即把多个特征放在一起处理的强烈倾向,被认为是人脸感知的一个标志。整体效应可以通过几项任务来揭示,包括部分-整体任务、标准复合任务和完整复合任务。虽然通过这些任务很容易观察到整体效应,但这些效应之间缺乏相关性,而且在研究不同人群或不同操作之间的效应时,这些任务的研究结果也不尽相同,这就提出了如何理解这些效应的问题。我们区分了完整综合任务中整体效应的促进效应和干扰效应,发现部分-整体任务中的整体效应似乎与完整综合任务中的促进效应相关,但与干扰效应无关;而标准综合任务中的整体效应与干扰效应相关,但与完整综合任务中的促进效应无关。这些发现表明,明确促进和干扰的作用对于理解整体人脸加工至关重要。
{"title":"Two faces of holistic face processing: Facilitation and interference underlying part-whole and composite effects.","authors":"Haiyang Jin, William G Hayward, Olivia S Cheung","doi":"10.1167/jov.24.11.13","DOIUrl":"10.1167/jov.24.11.13","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Holistic processing, a strong tendency to process multiple features together, is regarded as a hallmark of face perception. Holistic effects can be revealed by several tasks, including the part-whole task, standard composite task, and complete composite task. Although holistic effects are readily observed using these tasks, the lack of correlations among these effects and the mixed findings across these tasks when examining the effects among various populations or manipulations pose questions about how these effects should be understood. We distinguished facilitation and interference effects within the holistic effects in the complete composite task and found that the holistic effect in the part-whole task appeared to be correlated with facilitation but not interference in the complete composite task, whereas the holistic effect in the standard composite task was correlated with interference but not facilitation in the complete composite task. These findings suggest that clarifying the roles of facilitation and interference is critical for understanding holistic face processing.</p>","PeriodicalId":49955,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Vision","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.0,"publicationDate":"2024-10-03","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11512570/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142479247","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Color category and inter-item interaction influence color working memory codependently. 颜色类别和项目间的交互作用对颜色工作记忆的影响是相互依存的。
IF 2 4区 心理学 Q2 OPHTHALMOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-09-03 DOI: 10.1167/jov.24.9.5
Mengdan Sun, Xinyue Yang, Chundi Wang

Our brains do not always encode visual information in a veridical way. Visual working memory (WM) for features such as color can be biased. WM bias comes from several sources. Category priors can lead to WM bias. For example, color WM is biased toward or away from category prototypes. In addition to category knowledge, contextual factors can induce and modulate WM bias; however, these biases of different sources have usually been investigated independently with different tasks. The present study sought to explore how color WM is influenced by both color category and concurrent distractor. Specifically, we asked participants to retain two color items in WM to investigate how the WM representation of the target color is biased by learned category knowledge and contextual inter-item interactions. Our study found that the WM representation of the target color is biased toward or away from the category prototypes and away from the distractor color that is simultaneously held in WM, indicating that both color category and concurrent distractor bias color WM. More importantly, the weight of these two biases depends on the specific color category, suggesting that category priors and inter-item interaction biases are not simply additive but flexible. Furthermore, we revealed that both types of biases arise from perceptual processes.

我们的大脑并不总是以真实的方式对视觉信息进行编码。针对颜色等特征的视觉工作记忆(WM)可能存在偏差。WM偏差有多种来源。类别先验会导致 WM 偏差。例如,颜色工作记忆会偏向或偏离类别原型。除了类别知识外,情境因素也会诱发和调节 WM 偏差;然而,这些不同来源的偏差通常都是通过不同的任务进行独立研究的。本研究试图探讨颜色 WM 如何同时受到颜色类别和同时出现的分心物的影响。具体来说,我们要求被试在 WM 中保留两个颜色项目,以研究目标颜色的 WM 表征如何受到所学类别知识和项目间上下文相互作用的影响。我们的研究发现,目标颜色的 WM 表征偏向或偏离类别原型,偏离同时保留在 WM 中的分心颜色,这表明颜色类别和同时出现的分心颜色都会偏向颜色 WM。更重要的是,这两种偏向的权重取决于具体的颜色类别,这表明类别先验和项目间交互偏向不是简单的相加,而是灵活的。此外,我们还发现这两种偏差都来自知觉过程。
{"title":"Color category and inter-item interaction influence color working memory codependently.","authors":"Mengdan Sun, Xinyue Yang, Chundi Wang","doi":"10.1167/jov.24.9.5","DOIUrl":"10.1167/jov.24.9.5","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Our brains do not always encode visual information in a veridical way. Visual working memory (WM) for features such as color can be biased. WM bias comes from several sources. Category priors can lead to WM bias. For example, color WM is biased toward or away from category prototypes. In addition to category knowledge, contextual factors can induce and modulate WM bias; however, these biases of different sources have usually been investigated independently with different tasks. The present study sought to explore how color WM is influenced by both color category and concurrent distractor. Specifically, we asked participants to retain two color items in WM to investigate how the WM representation of the target color is biased by learned category knowledge and contextual inter-item interactions. Our study found that the WM representation of the target color is biased toward or away from the category prototypes and away from the distractor color that is simultaneously held in WM, indicating that both color category and concurrent distractor bias color WM. More importantly, the weight of these two biases depends on the specific color category, suggesting that category priors and inter-item interaction biases are not simply additive but flexible. Furthermore, we revealed that both types of biases arise from perceptual processes.</p>","PeriodicalId":49955,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Vision","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.0,"publicationDate":"2024-09-03","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11383193/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142141628","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Foveal neural adaptation to optically induced contrast reduction. 眼窝神经对光学诱导的对比度降低的适应。
IF 1.8 4区 心理学 Q2 OPHTHALMOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-09-03 DOI: 10.1167/jov.24.9.13
Antonia Roth,Katharina Breher,Niklas Domdei,Siegfried Wahl
Contrast processing is suggested to interact with eye growth and myopia development. A novel contrast-reducing myopia control lens design decreases image contrast and was shown to slow myopia progression. Limited insights exist regarding neural visual processing following adaptation to image contrast reduction. This study investigated foveal neural contrast sensitivity in 29 young adults following a 30-minute adaptation to scattering using a Bangerter occlusion foil 0.8, +0.5-diopter defocus, and a clear lens control condition. Neural contrast sensitivity at its peak sensitivity of 6 cycles per degree was assessed before and after adaptation to the lens conditions, employing a unique interferometric system. Pre-adaptation measurements were averaged from six replicates and post-adaptation measurements by the first and last three of six replicates. The change in neural contrast sensitivity was largest for scattering across the first and last three post-adaptation measurements (+0.05 ± 0.01 logCS and +0.04 ± 0.01 logCS, respectively) compared with control and defocus (all +0.03 ± 0.01 logCS). For scattering, the observed increase of neural contrast sensitivity within the first three measurements differed significantly from the pre-adaptation baseline (p = 0.04) and was significantly higher compared with the control condition (p = 0.04). The sensitivity increases in the control and defocus conditions were not significant (all p > 0.05). As the adaptation effect diminished, no significant differences were found from baseline or between the conditions in the last three measurements (all p > 0.05). When post-adaptation neural contrast sensitivities were clustered into 25-second sequences, a significant effect was observed between the conditions, with only a significant relevant effect between control and scattering at 25 seconds (p = 0.04) and no further significant effects (all p > 0.05). The alteration in neural contrast sensitivity at peak sensitivity was most pronounced following adaptation to the scattering condition compared with defocus and control, suggesting that induced scattering might be considered for myopia control.
对比度处理被认为与眼睛的生长和近视的发展相互影响。一种新颖的降低对比度的近视控制镜片设计可降低图像对比度,并能减缓近视的发展。关于适应图像对比度降低后的神经视觉处理,目前的研究还很有限。本研究调查了 29 名年轻成年人在使用 Bangerter 遮盖箔 0.8、+0.5-diopter 散焦和透明镜片控制条件下进行 30 分钟散焦适应后的眼窝神经对比敏感度。采用独特的干涉测量系统,在适应镜片条件之前和之后,评估了神经对比敏感度在每度 6 个周期的峰值敏感度。适应前的测量结果取六个重复样本的平均值,适应后的测量结果取六个重复样本中前三个和后三个的平均值。与对照组和散焦组(均为 +0.03 ± 0.01 logCS)相比,散射组神经对比敏感度在适应后的前三次和后三次测量中变化最大(分别为 +0.05 ± 0.01 logCS 和 +0.04 ± 0.01 logCS)。对于散焦,在前三次测量中观察到的神经对比灵敏度的增加与适应前的基线有显著差异(p = 0.04),并且与对照组相比显著更高(p = 0.04)。在对照和散焦条件下,灵敏度的增加并不显著(所有 p > 0.05)。随着适应效应的减弱,最后三次测量结果与基线或不同条件下的测量结果均无显著差异(均 p > 0.05)。当将适应后的神经对比敏感度集中到 25 秒序列时,观察到不同条件之间存在显著效应,只有在 25 秒时对照组和散射组之间存在显著相关效应(p = 0.04),并且没有进一步的显著效应(所有 p > 0.05)。与散焦和对照组相比,适应散射条件后神经对比敏感度峰值的改变最为明显,这表明可考虑将诱导散射用于近视控制。
{"title":"Foveal neural adaptation to optically induced contrast reduction.","authors":"Antonia Roth,Katharina Breher,Niklas Domdei,Siegfried Wahl","doi":"10.1167/jov.24.9.13","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1167/jov.24.9.13","url":null,"abstract":"Contrast processing is suggested to interact with eye growth and myopia development. A novel contrast-reducing myopia control lens design decreases image contrast and was shown to slow myopia progression. Limited insights exist regarding neural visual processing following adaptation to image contrast reduction. This study investigated foveal neural contrast sensitivity in 29 young adults following a 30-minute adaptation to scattering using a Bangerter occlusion foil 0.8, +0.5-diopter defocus, and a clear lens control condition. Neural contrast sensitivity at its peak sensitivity of 6 cycles per degree was assessed before and after adaptation to the lens conditions, employing a unique interferometric system. Pre-adaptation measurements were averaged from six replicates and post-adaptation measurements by the first and last three of six replicates. The change in neural contrast sensitivity was largest for scattering across the first and last three post-adaptation measurements (+0.05 ± 0.01 logCS and +0.04 ± 0.01 logCS, respectively) compared with control and defocus (all +0.03 ± 0.01 logCS). For scattering, the observed increase of neural contrast sensitivity within the first three measurements differed significantly from the pre-adaptation baseline (p = 0.04) and was significantly higher compared with the control condition (p = 0.04). The sensitivity increases in the control and defocus conditions were not significant (all p > 0.05). As the adaptation effect diminished, no significant differences were found from baseline or between the conditions in the last three measurements (all p > 0.05). When post-adaptation neural contrast sensitivities were clustered into 25-second sequences, a significant effect was observed between the conditions, with only a significant relevant effect between control and scattering at 25 seconds (p = 0.04) and no further significant effects (all p > 0.05). The alteration in neural contrast sensitivity at peak sensitivity was most pronounced following adaptation to the scattering condition compared with defocus and control, suggesting that induced scattering might be considered for myopia control.","PeriodicalId":49955,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Vision","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.8,"publicationDate":"2024-09-03","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142266496","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Visual event boundaries trigger forgetting despite active maintenance in visual working memory. 尽管视觉工作记忆中存在着积极的维护,但视觉事件边界仍会引发遗忘。
IF 1.8 4区 心理学 Q2 OPHTHALMOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-09-03 DOI: 10.1167/jov.24.9.9
Joan Danielle K Ongchoco,Yaoda Xu
The contents of visual perception are inherently dynamic-just as we experience objects in space, so too events in time. The boundaries between these events have downstream consequences. For example, memory for incidentally encountered items is impaired when walking through a doorway, perhaps because event boundaries serve as cues to clear obsolete information from previous events. Although this kind of "memory flushing" can be adaptive, work on visual working memory (VWM) has focused on the opposite function of active maintenance in the face of distraction. How do these two cognitive operations interact? In this study, observers watched animations in which they walked through three-dimensionally rendered rooms with picture frames on the walls. Within the frames, observers either saw images that they had to remember ("encoding") or recalled images they had seen in the immediately preceding frame ("test"). Half of the time, a doorway was crossed during the delay between encoding and test. Across experiments, there was a consistent memory decrement for the first image encoded in the doorway compared to the no-doorway condition while equating time elapsed, distance traveled, and distractibility of the doorway. This decrement despite top-down VWM efforts highlights the power of event boundaries to structure what and when we forget.
视觉感知的内容本质上是动态的--就像我们体验空间中的物体一样,时间中的事件也是动态的。这些事件之间的界限会产生下游影响。例如,当穿过一个门洞时,对偶然遇到的物品的记忆就会受损,这可能是因为事件的边界是清除先前事件中过时信息的线索。尽管这种 "记忆冲洗 "可能是适应性的,但有关视觉工作记忆(VWM)的研究却集中在面对分心时主动维护的相反功能上。这两种认知操作是如何相互作用的呢?在这项研究中,观察者观看动画,在动画中,他们穿过三维渲染的房间,房间的墙壁上有画框。在画框中,观察者要么看到他们必须记住的图像("编码"),要么回忆起他们在前一画框中看到的图像("测试")。在编码和测试之间的延迟时间内,有一半的时间是穿过门廊的。在所有实验中,与无门道条件相比,在门道中编码的第一幅图像的记忆力会持续下降,而所花时间、所走距离和门道的分心程度都是相同的。尽管自上而下的 VWM 发挥了作用,但这种记忆力下降的现象还是突显了事件边界对我们遗忘的内容和遗忘时间的影响。
{"title":"Visual event boundaries trigger forgetting despite active maintenance in visual working memory.","authors":"Joan Danielle K Ongchoco,Yaoda Xu","doi":"10.1167/jov.24.9.9","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1167/jov.24.9.9","url":null,"abstract":"The contents of visual perception are inherently dynamic-just as we experience objects in space, so too events in time. The boundaries between these events have downstream consequences. For example, memory for incidentally encountered items is impaired when walking through a doorway, perhaps because event boundaries serve as cues to clear obsolete information from previous events. Although this kind of \"memory flushing\" can be adaptive, work on visual working memory (VWM) has focused on the opposite function of active maintenance in the face of distraction. How do these two cognitive operations interact? In this study, observers watched animations in which they walked through three-dimensionally rendered rooms with picture frames on the walls. Within the frames, observers either saw images that they had to remember (\"encoding\") or recalled images they had seen in the immediately preceding frame (\"test\"). Half of the time, a doorway was crossed during the delay between encoding and test. Across experiments, there was a consistent memory decrement for the first image encoded in the doorway compared to the no-doorway condition while equating time elapsed, distance traveled, and distractibility of the doorway. This decrement despite top-down VWM efforts highlights the power of event boundaries to structure what and when we forget.","PeriodicalId":49955,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Vision","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.8,"publicationDate":"2024-09-03","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142182510","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Facial ambiguity and perception: How face-likeness affects breaking time in continuous flash suppression. 面部模糊性与感知:脸部相似性如何影响连续闪光抑制的中断时间。
IF 2 4区 心理学 Q2 OPHTHALMOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-09-03 DOI: 10.1167/jov.24.9.18
Michael Makoto Martinsen, Kairi Yoshino, Yuya Kinzuka, Fumiaki Sato, Hideki Tamura, Tetsuto Minami, Shigeki Nakauchi

Previous studies have elucidated that humans can implicitly process faces faster than they process objects. However, the mechanism through which the brain unconsciously processes ambiguous facial images remains unclear. In our experiment, upright and inverted black-and-white binary face stimuli were presented in a two-alternative forced-choice location discrimination task combined with continuous flash suppression, a technique that suppresses visual stimuli perception using rapidly changing masks. The breaking time (BT) or the time required for a stimulus to be perceptually recognized was recorded for each face stimulus. The results showed that the BT for inverted grayscale images was significantly longer than that for upright grayscale faces, whereas the BT for upright and inverted binary faces did not reach statistical significance. A significant correlation between face likeness and BT was established after evaluating face likeness for each binary face stimulus, with high-face-like binary faces exhibiting shorter BT and low-face-like stimuli resulting in a more prolonged BT. Our results suggest that even an ambiguous object rated highly in face likeness can reduce the BT under implicit processing, indicating the possibility that facial parts such as the eyes and nose are subconsciously detected in ambiguous facial stimuli, enabling facial perception.

以往的研究已经阐明,人类对人脸的内隐处理速度快于对物体的处理速度。然而,大脑在无意识中处理模棱两可的面部图像的机制仍不清楚。在我们的实验中,直立和倒立的黑白二元人脸刺激被呈现在一个两选一的强迫选择位置辨别任务中,并结合了连续闪光抑制,这是一种利用快速变化的遮罩抑制视觉刺激感知的技术。研究人员记录了每个人脸刺激的中断时间(BT)或刺激被感知识别所需的时间。结果显示,倒置灰度图像的断裂时间明显长于直立灰度人脸的断裂时间,而直立和倒置二元人脸的断裂时间没有达到统计学意义。在对每个二元人脸刺激的人脸相似度进行评估后,人脸相似度与 BT 之间建立了明显的相关性,高人脸相似度的二元人脸表现出更短的 BT,而低人脸相似度的刺激则导致更长的 BT。我们的研究结果表明,即使是脸部相似度高的模糊物体也能在内隐处理中缩短BT,这表明在模糊的脸部刺激中,眼睛和鼻子等脸部部位可能会被潜意识检测到,从而实现脸部感知。
{"title":"Facial ambiguity and perception: How face-likeness affects breaking time in continuous flash suppression.","authors":"Michael Makoto Martinsen, Kairi Yoshino, Yuya Kinzuka, Fumiaki Sato, Hideki Tamura, Tetsuto Minami, Shigeki Nakauchi","doi":"10.1167/jov.24.9.18","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1167/jov.24.9.18","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Previous studies have elucidated that humans can implicitly process faces faster than they process objects. However, the mechanism through which the brain unconsciously processes ambiguous facial images remains unclear. In our experiment, upright and inverted black-and-white binary face stimuli were presented in a two-alternative forced-choice location discrimination task combined with continuous flash suppression, a technique that suppresses visual stimuli perception using rapidly changing masks. The breaking time (BT) or the time required for a stimulus to be perceptually recognized was recorded for each face stimulus. The results showed that the BT for inverted grayscale images was significantly longer than that for upright grayscale faces, whereas the BT for upright and inverted binary faces did not reach statistical significance. A significant correlation between face likeness and BT was established after evaluating face likeness for each binary face stimulus, with high-face-like binary faces exhibiting shorter BT and low-face-like stimuli resulting in a more prolonged BT. Our results suggest that even an ambiguous object rated highly in face likeness can reduce the BT under implicit processing, indicating the possibility that facial parts such as the eyes and nose are subconsciously detected in ambiguous facial stimuli, enabling facial perception.</p>","PeriodicalId":49955,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Vision","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.0,"publicationDate":"2024-09-03","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11437706/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142331294","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Effects of spatial location on distractor interference. 空间位置对分心干扰的影响
IF 2 4区 心理学 Q2 OPHTHALMOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-09-03 DOI: 10.1167/jov.24.9.4
Dirk Kerzel, Martin Constant

When target and distractor stimuli are close together, they activate the same neurons and there is ambiguity as to what the neural activity represents. It has been suggested that the ambiguity is resolved by spatial competition between target and nontarget stimuli. A competitive advantage is conveyed by bottom-up biases (e.g., stimulus saliency) and top-down biases (e.g., the match to a stored representation of the target stimulus). Here, we tested the hypothesis that regions with high perceptual performance may provide a bottom-up bias, resulting in increased distractor interference. Initially, we focused on two known anisotropies. At equal distance from central fixation, perceptual performance is better along the horizontal than the vertical meridian, and in the lower than in the upper visual hemifield. Consistently, interference from distractors on the horizontal meridian was greater than interference from distractors on the vertical meridian. However, distractors in the lower hemifield interfered less than distractors in the upper visual hemifield, which is contrary to the known anisotropy. These results were obtained with targets and distractors on opposite meridians. Further, we observed greater interference from distractors on the meridians compared with distractors on the diagonals, possibly reflecting anisotropies in attentional scanning. Overall, the results are only partially consistent with the hypothesis that distractor interference is larger for distractors on regions with high perceptual performance.

当目标刺激和干扰刺激靠近时,它们会激活相同的神经元,而神经活动所代表的内容则模糊不清。有人认为,这种模糊性是通过目标刺激和非目标刺激之间的空间竞争来解决的。竞争优势通过自下而上的偏差(如刺激的显著性)和自上而下的偏差(如与目标刺激的存储表征的匹配)来传递。在这里,我们测试了一个假设,即知觉性能高的区域可能会提供自下而上的偏向,从而导致分心干扰增加。起初,我们关注两个已知的各向异性。在与中心定点距离相等的情况下,沿水平经线的知觉表现优于垂直经线,下视半区的知觉表现优于上视半区。一致的是,水平经线上的干扰比垂直经线上的干扰大。然而,下半视场的分心物比上半视场的分心物干扰小,这与已知的各向异性相反。这些结果是在目标和干扰物位于相反经线上时得出的。此外,我们还观察到经线上的干扰物比对角线上的干扰物干扰更大,这可能反映了注意扫描的各向异性。总体而言,研究结果与 "感知能力强的区域上的分心物干扰更大 "这一假设只有部分吻合。
{"title":"Effects of spatial location on distractor interference.","authors":"Dirk Kerzel, Martin Constant","doi":"10.1167/jov.24.9.4","DOIUrl":"10.1167/jov.24.9.4","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>When target and distractor stimuli are close together, they activate the same neurons and there is ambiguity as to what the neural activity represents. It has been suggested that the ambiguity is resolved by spatial competition between target and nontarget stimuli. A competitive advantage is conveyed by bottom-up biases (e.g., stimulus saliency) and top-down biases (e.g., the match to a stored representation of the target stimulus). Here, we tested the hypothesis that regions with high perceptual performance may provide a bottom-up bias, resulting in increased distractor interference. Initially, we focused on two known anisotropies. At equal distance from central fixation, perceptual performance is better along the horizontal than the vertical meridian, and in the lower than in the upper visual hemifield. Consistently, interference from distractors on the horizontal meridian was greater than interference from distractors on the vertical meridian. However, distractors in the lower hemifield interfered less than distractors in the upper visual hemifield, which is contrary to the known anisotropy. These results were obtained with targets and distractors on opposite meridians. Further, we observed greater interference from distractors on the meridians compared with distractors on the diagonals, possibly reflecting anisotropies in attentional scanning. Overall, the results are only partially consistent with the hypothesis that distractor interference is larger for distractors on regions with high perceptual performance.</p>","PeriodicalId":49955,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Vision","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.0,"publicationDate":"2024-09-03","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11382967/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142141629","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Characterizing serial dependence as an attraction to prior response. 将序列依赖性描述为对先前反应的吸引力。
IF 2 4区 心理学 Q2 OPHTHALMOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-09-03 DOI: 10.1167/jov.24.9.16
Geoffrey K Gallagher, Christopher P Benton

Serial dependence refers to a common misperception that can occur between subsequently observed stimuli. Observers misreport the current stimulus as being more similar to the previous stimulus than it objectively is. It has been proposed that this bias may reflect an attraction of the current percept to prior percept (Fischer & Whitney, 2014). Alternatively, serial dependence has also been proposed to be the result of an assimilative effect between observer decisions (Fritsche, Mostert, & de Lange, 2017; Pascucci, Mancuso, Santandrea, Libera, Plomp, & Chelazzi, 2019). Lying within this debate is the issue of how we quantify serial dependence. Should this be as a bias induced by prior stimuli or by prior responses? We investigated this by manipulating the orientation of the current stimuli such that they fell between previous stimulus and previous response. We observed an attraction to previous response and a concomitant repulsion from previous stimulus. This suggests that the attractive effect of serial dependence in orientation judgments is best quantified in relation to prior response.

序列依赖指的是在随后观察到的刺激之间可能出现的一种常见错觉。观察者会误认为当前刺激与之前刺激的相似度高于客观相似度。有人认为,这种偏差可能反映了当前感知对先前感知的吸引力(Fischer 和 Whitney,2014 年)。另外,序列依赖也被认为是观察者决策之间同化效应的结果(Fritsche, Mostert, & de Lange, 2017; Pascucci, Mancuso, Santandrea, Libera, Plomp, & Chelazzi, 2019)。这场争论的焦点在于如何量化序列依赖性。这应该是由先前的刺激还是先前的反应引起的偏差?我们通过操纵当前刺激的方向,使其位于先前刺激和先前反应之间,对这一问题进行了研究。我们观察到了对先前反应的吸引和对先前刺激的排斥。这表明,在方位判断中,序列依赖的吸引效应最好与先前的反应联系起来进行量化。
{"title":"Characterizing serial dependence as an attraction to prior response.","authors":"Geoffrey K Gallagher, Christopher P Benton","doi":"10.1167/jov.24.9.16","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1167/jov.24.9.16","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Serial dependence refers to a common misperception that can occur between subsequently observed stimuli. Observers misreport the current stimulus as being more similar to the previous stimulus than it objectively is. It has been proposed that this bias may reflect an attraction of the current percept to prior percept (Fischer & Whitney, 2014). Alternatively, serial dependence has also been proposed to be the result of an assimilative effect between observer decisions (Fritsche, Mostert, & de Lange, 2017; Pascucci, Mancuso, Santandrea, Libera, Plomp, & Chelazzi, 2019). Lying within this debate is the issue of how we quantify serial dependence. Should this be as a bias induced by prior stimuli or by prior responses? We investigated this by manipulating the orientation of the current stimuli such that they fell between previous stimulus and previous response. We observed an attraction to previous response and a concomitant repulsion from previous stimulus. This suggests that the attractive effect of serial dependence in orientation judgments is best quantified in relation to prior response.</p>","PeriodicalId":49955,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Vision","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.0,"publicationDate":"2024-09-03","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11437684/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142331293","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
A Bayesian inference model can predict the effects of attention on the serial dependence in heading estimation from optic flow. 贝叶斯推理模型可以预测注意力对根据视流估计航向的序列依赖性的影响。
IF 1.8 4区 心理学 Q2 OPHTHALMOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-09-03 DOI: 10.1167/jov.24.9.11
Qi Sun,Si-Yu Wang,Lin-Zhe Zhan,Fan-Huan You,Qian Sun
It has been demonstrated that observers can accurately estimate their self-motion direction (i.e., heading) from optic flow, which can be affected by attention. However, it remains unclear how attention affects the serial dependence in the estimation. In the current study, participants conducted two experiments. The results showed that the estimation accuracy decreased when attentional resources allocated to the heading estimation task were reduced. Additionally, the estimates of currently presented headings were biased toward the headings of previously seen headings, showing serial dependence. Especially, this effect decreased (increased) when the attentional resources allocated to the previously (currently) seen headings were reduced. Furthermore, importantly, we developed a Bayesian inference model, which incorporated attention-modulated likelihoods and qualitatively predicted changes in the estimation accuracy and serial dependence. In summary, the current study shows that attention affects the serial dependence in heading estimation from optic flow and reveals the Bayesian computational mechanism behind the heading estimation.
研究表明,观察者可以根据视流准确估计自我运动方向(即航向),而视流会受到注意力的影响。然而,注意力如何影响估计中的序列依赖性仍不清楚。在本研究中,参与者进行了两次实验。结果表明,当分配给航向估计任务的注意力资源减少时,估计的准确性会降低。此外,对当前呈现的标题的估计偏向于对之前看到的标题的估计,显示出序列依赖性。特别是,当分配给之前(当前)看到的标题的注意力资源减少时,这种效应会减弱(增强)。此外,重要的是,我们建立了一个贝叶斯推理模型,其中包含了受注意力调节的可能性,并定性地预测了估计准确性和序列依赖性的变化。总之,目前的研究表明,注意力会影响视流航向估计的序列依赖性,并揭示了航向估计背后的贝叶斯计算机制。
{"title":"A Bayesian inference model can predict the effects of attention on the serial dependence in heading estimation from optic flow.","authors":"Qi Sun,Si-Yu Wang,Lin-Zhe Zhan,Fan-Huan You,Qian Sun","doi":"10.1167/jov.24.9.11","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1167/jov.24.9.11","url":null,"abstract":"It has been demonstrated that observers can accurately estimate their self-motion direction (i.e., heading) from optic flow, which can be affected by attention. However, it remains unclear how attention affects the serial dependence in the estimation. In the current study, participants conducted two experiments. The results showed that the estimation accuracy decreased when attentional resources allocated to the heading estimation task were reduced. Additionally, the estimates of currently presented headings were biased toward the headings of previously seen headings, showing serial dependence. Especially, this effect decreased (increased) when the attentional resources allocated to the previously (currently) seen headings were reduced. Furthermore, importantly, we developed a Bayesian inference model, which incorporated attention-modulated likelihoods and qualitatively predicted changes in the estimation accuracy and serial dependence. In summary, the current study shows that attention affects the serial dependence in heading estimation from optic flow and reveals the Bayesian computational mechanism behind the heading estimation.","PeriodicalId":49955,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Vision","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.8,"publicationDate":"2024-09-03","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142266497","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The preferred retinal loci when the eyes converge. 双眼会聚时的首选视网膜位置。
IF 2 4区 心理学 Q2 OPHTHALMOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-09-03 DOI: 10.1167/jov.24.9.15
Norick R Bowers, Josselin Gautier, Susana T L Chung, Martin S Banks, Austin Roorda

The preferred retinal locus (PRL) is the position on the retina to which humans direct stimuli during fixation. In healthy normal eyes, it has been shown to be very stable across time and between different tasks. Previous measurements of the PRL have been made under monocular viewing conditions. The current study examines where the PRLs in the two eyes' retinas are when subjects fixate binocularly and whether they shift when the demand for the eyes to converge is changed. Our apparatus allows us to see exactly where binocular stimuli fell on the two retinas during binocular fixation. Thus, our technique bypasses some of the issues involved in measuring binocular alignment with subjective techniques and previous objective techniques that use conventional eye trackers. These results show that PRLs shift slightly but systematically as the demand for convergence increases. The shifts cause under-convergence (also called exo fixation disparity) for near targets. They are not large enough to cause a break in binocular fusion. The fixation disparity we observed with increasing vergence demand is similar to fixation disparity observed in previous reports.

视网膜首选位置(PRL)是人类在固定刺激物时将其指向的视网膜位置。在健康的正常眼睛中,它在不同时间和不同任务之间都非常稳定。以往对 PRL 的测量都是在单眼观看条件下进行的。目前的研究则是检查受试者双眼凝视时两眼视网膜上的 PRL 的位置,以及当改变双眼会聚的要求时,PRL 是否会发生变化。通过我们的仪器,我们可以准确地看到双眼固定时双眼刺激物落在两眼视网膜上的位置。因此,我们的技术绕过了使用主观技术和以前使用传统眼球跟踪器的客观技术测量双眼对准所涉及的一些问题。这些结果表明,随着辐辏需求的增加,PRL 会发生轻微但系统的偏移。这种偏移会导致近距离目标的辐辏不足(也称为外定点差距)。这种偏移不足以导致双眼融合中断。我们观察到的随着辐辏需求的增加而产生的固视差异与之前报告中观察到的固视差异相似。
{"title":"The preferred retinal loci when the eyes converge.","authors":"Norick R Bowers, Josselin Gautier, Susana T L Chung, Martin S Banks, Austin Roorda","doi":"10.1167/jov.24.9.15","DOIUrl":"10.1167/jov.24.9.15","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The preferred retinal locus (PRL) is the position on the retina to which humans direct stimuli during fixation. In healthy normal eyes, it has been shown to be very stable across time and between different tasks. Previous measurements of the PRL have been made under monocular viewing conditions. The current study examines where the PRLs in the two eyes' retinas are when subjects fixate binocularly and whether they shift when the demand for the eyes to converge is changed. Our apparatus allows us to see exactly where binocular stimuli fell on the two retinas during binocular fixation. Thus, our technique bypasses some of the issues involved in measuring binocular alignment with subjective techniques and previous objective techniques that use conventional eye trackers. These results show that PRLs shift slightly but systematically as the demand for convergence increases. The shifts cause under-convergence (also called exo fixation disparity) for near targets. They are not large enough to cause a break in binocular fusion. The fixation disparity we observed with increasing vergence demand is similar to fixation disparity observed in previous reports.</p>","PeriodicalId":49955,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Vision","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.0,"publicationDate":"2024-09-03","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11440557/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142299674","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Journal of Vision
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1