首页 > 最新文献

Journal of Vision最新文献

英文 中文
Retinotopy drives the variation in scene responses across visual field map divisions of the occipital place area. 视网膜视图驱动了枕叶位置区视场图各分区场景反应的变化。
IF 2 4区 心理学 Q2 OPHTHALMOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-08-01 DOI: 10.1167/jov.24.8.10
Catriona L Scrivener, Elisa Zamboni, Antony B Morland, Edward H Silson

The occipital place area (OPA) is a scene-selective region on the lateral surface of human occipitotemporal cortex that spatially overlaps multiple visual field maps, as well as portions of cortex that are not currently defined as retinotopic. Here we combined population receptive field modeling and responses to scenes in a representational similarity analysis (RSA) framework to test the prediction that the OPA's visual field map divisions contribute uniquely to the overall pattern of scene selectivity within the OPA. Consistent with this prediction, the patterns of response to a set of complex scenes were heterogeneous between maps. To explain this heterogeneity, we tested the explanatory power of seven candidate models using RSA. These models spanned different scene dimensions (Content, Expanse, Distance), low- and high-level visual features, and navigational affordances. None of the tested models could account for the variation in scene response observed between the OPA's visual field maps. However, the heterogeneity in scene response was correlated with the differences in retinotopic profiles across maps. These data highlight the need to carefully examine the relationship between regions defined as category-selective and the underlying retinotopy, and they suggest that, in the case of the OPA, it may not be appropriate to conceptualize it as a single scene-selective region.

枕叶位置区(OPA)是人类枕颞皮层外侧表面的一个场景选择区,它在空间上与多个视场图以及目前尚未定义为视网膜视位的皮层部分重叠。在这里,我们在表象相似性分析(RSA)框架中结合了群体感受野建模和对场景的反应,以检验 OPA 的视野图划分对 OPA 内场景选择性整体模式的独特贡献这一预测。与这一预测相一致的是,不同视场图之间对一组复杂场景的反应模式是不同的。为了解释这种异质性,我们使用 RSA 测试了七个候选模型的解释力。这些模型涵盖了不同的场景维度(内容、广度、距离)、低级和高级视觉特征以及导航能力。没有一个测试模型能够解释在 OPA 视场图之间观察到的场景反应差异。然而,场景反应的异质性与不同视网膜图之间视网膜图谱的差异相关。这些数据突出表明,有必要仔细研究被定义为类别选择性的区域与底层视网膜视图之间的关系,它们还表明,就 OPA 而言,将其概念化为单一的场景选择性区域可能并不合适。
{"title":"Retinotopy drives the variation in scene responses across visual field map divisions of the occipital place area.","authors":"Catriona L Scrivener, Elisa Zamboni, Antony B Morland, Edward H Silson","doi":"10.1167/jov.24.8.10","DOIUrl":"10.1167/jov.24.8.10","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The occipital place area (OPA) is a scene-selective region on the lateral surface of human occipitotemporal cortex that spatially overlaps multiple visual field maps, as well as portions of cortex that are not currently defined as retinotopic. Here we combined population receptive field modeling and responses to scenes in a representational similarity analysis (RSA) framework to test the prediction that the OPA's visual field map divisions contribute uniquely to the overall pattern of scene selectivity within the OPA. Consistent with this prediction, the patterns of response to a set of complex scenes were heterogeneous between maps. To explain this heterogeneity, we tested the explanatory power of seven candidate models using RSA. These models spanned different scene dimensions (Content, Expanse, Distance), low- and high-level visual features, and navigational affordances. None of the tested models could account for the variation in scene response observed between the OPA's visual field maps. However, the heterogeneity in scene response was correlated with the differences in retinotopic profiles across maps. These data highlight the need to carefully examine the relationship between regions defined as category-selective and the underlying retinotopy, and they suggest that, in the case of the OPA, it may not be appropriate to conceptualize it as a single scene-selective region.</p>","PeriodicalId":49955,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Vision","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.0,"publicationDate":"2024-08-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11343012/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142019360","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Consistent metacognitive efficiency and variable response biases in peripheral vision. 外围视觉中一致的元认知效率和可变的反应偏差。
IF 2 4区 心理学 Q2 OPHTHALMOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-08-01 DOI: 10.1167/jov.24.8.4
Joseph Pruitt, J D Knotts, Brian Odegaard

Across the visual periphery, perceptual and metacognitive abilities differ depending on the locus of visual attention, the location of peripheral stimulus presentation, the task design, and many other factors. In this investigation, we aimed to illuminate the relationship between attention and eccentricity in the visual periphery by estimating perceptual sensitivity, metacognitive sensitivity, and response biases across the visual field. In a 2AFC detection task, participants were asked to determine whether a signal was present or absent at one of eight peripheral locations (±10°, 20°, 30°, and 40°), using either a valid or invalid attentional cue. As expected, results revealed that perceptual sensitivity declined with eccentricity and was modulated by attention, with higher sensitivity on validly cued trials. Furthermore, a significant main effect of eccentricity on response bias emerged, with variable (but relatively unbiased) c'a values from 10° to 30°, and conservative c'a values at 40°. Regarding metacognitive sensitivity, significant main effects of attention and eccentricity were found, with metacognitive sensitivity decreasing with eccentricity, and decreasing in the invalid cue condition. Interestingly, metacognitive efficiency, as measured by the ratio of meta-d'a/d'a, was not modulated by attention or eccentricity. Overall, these findings demonstrate (1) that in some circumstances, observers have surprisingly robust metacognitive insights into how performance changes across the visual field and (2) that the periphery may be subject to variable detection biases that are contingent on the exact location in peripheral space.

在整个视觉外围,知觉和元认知能力因视觉注意力的位置、外围刺激呈现的位置、任务设计和许多其他因素的不同而不同。在这项研究中,我们旨在通过估计整个视野中的知觉灵敏度、元认知灵敏度和反应偏差,来阐明视觉周边的注意力和偏心率之间的关系。在一项 2AFC 检测任务中,受试者被要求使用有效或无效的注意线索,判断信号在八个外围位置(±10°、20°、30° 和 40°)中的一个位置是存在还是不存在。不出所料,结果显示感知灵敏度随偏心率下降,并受注意力调节,在有效提示的试验中灵敏度较高。此外,偏心率对反应偏差有明显的主效应,10°到30°之间的c'a值不尽相同(但相对无偏),而40°时的c'a值较保守。在元认知灵敏度方面,注意力和偏心率具有显著的主效应,元认知灵敏度随偏心率的增加而降低,并且在无效线索条件下降低。有趣的是,元认知效率(以元-d'a/d'a 的比率来衡量)并不受注意力或偏心率的调节。总之,这些研究结果表明:(1) 在某些情况下,观察者对整个视野中的表现如何变化具有令人惊讶的强大元认知洞察力;(2) 外围可能会受到可变检测偏差的影响,而这些偏差取决于外围空间的确切位置。
{"title":"Consistent metacognitive efficiency and variable response biases in peripheral vision.","authors":"Joseph Pruitt, J D Knotts, Brian Odegaard","doi":"10.1167/jov.24.8.4","DOIUrl":"10.1167/jov.24.8.4","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Across the visual periphery, perceptual and metacognitive abilities differ depending on the locus of visual attention, the location of peripheral stimulus presentation, the task design, and many other factors. In this investigation, we aimed to illuminate the relationship between attention and eccentricity in the visual periphery by estimating perceptual sensitivity, metacognitive sensitivity, and response biases across the visual field. In a 2AFC detection task, participants were asked to determine whether a signal was present or absent at one of eight peripheral locations (±10°, 20°, 30°, and 40°), using either a valid or invalid attentional cue. As expected, results revealed that perceptual sensitivity declined with eccentricity and was modulated by attention, with higher sensitivity on validly cued trials. Furthermore, a significant main effect of eccentricity on response bias emerged, with variable (but relatively unbiased) c'a values from 10° to 30°, and conservative c'a values at 40°. Regarding metacognitive sensitivity, significant main effects of attention and eccentricity were found, with metacognitive sensitivity decreasing with eccentricity, and decreasing in the invalid cue condition. Interestingly, metacognitive efficiency, as measured by the ratio of meta-d'a/d'a, was not modulated by attention or eccentricity. Overall, these findings demonstrate (1) that in some circumstances, observers have surprisingly robust metacognitive insights into how performance changes across the visual field and (2) that the periphery may be subject to variable detection biases that are contingent on the exact location in peripheral space.</p>","PeriodicalId":49955,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Vision","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.0,"publicationDate":"2024-08-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11314628/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141903375","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Weighted power summation and contrast normalization mechanisms account for short-latency eye movements to motion and disparity of sine-wave gratings and broadband visual stimuli in humans. 加权功率求和与对比度归一化机制解释了人类对正弦波光栅和宽带视觉刺激的运动和差异的短时眼动。
IF 2 4区 心理学 Q2 OPHTHALMOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-08-01 DOI: 10.1167/jov.24.8.14
Boris M Sheliga, Edmond J FitzGibbon

In this paper, we show that the model we proposed earlier to account for the disparity vergence eye movements (disparity vergence responses, or DVRs) in response to horizontal and vertical disparity steps of white noise visual stimuli also provides an excellent description of the short-latency ocular following responses (OFRs) to broadband stimuli in the visual motion domain. In addition, we reanalyzed the data and applied the model to several earlier studies that used sine-wave gratings (single or a combination of two or three gratings) and white noise stimuli. The model provides a very good account of all of these data. The model postulates that the short-latency eye movements-OFRs and DVRs-can be accounted for by the operation of two factors: an excitatory drive, determined by a weighted sum of contributions of stimulus Fourier components, scaled by a global contrast normalization mechanism. The output of the operation of these two factors is then nonlinearly scaled by the total contrast of the stimulus. Despite different roles of disparity (horizontal and vertical) and motion signals in visual scene analyses, the earliest processing stages of these different signals appear to be very similar.

在本文中,我们证明了早先提出的用于解释对白噪声视觉刺激的水平和垂直差距阶跃反应的差距辐辏眼动(差距辐辏反应或DVRs)的模型,也能很好地描述视觉运动域中对宽带刺激的短时眼球跟随反应(OFRs)。此外,我们还对数据进行了重新分析,并将该模型应用于早期的几项使用正弦波光栅(单个或两个或三个光栅的组合)和白噪声刺激的研究。该模型很好地解释了所有这些数据。该模型假定,短时眼动--OFR和DVR--可以通过两个因素的作用来解释:一个是兴奋驱动,由刺激傅立叶成分贡献的加权和决定,并通过全局对比度归一化机制进行缩放。然后,这两个因素运作的输出被刺激的总对比度非线性地缩放。尽管色差(水平和垂直)和运动信号在视觉场景分析中扮演着不同的角色,但这些不同信号的最初处理阶段似乎非常相似。
{"title":"Weighted power summation and contrast normalization mechanisms account for short-latency eye movements to motion and disparity of sine-wave gratings and broadband visual stimuli in humans.","authors":"Boris M Sheliga, Edmond J FitzGibbon","doi":"10.1167/jov.24.8.14","DOIUrl":"10.1167/jov.24.8.14","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>In this paper, we show that the model we proposed earlier to account for the disparity vergence eye movements (disparity vergence responses, or DVRs) in response to horizontal and vertical disparity steps of white noise visual stimuli also provides an excellent description of the short-latency ocular following responses (OFRs) to broadband stimuli in the visual motion domain. In addition, we reanalyzed the data and applied the model to several earlier studies that used sine-wave gratings (single or a combination of two or three gratings) and white noise stimuli. The model provides a very good account of all of these data. The model postulates that the short-latency eye movements-OFRs and DVRs-can be accounted for by the operation of two factors: an excitatory drive, determined by a weighted sum of contributions of stimulus Fourier components, scaled by a global contrast normalization mechanism. The output of the operation of these two factors is then nonlinearly scaled by the total contrast of the stimulus. Despite different roles of disparity (horizontal and vertical) and motion signals in visual scene analyses, the earliest processing stages of these different signals appear to be very similar.</p>","PeriodicalId":49955,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Vision","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.0,"publicationDate":"2024-08-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11363211/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142057109","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Inter-individual variability (but intra-individual stability) of overt versus covert rehearsal strategies in a digital Corsi task. 数字柯西任务中公开与隐蔽预演策略的个体间差异性(但个体内稳定性)。
IF 2 4区 心理学 Q2 OPHTHALMOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-08-01 DOI: 10.1167/jov.24.8.2
Lílian de Sardenberg Schmid, Gregor Hardiess

The Corsi (block-tapping) paradigm is a classic and well-established visuospatial working memory task in humans involving internal computations (memorizing of item sequences, organizing and updating the memorandum, and recall processes), as well as both overt and covert shifts of attention to facilitate rehearsal, serving to maintain the Corsi sequences during the retention phase. Here, we introduce a novel digital version of a Corsi task in which i) the difficulty of the memorandum (using sequence lengths ranging from 3 to 8) was controlled, ii) the execution of overt and/or covert attention as well as the visuospatial working memory load during the retention phase was manipulated, and iii) shifts of attention were quantified in all experimental phases. With this, we present behavioral data that demonstrate, characterize, and classify the individual effects of overt and covert strategies used as a means of encoding and rehearsal. In a full within-subject design, we tested 28 participants who had to solve three different Corsi conditions. While in condition A neither of the two strategies were restricted, in condition B the overt and in condition C the overt as well as the covert strategies were suppressed. Analyzing Corsi span, (eye) exploration index, and pupil size (change), data clearly show a continuum between overt and covert strategies over all participants (indicating inter-individual variability). Further, all participants showed stable strategy choice (indicating intra-individual stability), meaning that the preferred strategy was maintained in all three conditions, phases, and sequence lengths of the experiment.

科尔西(敲击积木)范式是一项经典且行之有效的人类视觉空间工作记忆任务,它涉及内部计算(记忆项目序列、组织和更新备忘录以及回忆过程),以及明显和隐蔽的注意力转移以促进排练,从而在保持阶段维持科尔西序列。在这里,我们介绍了一种新颖的数字版 Corsi 任务:i) 控制备忘录的难度(序列长度从 3 到 8 不等);ii) 在保留阶段操纵公开和/或隐蔽注意力的执行以及视觉空间工作记忆负荷;iii) 在所有实验阶段量化注意力的转移。通过这些数据,我们展示了作为编码和预演手段的公开和隐蔽策略的个体效应,并对这些效应进行了描述和分类。在一个完整的主体内设计中,我们对 28 名参与者进行了测试,他们必须解决三种不同的 Corsi 条件。在条件 A 中,两种策略都不受限制;而在条件 B 中,公开策略受到限制;在条件 C 中,公开策略和隐蔽策略都受到抑制。通过分析 Corsi 跨度、(眼睛)探索指数和瞳孔大小(变化),数据清楚地显示出所有参与者在公开和隐蔽策略之间的连续性(表明个体间存在差异)。此外,所有参与者都表现出稳定的策略选择(表明个体内部的稳定性),这意味着在实验的所有三个条件、阶段和序列长度中,首选策略都得以保持。
{"title":"Inter-individual variability (but intra-individual stability) of overt versus covert rehearsal strategies in a digital Corsi task.","authors":"Lílian de Sardenberg Schmid, Gregor Hardiess","doi":"10.1167/jov.24.8.2","DOIUrl":"10.1167/jov.24.8.2","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The Corsi (block-tapping) paradigm is a classic and well-established visuospatial working memory task in humans involving internal computations (memorizing of item sequences, organizing and updating the memorandum, and recall processes), as well as both overt and covert shifts of attention to facilitate rehearsal, serving to maintain the Corsi sequences during the retention phase. Here, we introduce a novel digital version of a Corsi task in which i) the difficulty of the memorandum (using sequence lengths ranging from 3 to 8) was controlled, ii) the execution of overt and/or covert attention as well as the visuospatial working memory load during the retention phase was manipulated, and iii) shifts of attention were quantified in all experimental phases. With this, we present behavioral data that demonstrate, characterize, and classify the individual effects of overt and covert strategies used as a means of encoding and rehearsal. In a full within-subject design, we tested 28 participants who had to solve three different Corsi conditions. While in condition A neither of the two strategies were restricted, in condition B the overt and in condition C the overt as well as the covert strategies were suppressed. Analyzing Corsi span, (eye) exploration index, and pupil size (change), data clearly show a continuum between overt and covert strategies over all participants (indicating inter-individual variability). Further, all participants showed stable strategy choice (indicating intra-individual stability), meaning that the preferred strategy was maintained in all three conditions, phases, and sequence lengths of the experiment.</p>","PeriodicalId":49955,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Vision","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.0,"publicationDate":"2024-08-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11305427/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141861396","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Contribution of low-level motion to position shifts. 低水平运动对位置移动的影响。
IF 2 4区 心理学 Q2 OPHTHALMOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-08-01 DOI: 10.1167/jov.24.8.13
Donald I A MacLeod, Patrick Cavanagh, Stuart Anstis

Motion can produce large changes in the apparent locations of briefly flashed tests presented on or near the motion. These motion-induced position shifts may have a variety of sources. They may be due to a frame effect where the moving pattern provides a frame of reference for the locations of events within it. The motion of the background may act through high-level mechanisms that track its explicit contours or the motion may act on position through the signals from low-level motion detectors. Here we isolate the contribution of low-level motion by eliminating explicit contours and trackable features. In this case, motion still supports a robust shift in probe locations with the shift being in the direction of the motion that follows the probe. Although robust, the magnitude of the shift in our first experiment is about 20% of the shift seen in a previous study with explicit frames and, in the second, about 45% of that found with explicit frames. Clearly, low-level motion alone can produce position shifts although the magnitude is much reduced compared to that seen when high-level mechanisms can contribute.

运动会使在运动中或运动附近出现的短暂闪烁测试的视位置发生巨大变化。这些由运动引起的位置移动可能有多种来源。它们可能是由框架效应引起的,即运动的图案为其中事件的位置提供了参照框架。背景的运动可能通过追踪其清晰轮廓的高级机制发挥作用,也可能通过低级运动检测器发出的信号对位置产生作用。在这里,我们通过消除明确的轮廓和可跟踪特征来隔离低层次运动的贡献。在这种情况下,运动仍能支持探头位置的稳健移动,移动方向与探头的运动方向一致。在我们的第一个实验中,虽然运动支持了探针位置的稳健移动,但其移动幅度仅为之前研究中显性框架移动幅度的 20%,而在第二个实验中,仅为显性框架移动幅度的 45%。很明显,低级运动本身就能产生位置偏移,尽管与高级机制所产生的位置偏移相比,其幅度要小得多。
{"title":"Contribution of low-level motion to position shifts.","authors":"Donald I A MacLeod, Patrick Cavanagh, Stuart Anstis","doi":"10.1167/jov.24.8.13","DOIUrl":"10.1167/jov.24.8.13","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Motion can produce large changes in the apparent locations of briefly flashed tests presented on or near the motion. These motion-induced position shifts may have a variety of sources. They may be due to a frame effect where the moving pattern provides a frame of reference for the locations of events within it. The motion of the background may act through high-level mechanisms that track its explicit contours or the motion may act on position through the signals from low-level motion detectors. Here we isolate the contribution of low-level motion by eliminating explicit contours and trackable features. In this case, motion still supports a robust shift in probe locations with the shift being in the direction of the motion that follows the probe. Although robust, the magnitude of the shift in our first experiment is about 20% of the shift seen in a previous study with explicit frames and, in the second, about 45% of that found with explicit frames. Clearly, low-level motion alone can produce position shifts although the magnitude is much reduced compared to that seen when high-level mechanisms can contribute.</p>","PeriodicalId":49955,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Vision","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.0,"publicationDate":"2024-08-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11346155/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142037572","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Influence of frame and probe paths on the frame effect. 框架和探针路径对框架效应的影响。
IF 2 4区 心理学 Q2 OPHTHALMOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-07-02 DOI: 10.1167/jov.24.7.11
Stuart Anstis, Patrick Cavanagh

Moving frames produce large displacements in the perceived location of flashed and continuously moving probes. In a series of experiments, we test the contributions of the probe's displacement and the frame's displacement on the strength of the frame's effect. In the first experiment, we find a dramatic position shift of flashed probes whereas the effect on a continuously moving probe is only one-third as strong. In Experiment 2, we show that the absence of an effect for the static probe is a consequence of its perceptual grouping with the static background. As long as the continuously present probe has some motion, it appears to group to some extent with the frame and show an illusory shift of intermediate magnitude. Finally, we informally explored the illusory shifts seen for a continuously moving probe when the frame itself has a more complex path. In this case, the probe appears to group more strongly with the frame. Overall, the effects of the frame on the probe demonstrate the outcome of a competition between the frame and the static background in determining the frame of reference for the probe's perceived position.

移动框架会对闪烁和连续移动探针的感知位置产生较大的位移。在一系列实验中,我们测试了探针位移和框架位移对框架效应强度的影响。在第一个实验中,我们发现闪烁探针的位置发生了显著的位移,而连续移动探针的位移效应仅为闪烁探针的三分之一。在实验 2 中,我们发现静态探针之所以没有效果,是因为它在知觉上被归类为静态背景。只要连续出现的探针有一定的运动,它似乎就会在一定程度上与框架组合在一起,并显示出中等程度的虚幻偏移。最后,我们非正式地探讨了连续运动的探针在框架本身具有更复杂路径时出现的虚幻偏移。在这种情况下,探针似乎会与框架发生更强烈的组合。总之,框架对探针的影响表明,在确定探针感知位置的参照框架时,框架与静态背景之间存在竞争。
{"title":"Influence of frame and probe paths on the frame effect.","authors":"Stuart Anstis, Patrick Cavanagh","doi":"10.1167/jov.24.7.11","DOIUrl":"10.1167/jov.24.7.11","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Moving frames produce large displacements in the perceived location of flashed and continuously moving probes. In a series of experiments, we test the contributions of the probe's displacement and the frame's displacement on the strength of the frame's effect. In the first experiment, we find a dramatic position shift of flashed probes whereas the effect on a continuously moving probe is only one-third as strong. In Experiment 2, we show that the absence of an effect for the static probe is a consequence of its perceptual grouping with the static background. As long as the continuously present probe has some motion, it appears to group to some extent with the frame and show an illusory shift of intermediate magnitude. Finally, we informally explored the illusory shifts seen for a continuously moving probe when the frame itself has a more complex path. In this case, the probe appears to group more strongly with the frame. Overall, the effects of the frame on the probe demonstrate the outcome of a competition between the frame and the static background in determining the frame of reference for the probe's perceived position.</p>","PeriodicalId":49955,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Vision","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.0,"publicationDate":"2024-07-02","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11257013/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141621597","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Super recognizers: Increased sensitivity or reduced biases? Insights from serial dependence. 超级识别器:提高灵敏度还是减少偏差?序列依赖性的启示
IF 2 4区 心理学 Q2 OPHTHALMOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-07-02 DOI: 10.1167/jov.24.7.13
Fiammetta Marini, Mauro Manassi, Meike Ramon

Super recognizers (SRs) are people that exhibit a naturally occurring superiority for processing facial identity. Despite the increase of SR research, the mechanisms underlying their exceptional abilities remain unclear. Here, we investigated whether the enhanced facial identity processing of SRs could be attributed to the lack of sequential effects, such as serial dependence. In serial dependence, perception of stimulus features is assimilated toward stimuli presented in previous trials. This constant error in visual perception has been proposed as a mechanism that promotes perceptual stability in everyday life. We hypothesized that an absence of this constant source of error in SRs could account for their superior processing-potentially in a domain-general fashion. We tested SRs (n = 17) identified via a recently proposed diagnostic framework (Ramon, 2021) and age-matched controls (n = 20) with two experiments probing serial dependence in the face and shape domains. In each experiment, observers were presented with randomly morphed face identities or shapes and were asked to adjust a face's identity or a shape to match the stimulus they saw. We found serial dependence in controls and SRs alike, with no difference in its magnitude across groups. Interestingly, we found that serial dependence impacted the performance of SRs more than that of controls. Taken together, our results show that enhanced face identity processing skills in SRs cannot be attributed to the lack of serial dependence. Rather, serial dependence, a beneficial nested error in our visual system, may in fact further stabilize the perception of SRs and thus enhance their visual processing proficiency.

超级识别者(SR)是指在处理面部特征方面表现出自然优势的人。尽管对超级辨认者的研究越来越多,但其超常能力的内在机制仍不清楚。在此,我们研究了超级辨认者面部身份处理能力的增强是否可归因于缺乏序列效应,如序列依赖。在序列依赖中,对刺激特征的感知会被之前试验中出现的刺激所同化。这种视觉感知中的恒定误差被认为是日常生活中促进感知稳定性的一种机制。我们假设,如果 SR 中不存在这种恒定的误差源,就可以解释其卓越的处理能力--有可能是一种领域通用的方式。我们通过最近提出的诊断框架(Ramon,2021 年)对 SR(n = 17)和年龄匹配的对照组(n = 20)进行了测试,这两个实验分别探究了脸部和形状领域的序列依赖性。在每个实验中,观察者都会看到随机变形的脸部特征或形状,并被要求调整脸部特征或形状以匹配他们所看到的刺激。我们在对照组和自闭症患者身上都发现了序列依赖性,而且不同组别之间的依赖程度没有差异。有趣的是,我们发现序列依赖对 SR 表现的影响大于对照组。综上所述,我们的研究结果表明,自闭症患者面部识别处理能力的提高不能归因于缺乏序列依赖。相反,序列依赖是我们视觉系统中一个有益的嵌套错误,它实际上可能会进一步稳定自闭症儿童的感知,从而提高他们的视觉处理能力。
{"title":"Super recognizers: Increased sensitivity or reduced biases? Insights from serial dependence.","authors":"Fiammetta Marini, Mauro Manassi, Meike Ramon","doi":"10.1167/jov.24.7.13","DOIUrl":"10.1167/jov.24.7.13","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Super recognizers (SRs) are people that exhibit a naturally occurring superiority for processing facial identity. Despite the increase of SR research, the mechanisms underlying their exceptional abilities remain unclear. Here, we investigated whether the enhanced facial identity processing of SRs could be attributed to the lack of sequential effects, such as serial dependence. In serial dependence, perception of stimulus features is assimilated toward stimuli presented in previous trials. This constant error in visual perception has been proposed as a mechanism that promotes perceptual stability in everyday life. We hypothesized that an absence of this constant source of error in SRs could account for their superior processing-potentially in a domain-general fashion. We tested SRs (n = 17) identified via a recently proposed diagnostic framework (Ramon, 2021) and age-matched controls (n = 20) with two experiments probing serial dependence in the face and shape domains. In each experiment, observers were presented with randomly morphed face identities or shapes and were asked to adjust a face's identity or a shape to match the stimulus they saw. We found serial dependence in controls and SRs alike, with no difference in its magnitude across groups. Interestingly, we found that serial dependence impacted the performance of SRs more than that of controls. Taken together, our results show that enhanced face identity processing skills in SRs cannot be attributed to the lack of serial dependence. Rather, serial dependence, a beneficial nested error in our visual system, may in fact further stabilize the perception of SRs and thus enhance their visual processing proficiency.</p>","PeriodicalId":49955,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Vision","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.0,"publicationDate":"2024-07-02","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11271810/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141753232","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
When knowing the activity is not enough to predict gaze. 当了解活动不足以预测凝视时。
IF 2 4区 心理学 Q2 OPHTHALMOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-07-02 DOI: 10.1167/jov.24.7.6
Andrea Ghiani, Daan Amelink, Eli Brenner, Ignace T C Hooge, Roy S Hessels

It is reasonable to assume that where people look in the world is largely determined by what they are doing. The reasoning is that the activity determines where it is useful to look at each moment in time. Assuming that it is vital to accurately judge the positions of the steps when navigating a staircase, it is surprising that people differ a lot in the extent to which they look at the steps. Apparently, some people consider the accuracy of peripheral vision, predictability of the step size, and feeling the edges of the steps with their feet to be good enough. If so, occluding part of the view of the staircase and making it more important to place one's feet gently might make it more beneficial to look directly at the steps before stepping onto them, so that people will more consistently look at many steps. We tested this idea by asking people to walk on staircases, either with or without a tray with two cups of water on it. When carrying the tray, people walked more slowly, but they shifted their gaze across steps in much the same way as they did when walking without the tray. They did not look at more steps. There was a clear positive correlation between the fraction of steps that people looked at when walking with and without the tray. Thus, the variability in the extent to which people look at the steps persists when one makes walking on the staircase more challenging.

我们有理由认为,人们观察世界的角度主要取决于他们在做什么。理由是,活动决定了在每个时刻看哪里是有用的。假设在走楼梯时,准确判断台阶的位置至关重要,那么令人惊讶的是,人们在看台阶的程度上存在很大差异。显然,有些人认为外围视觉的准确性、台阶大小的可预测性以及用脚感觉台阶边缘就足够了。如果是这样的话,遮住楼梯的部分视线,并让人们更加重视脚的轻放,可能会让人们在踏上台阶之前直接看台阶更有好处,这样人们就会更加持续地看很多台阶。我们测试了这一想法,让人们在楼梯上行走,可以带着或不带一个装有两杯水的托盘。端着托盘时,人们走得更慢,但他们在台阶上移动视线的方式与不端着托盘时基本相同。他们没有看更多的台阶。人们在端着托盘和不端着托盘的情况下所看台阶的比例明显呈正相关。因此,当在楼梯上行走更具挑战性时,人们注视台阶的程度仍然存在差异。
{"title":"When knowing the activity is not enough to predict gaze.","authors":"Andrea Ghiani, Daan Amelink, Eli Brenner, Ignace T C Hooge, Roy S Hessels","doi":"10.1167/jov.24.7.6","DOIUrl":"10.1167/jov.24.7.6","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>It is reasonable to assume that where people look in the world is largely determined by what they are doing. The reasoning is that the activity determines where it is useful to look at each moment in time. Assuming that it is vital to accurately judge the positions of the steps when navigating a staircase, it is surprising that people differ a lot in the extent to which they look at the steps. Apparently, some people consider the accuracy of peripheral vision, predictability of the step size, and feeling the edges of the steps with their feet to be good enough. If so, occluding part of the view of the staircase and making it more important to place one's feet gently might make it more beneficial to look directly at the steps before stepping onto them, so that people will more consistently look at many steps. We tested this idea by asking people to walk on staircases, either with or without a tray with two cups of water on it. When carrying the tray, people walked more slowly, but they shifted their gaze across steps in much the same way as they did when walking without the tray. They did not look at more steps. There was a clear positive correlation between the fraction of steps that people looked at when walking with and without the tray. Thus, the variability in the extent to which people look at the steps persists when one makes walking on the staircase more challenging.</p>","PeriodicalId":49955,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Vision","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.0,"publicationDate":"2024-07-02","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11238878/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141564951","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Illusory light drives pupil responses in primates. 虚幻光线驱动灵长类动物的瞳孔反应。
IF 2 4区 心理学 Q2 OPHTHALMOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-07-02 DOI: 10.1167/jov.24.7.14
Jean-Baptiste Durand, Sarah Marchand, Ilyas Nasres, Bruno Laeng, Vanessa De Castro

In humans, the eye pupils respond to both physical light sensed by the retina and mental representations of light produced by the brain. Notably, our pupils constrict when a visual stimulus is illusorily perceived brighter, even if retinal illumination is constant. However, it remains unclear whether such perceptual penetrability of pupil responses is an epiphenomenon unique to humans or whether it represents an adaptive mechanism shared with other animals to anticipate variations in retinal illumination between successive eye fixations. To address this issue, we measured the pupil responses of both humans and macaque monkeys exposed to three chromatic versions (cyan, magenta, and yellow) of the Asahi brightness illusion. We found that the stimuli illusorily perceived brighter or darker trigger differential pupil responses that are very similar in macaques and human participants. Additionally, we show that this phenomenon exhibits an analogous cyan bias in both primate species. Beyond evincing the macaque monkey as a relevant model to study the perceptual penetrability of pupil responses, our results suggest that this phenomenon is tuned to ecological conditions because the exposure to a "bright cyan-bluish sky" may be associated with increased risks of dazzle and retinal damages.

人类的瞳孔会对视网膜感知到的物理光线和大脑产生的光线心理表征做出反应。值得注意的是,当视觉刺激被错误地认为更亮时,即使视网膜照度保持不变,我们的瞳孔也会收缩。然而,瞳孔反应的这种知觉穿透性是人类独有的表象,还是代表了一种与其他动物共享的适应机制,以预测连续定睛之间视网膜照度的变化,目前仍不清楚。为了解决这个问题,我们测量了人类和猕猴对三种色度版本(青色、洋红色和黄色)的朝日亮度幻觉的瞳孔反应。我们发现,在猕猴和人类参与者中,被幻觉认为更亮或更暗的刺激会引发不同的瞳孔反应,这一点非常相似。此外,我们还发现这种现象在两种灵长类动物身上都表现出类似的青色偏差。除了证明猕猴是研究瞳孔反应知觉穿透性的相关模型外,我们的研究结果还表明,这种现象与生态条件有关,因为暴露于 "明亮的青蓝色天空 "可能会增加眩晕和视网膜损伤的风险。
{"title":"Illusory light drives pupil responses in primates.","authors":"Jean-Baptiste Durand, Sarah Marchand, Ilyas Nasres, Bruno Laeng, Vanessa De Castro","doi":"10.1167/jov.24.7.14","DOIUrl":"10.1167/jov.24.7.14","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>In humans, the eye pupils respond to both physical light sensed by the retina and mental representations of light produced by the brain. Notably, our pupils constrict when a visual stimulus is illusorily perceived brighter, even if retinal illumination is constant. However, it remains unclear whether such perceptual penetrability of pupil responses is an epiphenomenon unique to humans or whether it represents an adaptive mechanism shared with other animals to anticipate variations in retinal illumination between successive eye fixations. To address this issue, we measured the pupil responses of both humans and macaque monkeys exposed to three chromatic versions (cyan, magenta, and yellow) of the Asahi brightness illusion. We found that the stimuli illusorily perceived brighter or darker trigger differential pupil responses that are very similar in macaques and human participants. Additionally, we show that this phenomenon exhibits an analogous cyan bias in both primate species. Beyond evincing the macaque monkey as a relevant model to study the perceptual penetrability of pupil responses, our results suggest that this phenomenon is tuned to ecological conditions because the exposure to a \"bright cyan-bluish sky\" may be associated with increased risks of dazzle and retinal damages.</p>","PeriodicalId":49955,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Vision","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.0,"publicationDate":"2024-07-02","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11271809/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141753231","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Instruction alters the influence of allocentric landmarks in a reach task. 教学改变了伸手任务中分配中心地标的影响。
IF 2 4区 心理学 Q2 OPHTHALMOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-07-02 DOI: 10.1167/jov.24.7.17
Lina Musa, Xiaogang Yan, J Douglas Crawford

Allocentric landmarks have an implicit influence on aiming movements, but it is not clear how an explicit instruction (to aim relative to a landmark) influences reach accuracy and precision. Here, 12 participants performed a task with two instruction conditions (egocentric vs. allocentric) but with similar sensory and motor conditions. Participants fixated gaze near the center of a display aligned with their right shoulder while a target stimulus briefly appeared alongside a visual landmark in one visual field. After a brief mask/memory delay the landmark then reappeared at a different location (same or opposite visual field), creating an ego/allocentric conflict. In the egocentric condition, participants were instructed to ignore the landmark and point toward the remembered location of the target. In the allocentric condition, participants were instructed to remember the initial target location relative to the landmark and then reach relative to the shifted landmark (same or opposite visual field). To equalize motor execution between tasks, participants were instructed to anti-point (point to the visual field opposite to the remembered target) on 50% of the egocentric trials. Participants were more accurate and precise and quicker to react in the allocentric condition, especially when pointing to the opposite field. We also observed a visual field effect, where performance was worse overall in the right visual field. These results suggest that, when egocentric and allocentric cues conflict, explicit use of the visual landmark provides better reach performance than reliance on noisy egocentric signals. Such instructions might aid rehabilitation when the egocentric system is compromised by disease or injury.

异中心地标对瞄准动作有隐性影响,但显性指令(相对于地标进行瞄准)如何影响瞄准的准确性和精确度,目前尚不清楚。在这里,12 名参与者进行了一项任务,任务有两种指令条件(以自我为中心与以分配为中心),但感觉和运动条件相似。参与者将视线固定在与他们右肩对齐的显示屏中心附近,同时在一个视野中的视觉地标旁短暂出现一个目标刺激物。经过短暂的掩蔽/记忆延迟后,地标在不同的位置(同一视野或相反视野)再次出现,从而产生自我/全中心冲突。在自我中心条件下,参与者被要求忽略地标,指向记忆中的目标位置。在 "分配中心 "条件下,受试者被要求记住与地标相对的初始目标位置,然后相对于转移后的地标(相同或相反的视野)伸手。为了平衡不同任务之间的运动执行,参与者被要求在 50%的以自我为中心的试验中反指向(指向与记忆目标相反的视野)。在分配中心条件下,参与者的反应更准确、更精确、更迅速,尤其是在指向相反视野时。我们还观察到一种视野效应,即在右侧视野中的总体表现较差。这些结果表明,当 "自我中心 "和 "分配中心 "线索发生冲突时,明确地使用视觉地标比依赖于嘈杂的 "自我中心 "信号能提供更好的伸手能力。当自我中心系统因疾病或损伤而受到损害时,这样的指示可能有助于康复。
{"title":"Instruction alters the influence of allocentric landmarks in a reach task.","authors":"Lina Musa, Xiaogang Yan, J Douglas Crawford","doi":"10.1167/jov.24.7.17","DOIUrl":"10.1167/jov.24.7.17","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Allocentric landmarks have an implicit influence on aiming movements, but it is not clear how an explicit instruction (to aim relative to a landmark) influences reach accuracy and precision. Here, 12 participants performed a task with two instruction conditions (egocentric vs. allocentric) but with similar sensory and motor conditions. Participants fixated gaze near the center of a display aligned with their right shoulder while a target stimulus briefly appeared alongside a visual landmark in one visual field. After a brief mask/memory delay the landmark then reappeared at a different location (same or opposite visual field), creating an ego/allocentric conflict. In the egocentric condition, participants were instructed to ignore the landmark and point toward the remembered location of the target. In the allocentric condition, participants were instructed to remember the initial target location relative to the landmark and then reach relative to the shifted landmark (same or opposite visual field). To equalize motor execution between tasks, participants were instructed to anti-point (point to the visual field opposite to the remembered target) on 50% of the egocentric trials. Participants were more accurate and precise and quicker to react in the allocentric condition, especially when pointing to the opposite field. We also observed a visual field effect, where performance was worse overall in the right visual field. These results suggest that, when egocentric and allocentric cues conflict, explicit use of the visual landmark provides better reach performance than reliance on noisy egocentric signals. Such instructions might aid rehabilitation when the egocentric system is compromised by disease or injury.</p>","PeriodicalId":49955,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Vision","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.0,"publicationDate":"2024-07-02","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11290568/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141789580","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Journal of Vision
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1