首页 > 最新文献

Journal of Vision最新文献

英文 中文
Salience maps for judgments of frontal plane distance, centroids, numerosity, and letter identity inferred from substance-invariant processing. 显著性图用于判断额平面距离、质心、数量和从物质不变处理推断的字母同一性。
IF 2 4区 心理学 Q2 OPHTHALMOLOGY Pub Date : 2025-01-02 DOI: 10.1167/jov.25.1.8
Lingyu Gan, George Sperling

A salience map is a topographic map that has inputs at each x,y location from many different feature maps and summarizes the combined salience of all those inputs as a real number, salience, which is represented in the map. Of the more than 1 million Google references to salience maps, nearly all use the map for computing the relative priority of visual image components for subsequent processing. We observe that salience processing is an instance of substance-invariant processing, analogous to household measuring cups, weight scales, and measuring tapes, all of which make single-number substance-invariant measurements. Like these devices, the brain also collects material for substance-invariant measurements but by a different mechanism: salience maps that collect visual substances for subsequent measurement. Each salience map can be used by many different measurements. The instruction to attend is implemented by increasing the salience of the to-be-attended items so they can be collected in a salience map and then further processed. Here we show that, beyond processing priority, the following measurement tasks are substance invariant and therefore use salience maps: computing distance in the frontal plane, computing centroids (center of a cluster of items), computing the numerosity of a collection of items, and identifying alphabetic letters. We painstakingly demonstrate that defining items exclusively by color or texture not only is sufficient for these tasks, but that light-dark luminance information significantly improves performance only for letter recognition. Obviously, visual features are represented in the brain but their salience alone is sufficient for these four judgments.

显著性地图是一种地形图,它在每个x、y位置都有来自许多不同特征地图的输入,并将所有这些输入的显著性组合总结为一个实数,即显著性,在地图中表示。在对显著性地图的100多万次引用中,几乎所有都使用该地图来计算视觉图像组件的相对优先级,以便进行后续处理。我们观察到,显著性处理是物质不变处理的一个实例,类似于家用量杯、体重秤和卷尺,所有这些都是单数字的物质不变测量。就像这些装置一样,大脑也为物质不变的测量收集材料,但通过一种不同的机制:显著性地图,为随后的测量收集视觉物质。每个显著性图可以用于许多不同的测量。参与指令是通过增加待参与项目的显著性来实现的,这样它们就可以被收集到显著性地图中,然后进一步处理。在这里,我们表明,除了处理优先级之外,以下测量任务是物质不变的,因此使用显著性图:计算额平面上的距离,计算质心(一组项目的中心),计算项目集合的数量,以及识别字母。我们煞费苦心地证明,仅通过颜色或纹理来定义项目不仅足以完成这些任务,而且明暗亮度信息仅在字母识别方面显着提高了性能。显然,视觉特征是在大脑中表现出来的,但它们的显著性本身就足以用于这四种判断。
{"title":"Salience maps for judgments of frontal plane distance, centroids, numerosity, and letter identity inferred from substance-invariant processing.","authors":"Lingyu Gan, George Sperling","doi":"10.1167/jov.25.1.8","DOIUrl":"10.1167/jov.25.1.8","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>A salience map is a topographic map that has inputs at each x,y location from many different feature maps and summarizes the combined salience of all those inputs as a real number, salience, which is represented in the map. Of the more than 1 million Google references to salience maps, nearly all use the map for computing the relative priority of visual image components for subsequent processing. We observe that salience processing is an instance of substance-invariant processing, analogous to household measuring cups, weight scales, and measuring tapes, all of which make single-number substance-invariant measurements. Like these devices, the brain also collects material for substance-invariant measurements but by a different mechanism: salience maps that collect visual substances for subsequent measurement. Each salience map can be used by many different measurements. The instruction to attend is implemented by increasing the salience of the to-be-attended items so they can be collected in a salience map and then further processed. Here we show that, beyond processing priority, the following measurement tasks are substance invariant and therefore use salience maps: computing distance in the frontal plane, computing centroids (center of a cluster of items), computing the numerosity of a collection of items, and identifying alphabetic letters. We painstakingly demonstrate that defining items exclusively by color or texture not only is sufficient for these tasks, but that light-dark luminance information significantly improves performance only for letter recognition. Obviously, visual features are represented in the brain but their salience alone is sufficient for these four judgments.</p>","PeriodicalId":49955,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Vision","volume":"25 1","pages":"8"},"PeriodicalIF":2.0,"publicationDate":"2025-01-02","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11724370/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142957988","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Monocular eye-cueing shifts eye balance in amblyopia. 单眼眼信号改变弱视的眼平衡。
IF 2 4区 心理学 Q2 OPHTHALMOLOGY Pub Date : 2025-01-02 DOI: 10.1167/jov.25.1.6
Sandy P Wong, Robert F Hess, Kathy T Mullen

Here, we investigate the shift in eye balance in response to monocular cueing in adults with amblyopia. In normally sighted adults, biasing attention toward one eye, by presenting a monocular visual stimulus to it, can shift eye balance toward the stimulated eye, as measured by binocular rivalry. We investigated whether we can modulate eye balance by directing monocular stimulation/attention in adults with clinical binocular deficits associated with amblyopia and larger eye imbalances. In a dual-task paradigm, eight participants continuously reported ongoing rivalry percepts and simultaneously performed a task related to the cueing stimulus. Time series of eye balance dynamics, aligned to cue onset, are averaged across trials and participants. In different time series, we tested the effect of monocular cueing on the amblyopic and fellow eyes (compared to a binocular control condition) and the effect of an active versus passive task. Overall, we found a significant shift in eye balance toward the monocularly cued eye, when both the fellow eye or the amblyopic eye were cued, F(2, 14) = 27.649, p < 0.01, ω2 = 0.590. This was independent of whether, during the binocular rivalry, the cue stimulus was presented to the perceiving eye or the non-perceiving eye. Performing an active task tended to produce a larger eye balance change, but this effect did not reach significance. Our results suggest that the eye imbalance in adults with binocular deficits, such as amblyopia, can be transiently reduced by monocularly directed stimulation, at least through activation of bottom-up attentional processes.

在此,我们研究了成人弱视患者单眼信号对眼平衡的影响。在视力正常的成年人中,通过向一只眼睛提供单眼视觉刺激,将注意力偏向一只眼睛,可以将眼睛平衡转移到受刺激的眼睛,这是通过双眼竞争来测量的。我们研究了是否可以通过引导单眼刺激/注意力来调节伴有弱视和大眼不平衡的双眼缺陷成人的眼平衡。在双任务范式中,8名参与者连续报告正在进行的竞争感知,同时执行与提示刺激相关的任务。眼平衡动态的时间序列,与线索开始一致,在试验和参与者之间平均。在不同的时间序列中,我们测试了单眼线索对弱视和其他眼睛的影响(与双眼对照条件相比)以及主动任务和被动任务的影响。总的来说,我们发现,当双眼或弱视眼同时被提示时,眼睛平衡向单眼倾斜,F(2,14) = 27.649, p < 0.01, ω2 = 0.590。这与在双眼竞争过程中,线索刺激是呈现给感知眼还是非感知眼无关。执行主动任务往往会产生较大的眼平衡变化,但这种影响没有达到显著性。我们的研究结果表明,有双眼缺陷的成年人,如弱视,可以通过单眼定向刺激,至少通过激活自下而上的注意力过程,暂时减少眼睛失衡。
{"title":"Monocular eye-cueing shifts eye balance in amblyopia.","authors":"Sandy P Wong, Robert F Hess, Kathy T Mullen","doi":"10.1167/jov.25.1.6","DOIUrl":"10.1167/jov.25.1.6","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Here, we investigate the shift in eye balance in response to monocular cueing in adults with amblyopia. In normally sighted adults, biasing attention toward one eye, by presenting a monocular visual stimulus to it, can shift eye balance toward the stimulated eye, as measured by binocular rivalry. We investigated whether we can modulate eye balance by directing monocular stimulation/attention in adults with clinical binocular deficits associated with amblyopia and larger eye imbalances. In a dual-task paradigm, eight participants continuously reported ongoing rivalry percepts and simultaneously performed a task related to the cueing stimulus. Time series of eye balance dynamics, aligned to cue onset, are averaged across trials and participants. In different time series, we tested the effect of monocular cueing on the amblyopic and fellow eyes (compared to a binocular control condition) and the effect of an active versus passive task. Overall, we found a significant shift in eye balance toward the monocularly cued eye, when both the fellow eye or the amblyopic eye were cued, F(2, 14) = 27.649, p < 0.01, ω2 = 0.590. This was independent of whether, during the binocular rivalry, the cue stimulus was presented to the perceiving eye or the non-perceiving eye. Performing an active task tended to produce a larger eye balance change, but this effect did not reach significance. Our results suggest that the eye imbalance in adults with binocular deficits, such as amblyopia, can be transiently reduced by monocularly directed stimulation, at least through activation of bottom-up attentional processes.</p>","PeriodicalId":49955,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Vision","volume":"25 1","pages":"6"},"PeriodicalIF":2.0,"publicationDate":"2025-01-02","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11724371/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142957986","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Visual information shows dominance in determining the magnitude of intentional binding for audiovisual outcomes. 视觉信息在决定视听结果的有意结合程度方面占主导地位。
IF 2 4区 心理学 Q2 OPHTHALMOLOGY Pub Date : 2025-01-02 DOI: 10.1167/jov.25.1.7
De-Wei Dai, Po-Jang Brown Hsieh

Intentional binding (IB) refers to the compression of subjective timing between a voluntary action and its outcome. In this study, we investigate the IB of a multimodal (audiovisual) outcome. We used a modified Libet clock while depicting a dynamic physical event (collision). Experiment 1 examined whether IB for the unimodal (auditory) event could be generalized to the multimodal (audiovisual) event, compared their magnitudes, and assessed whether the level of integration between modalities could affect IB. Planned contrasts (n = 42) showed significant IB effects for all types of events; the magnitude of IB was significantly weaker in both audiovisual integrated and audiovisual irrelevant conditions compared with auditory, with no difference between the integrated and irrelevant conditions. Experiment 2 separated the components of the audiovisual event to test the appropriate model describing the magnitude of IB in multimodal contexts. Planned contrasts (n = 42) showed the magnitude of IB was significantly weaker in both the audiovisual and visual conditions compared with the auditory condition, with no difference between the audiovisual and visual conditions. Additional Bayesian analysis provided moderate evidence supporting the equivalence between the two conditions. In conclusion, this study demonstrated that the IB phenomenon can be generalized to multimodal (audiovisual) sensory outcomes, and visual information shows dominance in determining the magnitude of IB for audiovisual events.

意向性约束(Intentional binding, IB)是指在自愿行为与其结果之间主观时间的压缩。在这项研究中,我们研究了多模态(视听)结果的IB。在描述动态物理事件(碰撞)时,我们使用了一个改进的Libet时钟。实验1检验了单模态(听觉)事件的IB是否可以推广到多模态(视听)事件,比较了它们的大小,并评估了模式之间的整合水平是否会影响IB。计划对比(n = 42)显示,所有类型的事件都有显著的IB效应;视听综合条件和视听不相关条件下的IB值均明显弱于听觉条件,视听综合条件和视听不相关条件间无差异。实验2分离了视听事件的组成部分,以测试描述多模态背景下IB大小的适当模型。计划对比(n = 42)显示,与听觉条件相比,视听和视觉条件下的IB强度均明显弱于听觉条件,视听和视觉条件之间无差异。额外的贝叶斯分析提供了适度的证据支持两个条件之间的等效性。总之,本研究表明,IB现象可以推广到多模态(视听)感觉结果,视觉信息在决定视听事件的IB程度方面占主导地位。
{"title":"Visual information shows dominance in determining the magnitude of intentional binding for audiovisual outcomes.","authors":"De-Wei Dai, Po-Jang Brown Hsieh","doi":"10.1167/jov.25.1.7","DOIUrl":"10.1167/jov.25.1.7","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Intentional binding (IB) refers to the compression of subjective timing between a voluntary action and its outcome. In this study, we investigate the IB of a multimodal (audiovisual) outcome. We used a modified Libet clock while depicting a dynamic physical event (collision). Experiment 1 examined whether IB for the unimodal (auditory) event could be generalized to the multimodal (audiovisual) event, compared their magnitudes, and assessed whether the level of integration between modalities could affect IB. Planned contrasts (n = 42) showed significant IB effects for all types of events; the magnitude of IB was significantly weaker in both audiovisual integrated and audiovisual irrelevant conditions compared with auditory, with no difference between the integrated and irrelevant conditions. Experiment 2 separated the components of the audiovisual event to test the appropriate model describing the magnitude of IB in multimodal contexts. Planned contrasts (n = 42) showed the magnitude of IB was significantly weaker in both the audiovisual and visual conditions compared with the auditory condition, with no difference between the audiovisual and visual conditions. Additional Bayesian analysis provided moderate evidence supporting the equivalence between the two conditions. In conclusion, this study demonstrated that the IB phenomenon can be generalized to multimodal (audiovisual) sensory outcomes, and visual information shows dominance in determining the magnitude of IB for audiovisual events.</p>","PeriodicalId":49955,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Vision","volume":"25 1","pages":"7"},"PeriodicalIF":2.0,"publicationDate":"2025-01-02","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11721482/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142957992","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Improving the reliability and accuracy of population receptive field measures using a logarithmically warped stimulus. 利用对数扭曲刺激提高群体接受野测量的可靠性和准确性。
IF 2 4区 心理学 Q2 OPHTHALMOLOGY Pub Date : 2025-01-02 DOI: 10.1167/jov.25.1.5
Kelly Chang, Ione Fine, Geoffrey M Boynton

The population receptive field (pRF) method, which measures the region in visual space that elicits a blood-oxygen-level-dependent (BOLD) signal in a voxel in retinotopic cortex, is a powerful tool for investigating the functional organization of human visual cortex with fMRI (Dumoulin & Wandell, 2008). However, recent work has shown that pRF estimates for early retinotopic visual areas can be biased and unreliable, especially for voxels representing the fovea. Here, we show that a log-bar stimulus that is logarithmically warped along the eccentricity dimension produces more reliable estimates of pRF size and location than the traditional moving bar stimulus. The log-bar stimulus was better able to identify pRFs near the foveal representation, and pRFs were smaller in size, consistent with simulation estimates of receptive field sizes in the fovea.

群体感受野(pRF)方法测量视觉空间中引发视网膜异位皮层体素中血氧水平依赖(BOLD)信号的区域,是用功能磁共振成像(fMRI)研究人类视觉皮层功能组织的有力工具(Dumoulin & Wandell, 2008)。然而,最近的研究表明,对于早期视网膜置换视觉区域的pRF估计可能存在偏差和不可靠,特别是对于代表中央凹的体素。在这里,我们证明了对数条刺激沿着偏心尺寸弯曲,比传统的移动条刺激产生更可靠的pRF大小和位置估计。对数条刺激能够更好地识别靠近中央凹表征的prf,并且prf的大小更小,与模拟估计的中央凹感受野大小一致。
{"title":"Improving the reliability and accuracy of population receptive field measures using a logarithmically warped stimulus.","authors":"Kelly Chang, Ione Fine, Geoffrey M Boynton","doi":"10.1167/jov.25.1.5","DOIUrl":"10.1167/jov.25.1.5","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The population receptive field (pRF) method, which measures the region in visual space that elicits a blood-oxygen-level-dependent (BOLD) signal in a voxel in retinotopic cortex, is a powerful tool for investigating the functional organization of human visual cortex with fMRI (Dumoulin & Wandell, 2008). However, recent work has shown that pRF estimates for early retinotopic visual areas can be biased and unreliable, especially for voxels representing the fovea. Here, we show that a log-bar stimulus that is logarithmically warped along the eccentricity dimension produces more reliable estimates of pRF size and location than the traditional moving bar stimulus. The log-bar stimulus was better able to identify pRFs near the foveal representation, and pRFs were smaller in size, consistent with simulation estimates of receptive field sizes in the fovea.</p>","PeriodicalId":49955,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Vision","volume":"25 1","pages":"5"},"PeriodicalIF":2.0,"publicationDate":"2025-01-02","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142923817","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Oculomotor challenges in macular degeneration impact motion extrapolation.
IF 2 4区 心理学 Q2 OPHTHALMOLOGY Pub Date : 2025-01-02 DOI: 10.1167/jov.25.1.17
Jason F Rubinstein, Noelia Gabriela Alcalde, Adrien Chopin, Preeti Verghese

Macular degeneration (MD), which affects the central visual field including the fovea, has a profound impact on acuity and oculomotor control. We used a motion extrapolation task to investigate the contribution of various factors that potentially impact motion estimation, including the transient disappearance of the target into the scotoma, increased position uncertainty associated with eccentric target positions, and increased oculomotor noise due to the use of a non-foveal locus for fixation and for eye movements. Observers performed a perceptual baseball task where they judged whether the target would intersect or miss a rectangular region (the plate). The target was extinguished before reaching the plate and participants were instructed either to fixate a marker or smoothly track the target before making the judgment. We tested nine eyes of six participants with MD and four control observers with simulated scotomata that matched those of individual participants with MD. Both groups used their habitual oculomotor locus-eccentric preferred retinal locus (PRL) for MD and fovea for controls. In the fixation condition, motion extrapolation was less accurate for controls with simulated scotomata than without, indicating that occlusion by the scotoma impacted the task. In both the fixation and pursuit conditions, MD participants with eccentric preferred retinal loci typically had worse motion extrapolation than controls with a matched artificial scotoma and foveal preferred retinal loci. Statistical analysis revealed occlusion and target eccentricity significantly impacted motion extrapolation in the pursuit condition, indicating that these factors make it challenging to estimate and track the path of a moving target in MD.

{"title":"Oculomotor challenges in macular degeneration impact motion extrapolation.","authors":"Jason F Rubinstein, Noelia Gabriela Alcalde, Adrien Chopin, Preeti Verghese","doi":"10.1167/jov.25.1.17","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1167/jov.25.1.17","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Macular degeneration (MD), which affects the central visual field including the fovea, has a profound impact on acuity and oculomotor control. We used a motion extrapolation task to investigate the contribution of various factors that potentially impact motion estimation, including the transient disappearance of the target into the scotoma, increased position uncertainty associated with eccentric target positions, and increased oculomotor noise due to the use of a non-foveal locus for fixation and for eye movements. Observers performed a perceptual baseball task where they judged whether the target would intersect or miss a rectangular region (the plate). The target was extinguished before reaching the plate and participants were instructed either to fixate a marker or smoothly track the target before making the judgment. We tested nine eyes of six participants with MD and four control observers with simulated scotomata that matched those of individual participants with MD. Both groups used their habitual oculomotor locus-eccentric preferred retinal locus (PRL) for MD and fovea for controls. In the fixation condition, motion extrapolation was less accurate for controls with simulated scotomata than without, indicating that occlusion by the scotoma impacted the task. In both the fixation and pursuit conditions, MD participants with eccentric preferred retinal loci typically had worse motion extrapolation than controls with a matched artificial scotoma and foveal preferred retinal loci. Statistical analysis revealed occlusion and target eccentricity significantly impacted motion extrapolation in the pursuit condition, indicating that these factors make it challenging to estimate and track the path of a moving target in MD.</p>","PeriodicalId":49955,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Vision","volume":"25 1","pages":"17"},"PeriodicalIF":2.0,"publicationDate":"2025-01-02","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"143061059","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Estimating the contribution of early and late noise in vision from psychophysical data. 从心理物理数据估计早期和晚期噪声对视觉的贡献。
IF 2 4区 心理学 Q2 OPHTHALMOLOGY Pub Date : 2025-01-02 DOI: 10.1167/jov.25.1.12
Jesús Malo, José Juan Esteve-Taboada, Guillermo Aguilar, Marianne Maertens, Felix A Wichmann

Human performance in psychophysical detection and discrimination tasks is limited by inner noise. It is unclear to what extent this inner noise arises from early noise (e.g., in the photoreceptors) or from late noise (at or immediately prior to the decision stage, presumably in cortex). Very likely, the behaviorally limiting inner noise is a nontrivial combination of both early and late noise. Here we propose a method to quantify the contributions of early and late noise purely from psychophysical data. Our approach generalizes classical results for linear systems by combining the theory of noise propagation through a nonlinear network with expressions to obtain a perceptual metric through a nonlinear network. We show that from threshold-only data, the relative contributions of early and late noise can only be disentangled when the experiments include substantial external noise. When full psychometric functions are available, early and late noise sources can be quantified even in the absence of external noise. Our psychophysical estimate of the magnitude of early noise-assuming a standard cascade of linear and nonlinear model stages-is substantially lower than the noise in cone photocurrents computed via an accurate model of retinal physiology, the ISETBio. This is consistent with the idea that one of the fundamental tasks of early vision is to reduce the comparatively large retinal noise.

人类在心理物理检测和识别任务中的表现受到内部噪声的限制。目前尚不清楚这种内部噪音在多大程度上来自早期噪音(例如,在光感受器中)或晚期噪音(在决策阶段或之前,可能在皮层中)。很有可能,限制行为的内在噪音是早期和晚期噪音的重要组合。在这里,我们提出了一种方法来量化早期和晚期噪声的贡献纯粹从心理物理数据。我们的方法将噪声通过非线性网络传播的理论与通过非线性网络获得感知度量的表达式相结合,从而推广了线性系统的经典结果。我们表明,仅从阈值数据来看,只有当实验包含大量外部噪声时,早期和晚期噪声的相对贡献才能解耦。当完整的心理测量功能可用时,即使在没有外部噪声的情况下,也可以量化早期和晚期噪声源。我们对早期噪声的心理物理估计——假设线性和非线性模型阶段的标准级联——大大低于通过精确的视网膜生理学模型ISETBio计算出的锥体光电流中的噪声。这与早期视力的基本任务之一是减少相对较大的视网膜噪声的观点是一致的。
{"title":"Estimating the contribution of early and late noise in vision from psychophysical data.","authors":"Jesús Malo, José Juan Esteve-Taboada, Guillermo Aguilar, Marianne Maertens, Felix A Wichmann","doi":"10.1167/jov.25.1.12","DOIUrl":"10.1167/jov.25.1.12","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Human performance in psychophysical detection and discrimination tasks is limited by inner noise. It is unclear to what extent this inner noise arises from early noise (e.g., in the photoreceptors) or from late noise (at or immediately prior to the decision stage, presumably in cortex). Very likely, the behaviorally limiting inner noise is a nontrivial combination of both early and late noise. Here we propose a method to quantify the contributions of early and late noise purely from psychophysical data. Our approach generalizes classical results for linear systems by combining the theory of noise propagation through a nonlinear network with expressions to obtain a perceptual metric through a nonlinear network. We show that from threshold-only data, the relative contributions of early and late noise can only be disentangled when the experiments include substantial external noise. When full psychometric functions are available, early and late noise sources can be quantified even in the absence of external noise. Our psychophysical estimate of the magnitude of early noise-assuming a standard cascade of linear and nonlinear model stages-is substantially lower than the noise in cone photocurrents computed via an accurate model of retinal physiology, the ISETBio. This is consistent with the idea that one of the fundamental tasks of early vision is to reduce the comparatively large retinal noise.</p>","PeriodicalId":49955,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Vision","volume":"25 1","pages":"12"},"PeriodicalIF":2.0,"publicationDate":"2025-01-02","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11758886/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142973014","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
A comparative analysis of perceptual noise in lateral and depth motion: Evidence from eye tracking.
IF 2 4区 心理学 Q2 OPHTHALMOLOGY Pub Date : 2025-01-02 DOI: 10.1167/jov.25.1.15
Joan López-Moliner

The characterization of how precisely we perceive visual speed has traditionally relied on psychophysical judgments in discrimination tasks. Such tasks are often considered laborious and susceptible to biases, particularly without the involvement of highly trained participants. Additionally, thresholds for motion-in-depth perception are frequently reported as higher compared to lateral motion, a discrepancy that contrasts with everyday visuomotor tasks. In this research, we rely on a smooth pursuit model, based on a Kalman filter, to quantify speed observational uncertainties. This model allows us to distinguish between additive and multiplicative noise across three conditions of motion dynamics within a virtual reality setting: random walk, linear motion, and nonlinear motion, incorporating both lateral and depth motion components. We aim to assess tracking performance and perceptual uncertainties for lateral versus motion-in-depth. In alignment with prior research, our results indicate diminished performance for depth motion in the random walk condition, characterized by unpredictable positioning. However, when velocity information is available and facilitates predictions of future positions, perceptual uncertainties become more consistent between lateral and in-depth motion. This consistency is particularly noticeable within ranges where retinal speeds overlap between these two dimensions. Significantly, additive noise emerges as the primary source of uncertainty, largely exceeding multiplicative noise. This predominance of additive noise is consistent with computational accounts of visual motion. Our study challenges earlier beliefs of marked differences in processing lateral versus in-depth motions, suggesting similar levels of perceptual uncertainty and underscoring the significant role of additive noise.

{"title":"A comparative analysis of perceptual noise in lateral and depth motion: Evidence from eye tracking.","authors":"Joan López-Moliner","doi":"10.1167/jov.25.1.15","DOIUrl":"10.1167/jov.25.1.15","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The characterization of how precisely we perceive visual speed has traditionally relied on psychophysical judgments in discrimination tasks. Such tasks are often considered laborious and susceptible to biases, particularly without the involvement of highly trained participants. Additionally, thresholds for motion-in-depth perception are frequently reported as higher compared to lateral motion, a discrepancy that contrasts with everyday visuomotor tasks. In this research, we rely on a smooth pursuit model, based on a Kalman filter, to quantify speed observational uncertainties. This model allows us to distinguish between additive and multiplicative noise across three conditions of motion dynamics within a virtual reality setting: random walk, linear motion, and nonlinear motion, incorporating both lateral and depth motion components. We aim to assess tracking performance and perceptual uncertainties for lateral versus motion-in-depth. In alignment with prior research, our results indicate diminished performance for depth motion in the random walk condition, characterized by unpredictable positioning. However, when velocity information is available and facilitates predictions of future positions, perceptual uncertainties become more consistent between lateral and in-depth motion. This consistency is particularly noticeable within ranges where retinal speeds overlap between these two dimensions. Significantly, additive noise emerges as the primary source of uncertainty, largely exceeding multiplicative noise. This predominance of additive noise is consistent with computational accounts of visual motion. Our study challenges earlier beliefs of marked differences in processing lateral versus in-depth motions, suggesting similar levels of perceptual uncertainty and underscoring the significant role of additive noise.</p>","PeriodicalId":49955,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Vision","volume":"25 1","pages":"15"},"PeriodicalIF":2.0,"publicationDate":"2025-01-02","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11761139/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"143034744","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Serial dependencies in motor targeting as a function of target appearance. 运动瞄准中靶标外观的序列依赖性。
IF 2 4区 心理学 Q2 OPHTHALMOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-12-02 DOI: 10.1167/jov.24.13.6
Sandra Tyralla, Eckart Zimmermann

In order to bring stimuli of interest into our central field of vision, we perform saccadic eye movements. After every saccade, the error between the predicted and actual landing position is monitored. In the laboratory, artificial post-saccadic errors are created by displacing the target during saccade execution. Previous research found that even a single post-saccadic error induces immediate amplitude changes to minimize that error. The saccadic amplitude adjustment could result from a recalibration of the saccade target representation. We asked if recalibration follows an integration scheme in which the impact magnitude of the previous post-saccadic target location depends on the certainty of the current target. We asked subjects to perform saccades to Gaussian blobs as targets, the visuospatial certainty of which we manipulated by changing its spatial constant. In separate sessions, either the pre-saccadic or post-saccadic target was uncertain. Additionally, we manipulated the contrast to further decrease certainty, changing the spatial constant mid-saccade. We found saccade-by-saccade amplitude reductions only with a currently uncertain target, a previously certain one, and a constant target contrast. We conclude that the features of the pre-saccadic target (i.e., size and contrast) determine the extent to which post-saccadic error shapes upcoming saccade amplitudes.

为了将感兴趣的刺激带到我们的中央视野,我们进行跳眼运动。在每次跳瞄后,预测和实际着陆位置之间的误差被监测。在实验室中,通过在扫视执行过程中移动目标来制造人工的扫视后误差。先前的研究发现,即使是一个单一的跳后误差也会引起立即的振幅变化,以最小化该误差。跳眼振幅的调整可以通过对跳眼目标表示的重新校准来实现。我们询问重新校准是否遵循一种整合方案,在该方案中,先前的后跳眼目标位置的影响程度取决于当前目标的确定性。我们要求受试者对高斯斑点进行扫视,我们通过改变其空间常数来操纵其视觉空间确定性。在单独的会议中,无论是跳眼前目标还是跳眼后目标都是不确定的。此外,我们还对对比度进行了操作,以进一步降低确定性,改变了眼动中期的空间常数。我们发现,只有在当前不确定的目标、先前确定的目标和恒定的目标对比度下,眼跳逐眼跳幅度才会降低。我们得出结论,跳眼前目标的特征(即大小和对比度)决定了跳眼后误差对即将到来的跳眼振幅的影响程度。
{"title":"Serial dependencies in motor targeting as a function of target appearance.","authors":"Sandra Tyralla, Eckart Zimmermann","doi":"10.1167/jov.24.13.6","DOIUrl":"10.1167/jov.24.13.6","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>In order to bring stimuli of interest into our central field of vision, we perform saccadic eye movements. After every saccade, the error between the predicted and actual landing position is monitored. In the laboratory, artificial post-saccadic errors are created by displacing the target during saccade execution. Previous research found that even a single post-saccadic error induces immediate amplitude changes to minimize that error. The saccadic amplitude adjustment could result from a recalibration of the saccade target representation. We asked if recalibration follows an integration scheme in which the impact magnitude of the previous post-saccadic target location depends on the certainty of the current target. We asked subjects to perform saccades to Gaussian blobs as targets, the visuospatial certainty of which we manipulated by changing its spatial constant. In separate sessions, either the pre-saccadic or post-saccadic target was uncertain. Additionally, we manipulated the contrast to further decrease certainty, changing the spatial constant mid-saccade. We found saccade-by-saccade amplitude reductions only with a currently uncertain target, a previously certain one, and a constant target contrast. We conclude that the features of the pre-saccadic target (i.e., size and contrast) determine the extent to which post-saccadic error shapes upcoming saccade amplitudes.</p>","PeriodicalId":49955,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Vision","volume":"24 13","pages":"6"},"PeriodicalIF":2.0,"publicationDate":"2024-12-02","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11629911/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142787496","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Preferred fixation position and gaze location: Two factors modulating the composite face effect. 偏好注视位置和注视位置:调节复合面部效应的两个因素。
IF 2 4区 心理学 Q2 OPHTHALMOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-12-02 DOI: 10.1167/jov.24.13.15
Puneeth N Chakravarthula, Ansh K Soni, Miguel P Eckstein

Humans consistently land their first saccade to a face at a preferred fixation location (PFL). Humans also typically process faces as wholes, as evidenced by perceptual effects such as the composite face effect (CFE). However, not known is whether an individual's tendency to process faces as wholes varies with their gaze patterns on the face. Here, we investigated variation of the CFE with the PFL. We compared the strength of the CFE for two groups of observers who were screened to have their PFLs either higher up, closer to the eyes, or lower on the face, closer to the tip of the nose. During the task, observers maintained their gaze at either their own group's mean PFL or at the other group's mean PFL. We found that the top half of the face elicits a stronger CFE than the bottom half. Further, the strength of the CFE was modulated by the distance of the PFL from the eyes, such that individuals with a PFL closer to the eyes had a stronger CFE than those with a PFL closer to the mouth. Finally, the top-half CFE for both upper-lookers and lower-lookers was abolished when they fixated at a non-preferred location on the face. Our findings show that the CFE relies on internal face representations shaped by the long-term use of a consistent oculomotor strategy to view faces.

人类总是在首选注视位置(PFL)上进行第一次扫视。人类通常也会将人脸作为一个整体来处理,如复合人脸效应(CFE)等感知效应就证明了这一点。然而,目前尚不清楚的是,一个人对面部的整体处理倾向是否会随着他们对面部的注视模式而变化。在这里,我们研究了CFE随PFL的变化。我们比较了两组被筛选的观察者的CFE强度,他们的PFLs要么更高,更靠近眼睛,要么更低,更靠近鼻尖。在任务中,观察者要么盯着自己组的平均PFL,要么盯着另一组的平均PFL。我们发现脸的上半部分比下半部分产生更强的CFE。此外,CFE的强度受到PFL与眼睛的距离的调节,因此PFL靠近眼睛的个体比PFL靠近嘴巴的个体有更强的CFE。最后,当注视面部非首选位置时,上半部分的CFE都被取消。我们的研究结果表明,CFE依赖于长期使用一致的动眼力策略形成的内部面部表征来观察面部。
{"title":"Preferred fixation position and gaze location: Two factors modulating the composite face effect.","authors":"Puneeth N Chakravarthula, Ansh K Soni, Miguel P Eckstein","doi":"10.1167/jov.24.13.15","DOIUrl":"10.1167/jov.24.13.15","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Humans consistently land their first saccade to a face at a preferred fixation location (PFL). Humans also typically process faces as wholes, as evidenced by perceptual effects such as the composite face effect (CFE). However, not known is whether an individual's tendency to process faces as wholes varies with their gaze patterns on the face. Here, we investigated variation of the CFE with the PFL. We compared the strength of the CFE for two groups of observers who were screened to have their PFLs either higher up, closer to the eyes, or lower on the face, closer to the tip of the nose. During the task, observers maintained their gaze at either their own group's mean PFL or at the other group's mean PFL. We found that the top half of the face elicits a stronger CFE than the bottom half. Further, the strength of the CFE was modulated by the distance of the PFL from the eyes, such that individuals with a PFL closer to the eyes had a stronger CFE than those with a PFL closer to the mouth. Finally, the top-half CFE for both upper-lookers and lower-lookers was abolished when they fixated at a non-preferred location on the face. Our findings show that the CFE relies on internal face representations shaped by the long-term use of a consistent oculomotor strategy to view faces.</p>","PeriodicalId":49955,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Vision","volume":"24 13","pages":"15"},"PeriodicalIF":2.0,"publicationDate":"2024-12-02","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11681917/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142899810","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Reviewers. 评论家。
IF 2 4区 心理学 Q2 OPHTHALMOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-12-02 DOI: 10.1167/jov.24.13.16
{"title":"Reviewers.","authors":"","doi":"10.1167/jov.24.13.16","DOIUrl":"10.1167/jov.24.13.16","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":49955,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Vision","volume":"24 13","pages":"16"},"PeriodicalIF":2.0,"publicationDate":"2024-12-02","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11684487/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142957898","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Journal of Vision
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1