首页 > 最新文献

Journal of Experimental Psychology-Human Perception and Performance最新文献

英文 中文
Competition between parts and whole: A new approach to Chinese compound word processing. 部分与整体的竞争:中文复合词处理的新方法。
IF 2.1 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-05-01 Epub Date: 2024-03-28 DOI: 10.1037/xhp0001198
Qiwei Zhang, Kuan-Jung Huang, Xingshan Li

How compound words are processed remains a central question in research on Chinese reading. The Chinese reading model assumes that all possible words sharing characters are activated during word processing and these activated words compete for a winner (Li & Pollatsek, 2020). The present studies aimed to examine whether embedded component words compete with whole compound words in Chinese reading. In Study 1, we analyzed two existing lexical decision databases and revealed inhibitory effects of component-word frequency and facilitative effects of character frequency on the first components. In Study 2, we conducted two factorial experiments to further examine the effects of first component-word frequency, with character frequencies controlled. The results consistently indicated significant inhibitory effects of component-word frequency. Collectively, these findings support the theoretical proposition that both component words and compound words are activated and engage in competition during word processing. This provides a new approach to compound word processing in Chinese reading and a possible solution to mixed results of character frequency effects reported in the literature. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

如何处理复合词仍然是中文阅读研究的核心问题。汉语阅读模型假定,在词语处理过程中,所有可能的共用字词都会被激活,这些被激活的词会在竞争中胜出(Li & Pollatsek, 2020)。本研究旨在探讨在中文阅读中,嵌入成分词是否会与整个复合词竞争。在研究 1 中,我们分析了两个现有的词汇决策数据库,发现成分词频率对第一成分词有抑制作用,而字符频率对第一成分词有促进作用。在研究 2 中,我们进行了两个因子实验,在控制字频的情况下进一步考察了第一成分-词频的影响。结果一致表明,成分-词频具有明显的抑制作用。总之,这些研究结果支持了这样一个理论命题,即在单词处理过程中,成分词和复合词都会被激活并参与竞争。这为中文阅读中的复合词处理提供了一种新的方法,也为文献中报道的字频效应的混合结果提供了一种可能的解决方案。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved)。
{"title":"Competition between parts and whole: A new approach to Chinese compound word processing.","authors":"Qiwei Zhang, Kuan-Jung Huang, Xingshan Li","doi":"10.1037/xhp0001198","DOIUrl":"10.1037/xhp0001198","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>How compound words are processed remains a central question in research on Chinese reading. The Chinese reading model assumes that all possible words sharing characters are activated during word processing and these activated words compete for a winner (Li & Pollatsek, 2020). The present studies aimed to examine whether embedded component words compete with whole compound words in Chinese reading. In Study 1, we analyzed two existing lexical decision databases and revealed inhibitory effects of component-word frequency and facilitative effects of character frequency on the first components. In Study 2, we conducted two factorial experiments to further examine the effects of first component-word frequency, with character frequencies controlled. The results consistently indicated significant inhibitory effects of component-word frequency. Collectively, these findings support the theoretical proposition that both component words and compound words are activated and engage in competition during word processing. This provides a new approach to compound word processing in Chinese reading and a possible solution to mixed results of character frequency effects reported in the literature. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":50195,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Human Perception and Performance","volume":" ","pages":"479-497"},"PeriodicalIF":2.1,"publicationDate":"2024-05-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140307674","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
On the timing of overt attention deployment: Eye-movement evidence for the priority accumulation framework. 关于公开注意力部署的时机:优先权积累框架的眼动证据。
IF 2.1 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-05-01 Epub Date: 2024-02-29 DOI: 10.1037/xhp0001192
Daniel Toledano, Mor Sasi, Shlomit Yuval-Greenberg, Dominique Lamy

Most visual-search theories assume that our attention is automatically allocated to the location with the highest priority at any given moment. The Priority Accumulation Framework (PAF) challenges this assumption. It suggests that the priority weight at each location accumulates across sequential events and that evidence for the presence of action-relevant information contributes to determining when attention is deployed to the location with the highest accumulated priority. Here, we tested these hypotheses for overt attention by recording first saccades in a free-viewing spatial-cueing task. We manipulated search difficulty (Experiments 1 and 2) and cue salience (Experiment 2). Standard theories posit that when oculomotor capture by the cue occurs, it is initiated before the search display appears; therefore, these theories predict that the cue's impact on the distribution of first saccades should be independent of search difficulty but influenced by the cue's saliency. By contrast, PAF posits that the cue can bias competition later, after processing of the search display has already started, and therefore predicts that such late impact should increase with both search difficulty and cue salience. The results fully supported PAF's predictions. Our account suggests a distinction between attentional capture and attentional-priority bias that resolves enduring inconsistencies in the attentional-capture literature. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

大多数视觉搜索理论都认为,我们的注意力会在任何特定时刻自动分配到优先级最高的位置。优先权累积框架(PAF)挑战了这一假设。它认为,每个位置的优先权重会在连续事件中不断累积,而行动相关信息的存在有助于确定何时将注意力分配到优先权累积最高的位置。在这里,我们通过记录自由观察空间线索任务中的首次囊回来测试这些关于公开注意的假设。我们操纵了搜索难度(实验 1 和 2)和线索显著性(实验 2)。标准理论认为,当眼球运动被线索捕捉时,它是在搜索显示出现之前开始的;因此,这些理论预测线索对首次注视分布的影响应与搜索难度无关,但受线索显著性的影响。与此相反,PAF 则认为线索会在搜索显示的处理过程开始之后对竞争产生影响,因此预测这种晚期影响会随着搜索难度和线索显著性的增加而增加。结果完全支持 PAF 的预测。我们的解释提出了注意捕捉和注意优先偏差之间的区别,解决了注意捕捉文献中长期存在的不一致问题。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"On the timing of overt attention deployment: Eye-movement evidence for the priority accumulation framework.","authors":"Daniel Toledano, Mor Sasi, Shlomit Yuval-Greenberg, Dominique Lamy","doi":"10.1037/xhp0001192","DOIUrl":"10.1037/xhp0001192","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Most visual-search theories assume that our attention is automatically allocated to the location with the highest priority at any given moment. The Priority Accumulation Framework (PAF) challenges this assumption. It suggests that the priority weight at each location accumulates across sequential events and that evidence for the presence of action-relevant information contributes to determining when attention is deployed to the location with the highest accumulated priority. Here, we tested these hypotheses for overt attention by recording first saccades in a free-viewing spatial-cueing task. We manipulated search difficulty (Experiments 1 and 2) and cue salience (Experiment 2). Standard theories posit that when oculomotor capture by the cue occurs, it is initiated before the search display appears; therefore, these theories predict that the cue's impact on the distribution of first saccades should be independent of search difficulty but influenced by the cue's saliency. By contrast, PAF posits that the cue can bias competition later, after processing of the search display has already started, and therefore predicts that such late impact should increase with both search difficulty and cue salience. The results fully supported PAF's predictions. Our account suggests a distinction between attentional capture and attentional-priority bias that resolves enduring inconsistencies in the attentional-capture literature. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":50195,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Human Perception and Performance","volume":" ","pages":"431-450"},"PeriodicalIF":2.1,"publicationDate":"2024-05-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139998068","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
A discrete component in visual working memory encoding. 视觉工作记忆编码中的离散成分
IF 2.1 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-05-01 Epub Date: 2024-02-29 DOI: 10.1037/xhp0001196
Hyung-Bum Park, Weiwei Zhang

Working memory (WM) is a central cognitive bottleneck, which has primarily been attributed to its well-known storage limit. However, relatively little is known about the processing limit during the initial memory encoding stage, which may also constrain various cognitive processes. The present study introduces a novel method using dynamic stimulus presentation and hierarchical Bayesian modeling to quantitatively estimate visual WM encoding speed. Participants performed a delayed-estimation task with two memory items continuously changing color hues in perceptually unnoticeable steps. Across three experiments, the recall errors systematically shifted toward the direction of color change, providing a proxy measure of encoding speed. Importantly, the observed shifts were best characterized by a temporal lag during the encoding of different items, supported by a mixture of two distributions with credibly distinct encoding times. A supplementary model-free analysis further confirmed the discrete encoding component in visual WM for multiple items. These findings shed light on the temporal dynamics of WM encoding processes. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

工作记忆(WM)是认知的核心瓶颈,其主要原因是众所周知的存储极限。然而,人们对初始记忆编码阶段的处理限制知之甚少,而这一限制也可能制约各种认知过程。本研究介绍了一种使用动态刺激呈现和分层贝叶斯建模来定量估计视觉 WM 编码速度的新方法。受试者进行了一项延迟估计任务,两个记忆项目以知觉上不易察觉的步骤不断改变颜色色调。在三次实验中,回忆错误系统地向颜色变化的方向移动,从而提供了编码速度的替代测量。重要的是,所观察到的偏移的最佳特征是不同项目编码过程中的时间滞后,这得到了编码时间截然不同的两种分布的混合支持。一项无模型补充分析进一步证实了视觉 WM 中多个项目的离散编码部分。这些发现揭示了 WM 编码过程的时间动态。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"A discrete component in visual working memory encoding.","authors":"Hyung-Bum Park, Weiwei Zhang","doi":"10.1037/xhp0001196","DOIUrl":"10.1037/xhp0001196","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Working memory (WM) is a central cognitive bottleneck, which has primarily been attributed to its well-known storage limit. However, relatively little is known about the processing limit during the initial memory encoding stage, which may also constrain various cognitive processes. The present study introduces a novel method using dynamic stimulus presentation and hierarchical Bayesian modeling to quantitatively estimate visual WM encoding speed. Participants performed a delayed-estimation task with two memory items continuously changing color hues in perceptually unnoticeable steps. Across three experiments, the recall errors systematically shifted toward the direction of color change, providing a proxy measure of encoding speed. Importantly, the observed shifts were best characterized by a temporal lag during the encoding of different items, supported by a mixture of two distributions with credibly distinct encoding times. A supplementary model-free analysis further confirmed the discrete encoding component in visual WM for multiple items. These findings shed light on the temporal dynamics of WM encoding processes. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":50195,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Human Perception and Performance","volume":" ","pages":"464-478"},"PeriodicalIF":2.1,"publicationDate":"2024-05-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11850077/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139998065","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Use one system for all results to avoid contradiction: Advice for using significance tests, equivalence tests, and Bayes factors. 对所有结果使用一个系统,以避免矛盾:使用显著性检验、等效性检验和贝叶斯系数的建议。
IF 2.1 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-05-01 DOI: 10.1037/xhp0001202
Zoltan Dienes

A nonsignificant result against an H0 of no effect does not distinguish evidence for no effect from no evidence at all one way or the other. Thus, a researcher engaged primarily in significance testing may decide to follow up just the nonsignificant results with a test from another system of inference, such as equivalence tests (more generally, inference by intervals) or Bayes factors. However, selectively using two systems of inference in this way, can lead to inferential inconsistency because different tests are based on different principles, and therefore a researcher can be tempted to select the way each system is used to get the results the researcher wants for just the tests that system is applied to. For a related set of tests, one system of inference should be consistently used. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

针对 "无效应 H0 "的非显著性结果并不能以某种方式区分 "无效应 "和 "无证据"。因此,主要从事显著性检验的研究人员可能会决定仅对不显著的结果进行后续检验,而采用另一种推理系统,如等效检验(更广泛地说,区间推理)或贝叶斯因子。然而,以这种方式有选择地使用两种推论系统,可能会导致推论的不一致性,因为不同的检验基于不同的原理,因此,研究人员可能会倾向于选择使用每种系统的方式,以便仅在应用该系统的检验中获得研究人员想要的结果。对于一组相关的测试,应始终使用一种推论系统。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Use one system for all results to avoid contradiction: Advice for using significance tests, equivalence tests, and Bayes factors.","authors":"Zoltan Dienes","doi":"10.1037/xhp0001202","DOIUrl":"10.1037/xhp0001202","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>A nonsignificant result against an H0 of no effect does not distinguish evidence for no effect from no evidence at all one way or the other. Thus, a researcher engaged primarily in significance testing may decide to follow up just the nonsignificant results with a test from another system of inference, such as equivalence tests (more generally, inference by intervals) or Bayes factors. However, selectively using two systems of inference in this way, can lead to inferential inconsistency because different tests are based on different principles, and therefore a researcher can be tempted to select the way each system is used to get the results the researcher wants for just the tests that system is applied to. For a related set of tests, one system of inference should be consistently used. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":50195,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Human Perception and Performance","volume":"50 5","pages":"531-534"},"PeriodicalIF":2.1,"publicationDate":"2024-05-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140867248","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Do accent and input modality modulate processing of language switches in bilingual language comprehension? 口音和输入模式会调节双语语言理解中的语言转换处理吗?
IF 2.1 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-04-01 Epub Date: 2024-02-22 DOI: 10.1037/xhp0001190
Marion Coumel, Cong Liu, Danijela Trenkic, Angela de Bruin

We examined how bilinguals process language switches between their first (L1) and second language (L2). Language switching costs (slower responses to language switch than nonswitch trials) appear to arise more systematically in production than in comprehension, possibly because the latter context might sometimes elicit less language coactivation (Declerck et al., 2019). This might reduce language competition and in turn the need for bilinguals to apply language control when processing language switches. Yet even in comprehension, language coactivation may vary depending on variables such as the accent of the speaker (e.g., whether the L2 words are pronounced with an L1 or L2 accent) and input modality (spoken or written). In three experiments conducted during 2021-2022, we tested how unbalanced Mandarin-English bilinguals processed language switches during comprehension and the potential influence of a speaker's accent and input modality. Overall, across settings, participants experienced significant language switching costs. In some conditions, switching costs were larger to L1-Mandarin than to L2-English, an asymmetry consistent with the participants' dominance in L1-Mandarin and the application of language control. However, manipulating accent and input modality did not influence language switches, suggesting they did not impact language coactivation sufficiently to modulate language control. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

我们研究了双语者如何处理第一语言(L1)和第二语言(L2)之间的语言转换。语言转换成本(对语言转换的反应慢于非转换试验)似乎在生产中比在理解中更系统地出现,这可能是因为后一种语境有时可能引起较少的语言共激活(Declerck 等人,2019)。这可能会减少语言竞争,进而减少二语者在处理语言转换时应用语言控制的需要。然而,即使在理解过程中,语言共激活也会因说话者的口音(例如,L2单词的发音是L1口音还是L2口音)和输入模式(口语还是书面语)等变量的不同而变化。在 2021-2022 年期间进行的三项实验中,我们测试了不平衡的普通话-英语二语者在理解过程中如何处理语言转换,以及说话者的口音和输入模式的潜在影响。总体而言,在各种环境下,参与者都经历了显著的语言转换成本。在某些情况下,L1-普通话的转换成本大于 L2-英语的转换成本,这种不对称性与受试者的 L1-普通话优势和语言控制的应用相一致。然而,对口音和输入模式的操作并不影响语言转换,这表明它们对语言共激活的影响不足以调节语言控制。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Do accent and input modality modulate processing of language switches in bilingual language comprehension?","authors":"Marion Coumel, Cong Liu, Danijela Trenkic, Angela de Bruin","doi":"10.1037/xhp0001190","DOIUrl":"10.1037/xhp0001190","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>We examined how bilinguals process language switches between their first (L1) and second language (L2). Language switching costs (slower responses to language switch than nonswitch trials) appear to arise more systematically in production than in comprehension, possibly because the latter context might sometimes elicit less language coactivation (Declerck et al., 2019). This might reduce language competition and in turn the need for bilinguals to apply language control when processing language switches. Yet even in comprehension, language coactivation may vary depending on variables such as the accent of the speaker (e.g., whether the L2 words are pronounced with an L1 or L2 accent) and input modality (spoken or written). In three experiments conducted during 2021-2022, we tested how unbalanced Mandarin-English bilinguals processed language switches during comprehension and the potential influence of a speaker's accent and input modality. Overall, across settings, participants experienced significant language switching costs. In some conditions, switching costs were larger to L1-Mandarin than to L2-English, an asymmetry consistent with the participants' dominance in L1-Mandarin and the application of language control. However, manipulating accent and input modality did not influence language switches, suggesting they did not impact language coactivation sufficiently to modulate language control. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":50195,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Human Perception and Performance","volume":" ","pages":"395-415"},"PeriodicalIF":2.1,"publicationDate":"2024-04-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139933891","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Exploring individual differences in native phonetic perception and their link to nonnative phonetic perception. 探索母语语音感知的个体差异及其与非母语语音感知的联系。
IF 2.1 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-04-01 Epub Date: 2024-02-01 DOI: 10.1037/xhp0001191
Claire T Honda, Meghan Clayards, Shari R Baum

Adults differ considerably in their perception of both native and nonnative phonemes. For instance, when presented with continua of native phonemes on two-alternative forced choice (2AFC) or visual analog scaling (VAS) tasks, some people show sudden changes in responses (i.e., steep identification slopes) and others show gradual changes (i.e., shallow identification slopes). Moreover, some adults are more successful than others at learning unfamiliar phonemes. The predictors of these individual differences and the relationships between them are poorly understood. It also remains unclear to what extent different tasks (2AFC vs. VAS) may reflect distinct individual differences in perception. In two experiments, we addressed these questions by examining the relationships between individual differences in performance on native and nonnative phonetic perception tasks. We found that shallow 2AFC identification slopes were not related to shallow VAS identification slopes but were related to inconsistent VAS responses. Additionally, our results suggest that consistent native perception may play a role in promoting successful nonnative perception. These findings help characterize the nature of individual differences in phonetic perception and contribute to our understanding of how to measure such differences. This work also has implications for encouraging successful acquisition of new languages in adulthood. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

成年人对母语和非母语音素的感知存在很大差异。例如,当在两选一强迫选择(2AFC)或视觉模拟标度(VAS)任务中出现连续的母语音素时,有些人的反应会突然发生变化(即陡峭的识别斜坡),而有些人的反应则会逐渐发生变化(即较浅的识别斜坡)。此外,一些成年人在学习陌生音素时比其他人更成功。对于这些个体差异的预测因素以及它们之间的关系,人们还知之甚少。此外,不同的任务(2AFC 与 VAS)在多大程度上反映了感知方面的个体差异也仍不清楚。在两个实验中,我们通过研究母语和非母语语音感知任务中个体差异之间的关系来解决这些问题。我们发现,浅 2AFC 识别斜率与浅 VAS 识别斜率无关,但与不一致的 VAS 反应有关。此外,我们的研究结果表明,一致的母语感知可能对成功的非母语感知起到促进作用。这些发现有助于描述语音感知个体差异的性质,并有助于我们理解如何测量这种差异。这项研究还对鼓励人们在成年后成功学习新语言具有重要意义。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Exploring individual differences in native phonetic perception and their link to nonnative phonetic perception.","authors":"Claire T Honda, Meghan Clayards, Shari R Baum","doi":"10.1037/xhp0001191","DOIUrl":"10.1037/xhp0001191","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Adults differ considerably in their perception of both native and nonnative phonemes. For instance, when presented with continua of native phonemes on two-alternative forced choice (2AFC) or visual analog scaling (VAS) tasks, some people show sudden changes in responses (i.e., steep identification slopes) and others show gradual changes (i.e., shallow identification slopes). Moreover, some adults are more successful than others at learning unfamiliar phonemes. The predictors of these individual differences and the relationships between them are poorly understood. It also remains unclear to what extent different tasks (2AFC vs. VAS) may reflect distinct individual differences in perception. In two experiments, we addressed these questions by examining the relationships between individual differences in performance on native and nonnative phonetic perception tasks. We found that shallow 2AFC identification slopes were not related to shallow VAS identification slopes but were related to inconsistent VAS responses. Additionally, our results suggest that consistent native perception may play a role in promoting successful nonnative perception. These findings help characterize the nature of individual differences in phonetic perception and contribute to our understanding of how to measure such differences. This work also has implications for encouraging successful acquisition of new languages in adulthood. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":50195,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Human Perception and Performance","volume":" ","pages":"370-394"},"PeriodicalIF":2.1,"publicationDate":"2024-04-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139652066","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Moving stimuli enhance beat timing and sensorimotor coupling in vision. 移动刺激增强了视觉中的节拍计时和感觉运动耦合。
IF 2.1 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-04-01 Epub Date: 2024-02-29 DOI: 10.1037/xhp0001193
Liang Zhou, Lianzi Xing, Chenhao Zheng, Shouxin Li

Vision has long been known for its inefficiency in beat perception and synchronization. However, this has been challenged by the finding that moving stimuli (bouncing ball or moving bar) can significantly improve visual beat synchronization. The present study examined two possible mechanisms for this phenomenon: visual motion facilitates temporal processing or promotes sensorimotor coupling. Instead of a single visual object (such as a ball or bar), random-dot kinematograms (RDKs) were used to construct visual motion sequences to avoid confounding factors, such as changes in trajectory and velocity. Experiment 1 showed that RDKs improved beat-timing discrimination compared with visual flashes, but auditory tones were still superior to RDKs. In Experiment 2, synchronized movements improved auditory-tone beat timing but impaired visual-flash beat timing, with no effect on RDK beat timing. Experiment 3 indicated that the regression slope of the phase correction response in RDKs was higher than that in visual flashes but still lower than that in auditory tones. The results showed that moving stimuli enhances both temporal processing (Experiment 1) and sensorimotor coupling (Experiments 2 and 3) in vision, but to a lesser degree, with audition retaining an advantage. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

长期以来,人们一直认为视觉在节拍感知和同步方面效率低下。然而,移动刺激(弹跳球或移动棒)能显著提高视觉节拍同步性,这一发现对上述观点提出了挑战。本研究探讨了这一现象的两种可能机制:视觉运动促进了时间处理或促进了感觉运动耦合。为了避免轨迹和速度变化等干扰因素,我们使用随机点运动图(RDK)来构建视觉运动序列,而不是单一的视觉对象(如球或杆)。实验 1 显示,与视觉闪光相比,随机点运动图提高了节拍时间辨别能力,但听觉音调仍优于随机点运动图。在实验 2 中,同步运动改善了听觉音调节拍计时,但损害了视觉闪光节拍计时,对 RDK 节拍计时没有影响。实验 3 表明,RDK 的相位校正反应回归斜率高于视觉闪光,但仍低于听觉音调。结果表明,移动刺激增强了视觉的时间处理(实验 1)和感觉运动耦合(实验 2 和 3),但程度较低,听觉仍保持优势。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Moving stimuli enhance beat timing and sensorimotor coupling in vision.","authors":"Liang Zhou, Lianzi Xing, Chenhao Zheng, Shouxin Li","doi":"10.1037/xhp0001193","DOIUrl":"10.1037/xhp0001193","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Vision has long been known for its inefficiency in beat perception and synchronization. However, this has been challenged by the finding that moving stimuli (bouncing ball or moving bar) can significantly improve visual beat synchronization. The present study examined two possible mechanisms for this phenomenon: visual motion facilitates temporal processing or promotes sensorimotor coupling. Instead of a single visual object (such as a ball or bar), random-dot kinematograms (RDKs) were used to construct visual motion sequences to avoid confounding factors, such as changes in trajectory and velocity. Experiment 1 showed that RDKs improved beat-timing discrimination compared with visual flashes, but auditory tones were still superior to RDKs. In Experiment 2, synchronized movements improved auditory-tone beat timing but impaired visual-flash beat timing, with no effect on RDK beat timing. Experiment 3 indicated that the regression slope of the phase correction response in RDKs was higher than that in visual flashes but still lower than that in auditory tones. The results showed that moving stimuli enhances both temporal processing (Experiment 1) and sensorimotor coupling (Experiments 2 and 3) in vision, but to a lesser degree, with audition retaining an advantage. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":50195,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Human Perception and Performance","volume":" ","pages":"416-429"},"PeriodicalIF":2.1,"publicationDate":"2024-04-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139998067","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Social priming of speech perception: The role of individual differences in implicit racial and ethnic associations. 言语感知的社会诱导:个体差异在内隐性种族和民族联想中的作用。
IF 2.1 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-04-01 Epub Date: 2024-02-08 DOI: 10.1037/xhp0001187
Drew J McLaughlin, Kristin J Van Engen

Prior research has shown that visual information, such as a speaker's perceived race or ethnicity, prompts listeners to expect a specific sociophonetic pattern ("social priming"). Indeed, a picture of an East Asian face may facilitate perception of second language (L2) Mandarin Chinese-accented English but interfere with perception of first language- (L1-) accented English. The present study builds on this line of inquiry, addressing the relationship between social priming effects and implicit racial/ethnic associations for L1- and L2-accented speech. For L1-accented speech, we found no priming effects when comparing White versus East Asian or Latina primes. For L2- (Mandarin Chinese-) accented speech, however, transcription accuracy was slightly better following an East Asian prime than a White prime. Across all experiments, a relationship between performance and individual differences in implicit associations emerged, but in no cases did this relationship interact with the priming manipulation. Ultimately, exploring social priming effects with additional methodological approaches, and in different populations of listeners, will help to determine whether these effects operate differently in the context of L1- and L2-accented speech. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

先前的研究表明,视觉信息(如说话者的种族或民族)会促使听者期待特定的社会语音模式("社会引物")。事实上,一张东亚人面孔的图片可能会促进对第二语言(L2)汉语普通话口音英语的感知,但会干扰对第一语言(L1)口音英语的感知。本研究以这一研究思路为基础,探讨了 L1 和 L2 口音语音的社会引物效应与内隐种族/民族关联之间的关系。对于 L1 口音的语音,在比较白人与东亚人或拉丁裔人的引物时,我们没有发现引物效应。但是,对于 L2(中国普通话)重音语音,东亚语素比白人语素的转录准确率略高。在所有实验中,成绩与内隐联想的个体差异之间都存在着某种关系,但在任何情况下,这种关系都不会与引物操作产生交互作用。最终,通过更多的方法和在不同的听者群体中探索社会引物效应,将有助于确定这些效应在 L1- 和 L2- 口音语音中是否有不同的作用。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Social priming of speech perception: The role of individual differences in implicit racial and ethnic associations.","authors":"Drew J McLaughlin, Kristin J Van Engen","doi":"10.1037/xhp0001187","DOIUrl":"10.1037/xhp0001187","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Prior research has shown that visual information, such as a speaker's perceived race or ethnicity, prompts listeners to expect a specific sociophonetic pattern (\"social priming\"). Indeed, a picture of an East Asian face may facilitate perception of second language (L2) Mandarin Chinese-accented English but interfere with perception of first language- (L1-) accented English. The present study builds on this line of inquiry, addressing the relationship between social priming effects and implicit racial/ethnic associations for L1- and L2-accented speech. For L1-accented speech, we found no priming effects when comparing White versus East Asian or Latina primes. For L2- (Mandarin Chinese-) accented speech, however, transcription accuracy was slightly better following an East Asian prime than a White prime. Across all experiments, a relationship between performance and individual differences in implicit associations emerged, but in no cases did this relationship interact with the priming manipulation. Ultimately, exploring social priming effects with additional methodological approaches, and in different populations of listeners, will help to determine whether these effects operate differently in the context of L1- and L2-accented speech. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":50195,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Human Perception and Performance","volume":" ","pages":"329-357"},"PeriodicalIF":2.1,"publicationDate":"2024-04-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139708340","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
What drives the automatic retrieval of real-world object size knowledge? 是什么驱动了现实世界物体尺寸知识的自动检索?
IF 2.1 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-04-01 Epub Date: 2024-02-01 DOI: 10.1037/xhp0001189
Simen Hagen, Yuanfang Zhao, Lydia Moonen, Neele Ulken, Marius V Peelen

Real-world object size is a behaviorally relevant object property that is automatically retrieved when viewing object images: participants are faster to indicate the bigger of two object images when this object is also bigger in the real world. What drives this size Stroop effect? One possibility is that it reflects the automatic retrieval of real-world size after objects are recognized at the basic level (e.g., recognizing an object as a plane activates large real-world size). An alternative possibility is that the size Stroop effect is driven by automatic associations between low-/mid-level visual features (e.g., rectilinearity) and real-world size, bypassing object recognition. Here, we tested both accounts. In Experiment 1, objects were displayed upright and inverted, slowing down recognition while equating visual features. Inversion strongly reduced the Stroop effect, indicating that object recognition contributed to the Stroop effect. Independently of inversion, however, trial-wise differences in rectilinearity also contributed to the Stroop effect. In Experiment 2, the Stroop effect was compared between manmade objects (for which rectilinearity was associated with size) and animals (no association between rectilinearity and size). The Stroop effect was larger for animals than for manmade objects, indicating that rectilinear feature differences were not necessary for the Stroop effect. Finally, in Experiment 3, unrecognizable "texform" objects that maintained size-related visual feature differences were displayed upright and inverted. Results revealed a small Stroop effect for both upright and inverted conditions. Altogether, these results indicate that the size Stroop effect partly follows object recognition with an additional contribution from visual feature associations. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

现实世界中的物体大小是一种与行为相关的物体属性,在观看物体图像时会自动检索:当两个物体图像中的一个物体在现实世界中也较大时,参与者会更快地指出该物体较大。是什么驱动了这种大小斯特罗普效应?一种可能是,它反映了在基本水平上识别物体后对现实世界大小的自动检索(例如,将物体识别为平面会激活现实世界的大尺寸)。另一种可能是,大小 Stroop 效应是由中低级视觉特征(如直线性)和真实世界大小之间的自动关联驱动的,绕过了物体识别。在这里,我们测试了这两种说法。在实验 1 中,物体被直立和倒置显示,在等同视觉特征的同时减慢识别速度。倒置大大降低了 Stroop 效应,表明物体识别促成了 Stroop 效应。然而,与倒置无关的是,试验过程中的直线度差异也会导致斯特罗普效应。在实验 2 中,对人造物体(直线度与大小相关)和动物(直线度与大小无关)的 Stroop 效应进行了比较。动物的 Stroop 效应大于人造物体,这表明直角特征差异并非 Stroop 效应的必要条件。最后,在实验 3 中,对无法识别的 "texform "物体进行了直立和倒置显示,这些物体保持了与尺寸相关的视觉特征差异。结果显示,在直立和倒置的条件下都出现了轻微的 Stroop 效应。总之,这些结果表明,尺寸的 Stroop 效应部分是在物体识别之后产生的,另外还有视觉特征联想的贡献。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"What drives the automatic retrieval of real-world object size knowledge?","authors":"Simen Hagen, Yuanfang Zhao, Lydia Moonen, Neele Ulken, Marius V Peelen","doi":"10.1037/xhp0001189","DOIUrl":"10.1037/xhp0001189","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Real-world object size is a behaviorally relevant object property that is automatically retrieved when viewing object images: participants are faster to indicate the bigger of two object images when this object is also bigger in the real world. What drives this size Stroop effect? One possibility is that it reflects the automatic retrieval of real-world size after objects are recognized at the basic level (e.g., recognizing an object as a plane activates large real-world size). An alternative possibility is that the size Stroop effect is driven by automatic associations between low-/mid-level visual features (e.g., rectilinearity) and real-world size, bypassing object recognition. Here, we tested both accounts. In Experiment 1, objects were displayed upright and inverted, slowing down recognition while equating visual features. Inversion strongly reduced the Stroop effect, indicating that object recognition contributed to the Stroop effect. Independently of inversion, however, trial-wise differences in rectilinearity also contributed to the Stroop effect. In Experiment 2, the Stroop effect was compared between manmade objects (for which rectilinearity was associated with size) and animals (no association between rectilinearity and size). The Stroop effect was larger for animals than for manmade objects, indicating that rectilinear feature differences were not necessary for the Stroop effect. Finally, in Experiment 3, unrecognizable \"texform\" objects that maintained size-related visual feature differences were displayed upright and inverted. Results revealed a small Stroop effect for both upright and inverted conditions. Altogether, these results indicate that the size Stroop effect partly follows object recognition with an additional contribution from visual feature associations. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":50195,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Human Perception and Performance","volume":" ","pages":"358-369"},"PeriodicalIF":2.1,"publicationDate":"2024-04-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC7616435/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139652067","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Object-based attention is accentuated by object reward association. 基于物体的注意力会因物体奖励联想而增强。
IF 2.1 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-03-01 DOI: 10.1037/xhp0001177
Damiano Grignolio, David J Acunzo, Clayton Hickey

Humans use selective attention to prioritize visual features, like color or shape, as well as discrete spatial locations, and these effects are sensitive to the experience of reward. Reward-associated features and locations are accordingly prioritized from early in the visual hierarchy. Attention is also sensitive to the establishment of visual objects: selection of one constituent object part often leads to prioritization of other locations on that object. But very little is known about the influence of reward on this object-based control of attention. Here we show in four experiments that reward prioritization and object prioritization interact in visual cognition to guide selection. Experiment 1 establishes groundwork for this investigation, showing that reward feedback does not negate object prioritization. In Experiment 2, we corroborate the hypothesis that reward prioritization and object prioritization emerge concurrently. In Experiment 3, we find that reward prioritization and object prioritization sustain and interact in extinction, when reward feedback is discontinued. We verify this interaction in Experiment 4, linking it to task experience rather than the strategic utility of the reward association. Results suggest that information gathered from locations on reward-associated objects gains preferential access to cognition. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

人类利用选择性注意来确定视觉特征(如颜色或形状)以及离散空间位置的优先顺序,这些效应对奖励体验非常敏感。因此,与奖励相关的特征和位置在视觉层次结构的早期就被优先考虑。注意力对视觉对象的建立也很敏感:选择一个组成对象的部分往往会导致优先选择该对象上的其他位置。但是,人们对奖励对这种基于对象的注意力控制的影响知之甚少。在这里,我们通过四个实验证明,奖励优先和对象优先在视觉认知中相互作用,从而引导选择。实验 1 为这一研究奠定了基础,表明奖励反馈不会否定对象优先化。在实验 2 中,我们证实了奖励优先和对象优先同时出现的假设。在实验 3 中,我们发现当奖赏反馈停止时,奖赏优先化和对象优先化在消退过程中会持续并相互作用。我们在实验 4 中验证了这种相互作用,并将其与任务经验而非奖励关联的策略效用联系起来。结果表明,从奖励相关对象的位置收集到的信息会优先进入认知。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Object-based attention is accentuated by object reward association.","authors":"Damiano Grignolio, David J Acunzo, Clayton Hickey","doi":"10.1037/xhp0001177","DOIUrl":"10.1037/xhp0001177","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Humans use selective attention to prioritize visual features, like color or shape, as well as discrete spatial locations, and these effects are sensitive to the experience of reward. Reward-associated features and locations are accordingly prioritized from early in the visual hierarchy. Attention is also sensitive to the establishment of visual objects: selection of one constituent object part often leads to prioritization of other locations on that object. But very little is known about the influence of reward on this object-based control of attention. Here we show in four experiments that reward prioritization and object prioritization interact in visual cognition to guide selection. Experiment 1 establishes groundwork for this investigation, showing that reward feedback does not negate object prioritization. In Experiment 2, we corroborate the hypothesis that reward prioritization and object prioritization emerge concurrently. In Experiment 3, we find that reward prioritization and object prioritization sustain and interact in extinction, when reward feedback is discontinued. We verify this interaction in Experiment 4, linking it to task experience rather than the strategic utility of the reward association. Results suggest that information gathered from locations on reward-associated objects gains preferential access to cognition. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":50195,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Human Perception and Performance","volume":"50 3","pages":"280-294"},"PeriodicalIF":2.1,"publicationDate":"2024-03-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139998128","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Journal of Experimental Psychology-Human Perception and Performance
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1