首页 > 最新文献

Journal of Experimental Psychology-Human Perception and Performance最新文献

英文 中文
The surprising robustness of visual search against concurrent auditory distraction. 视觉搜索在同时受到听觉干扰时的惊人稳定性
IF 2.1 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-01-01 DOI: 10.1037/xhp0001168
Ananya Mandal, Anna M Liesefeld, Heinrich R Liesefeld

People often complain about distraction by irrelevant sounds that reportedly hamper performance on concurrent visual tasks demanding the allocation of focused attention toward relevant stimuli, such as processing street signs during driving. To study this everyday issue experimentally, we devised a cross-modal distraction paradigm, inspired by a standard visual-distraction paradigm (additional-singleton paradigm) that is highly sensitive to measure interference on the allocation of attention. In a visual-search pop-out task, participants reported whether a salient target (a tilted bar) was present or absent, while a completely irrelevant, but salient auditory distractor accompanied some trials. To our surprise, the results revealed no notable distraction on visual-search performance (controlled for speed-accuracy tradeoffs). Reliable auditory distraction failed to occur even when the distractor was a (highly salient) auditory oddball or was additionally presented with a temporal advantage of 300 ms. However, when the auditory modality was made relevant globally while maintaining its irrelevance to the visual-search task, we finally observed the expected interference effect. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

人们经常抱怨受到无关声音的干扰,据说这会影响同时进行的要求将注意力集中到相关刺激的视觉任务的表现,例如在驾驶过程中处理路标。为了对这一日常问题进行实验研究,我们设计了一种跨模态分心范式,其灵感来源于标准的视觉分心范式(附加单子范式),该范式能高度灵敏地测量对注意力分配的干扰。在一个视觉搜索弹出任务中,参与者报告一个突出的目标(一个倾斜的横杆)是存在还是不存在,而一个完全不相关但突出的听觉分心物则伴随着一些试验。出乎我们意料的是,结果显示视觉搜索成绩并没有受到明显的干扰(速度-准确性权衡控制)。即使干扰物是一个(高度突出的)听觉怪异物,或者额外呈现的时间优势为 300 毫秒,也没有出现可靠的听觉干扰。然而,当听觉模式在保持与视觉搜索任务无关的情况下变得全局相关时,我们最终观察到了预期的干扰效应。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"The surprising robustness of visual search against concurrent auditory distraction.","authors":"Ananya Mandal, Anna M Liesefeld, Heinrich R Liesefeld","doi":"10.1037/xhp0001168","DOIUrl":"10.1037/xhp0001168","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>People often complain about distraction by irrelevant sounds that reportedly hamper performance on concurrent visual tasks demanding the allocation of focused attention toward relevant stimuli, such as processing street signs during driving. To study this everyday issue experimentally, we devised a cross-modal distraction paradigm, inspired by a standard visual-distraction paradigm (additional-singleton paradigm) that is highly sensitive to measure interference on the allocation of attention. In a visual-search pop-out task, participants reported whether a salient target (a tilted bar) was present or absent, while a completely irrelevant, but salient auditory distractor accompanied some trials. To our surprise, the results revealed no notable distraction on visual-search performance (controlled for speed-accuracy tradeoffs). Reliable auditory distraction failed to occur even when the distractor was a (highly salient) auditory oddball or was additionally presented with a temporal advantage of 300 ms. However, when the auditory modality was made relevant globally while maintaining its irrelevance to the visual-search task, we finally observed the expected interference effect. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":50195,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Human Perception and Performance","volume":"50 1","pages":"99-118"},"PeriodicalIF":2.1,"publicationDate":"2024-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139486565","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Generalizability of control across cognitive and emotional conflict. 控制在认知和情绪冲突中的普遍性。
IF 2.1 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-01-01 DOI: 10.1037/xhp0001155
Elisa Ruth Straub, Moritz Schiltenwolf, Andrea Kiesel, David Dignath

People can learn to control their thoughts and emotions. The scientific study of control has been conducted mostly independently for cognitive and emotional conflicts. However, recent theoretical proposals suggest a close link between emotional and cognitive control processes. Indeed, mounting evidence from clinical sciences, social and personality psychology, and developmental neuroscience suggests that the ability to control thoughts and behavior goes hand in hand with the ability to control emotions. Yet, the precise interface between control over cognition and emotions remains controversial. The present study investigates the question whether control is a general-purpose mechanism or rather a set of domain-specific mechanisms. Following previous research, we tested participants' control in a cognitive and an emotional Stroop task and assessed the congruency sequence effect (CSE) which has been taken as a marker of cognitive or (implicit) emotional control, respectively. Going beyond previous research, we asked how control in one domain (e.g., cognitive) interacts with control in the other domain (e.g., emotional) on a trial-by-trial basis. In four experiments (N = 259) presented participants with a task-switching design that intermixed cognitive and emotional conflicts. This procedure produced significant CSEs across cognitive-emotional domains, suggesting that control can interact across domains. However, effect sizes of within-domain CSEs were twice as large, indicating that control is also domain-specific. These results neither support the general-purpose account nor the domain-specificity hypothesis of control. Rather, a hybrid account fits the data best, which also reconciles previous behavioral and neurophysiological findings, suggesting domain-general and specific processes. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

人们可以学会控制自己的思想和情绪。关于控制的科学研究大多是针对认知冲突和情绪冲突独立进行的。然而,最近的理论建议表明,情绪和认知控制过程之间存在密切联系。事实上,来自临床科学、社会和人格心理学以及发育神经科学的越来越多的证据表明,控制思想和行为的能力与控制情绪的能力是相辅相成的。然而,认知控制和情绪控制之间的准确衔接仍然存在争议。本研究探讨的问题是,控制究竟是一种通用机制,还是一系列特定领域的机制。根据以往的研究,我们测试了参与者在认知和情绪 Stroop 任务中的控制能力,并评估了一致性序列效应(CSE),该效应分别被视为认知或(内隐)情绪控制能力的标志。在以往研究的基础上,我们询问了一个领域(如认知)的控制与另一个领域(如情感)的控制在逐次试验的基础上是如何相互作用的。在四次实验中(N = 259),我们向参与者展示了一种任务切换设计,将认知冲突和情绪冲突混合在一起。这一过程在认知-情绪领域中产生了显著的 CSE,表明控制可以在不同领域中相互作用。然而,域内 CSE 的效应大小是域外 CSE 的两倍,这表明控制也是有域特异性的。这些结果既不支持控制的通用性假设,也不支持控制的领域特异性假设。相反,混合解释最适合这些数据,它也调和了之前的行为学和神经生理学发现,表明了领域通用性和特定性过程。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Generalizability of control across cognitive and emotional conflict.","authors":"Elisa Ruth Straub, Moritz Schiltenwolf, Andrea Kiesel, David Dignath","doi":"10.1037/xhp0001155","DOIUrl":"10.1037/xhp0001155","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>People can learn to control their thoughts and emotions. The scientific study of control has been conducted mostly independently for cognitive and emotional conflicts. However, recent theoretical proposals suggest a close link between emotional and cognitive control processes. Indeed, mounting evidence from clinical sciences, social and personality psychology, and developmental neuroscience suggests that the ability to control thoughts and behavior goes hand in hand with the ability to control emotions. Yet, the precise interface between control over cognition and emotions remains controversial. The present study investigates the question whether control is a general-purpose mechanism or rather a set of domain-specific mechanisms. Following previous research, we tested participants' control in a cognitive and an emotional Stroop task and assessed the congruency sequence effect (CSE) which has been taken as a marker of cognitive or (implicit) emotional control, respectively. Going beyond previous research, we asked how control in one domain (e.g., cognitive) interacts with control in the other domain (e.g., emotional) on a trial-by-trial basis. In four experiments (N = 259) presented participants with a task-switching design that intermixed cognitive and emotional conflicts. This procedure produced significant CSEs across cognitive-emotional domains, suggesting that control can interact across domains. However, effect sizes of within-domain CSEs were twice as large, indicating that control is also domain-specific. These results neither support the general-purpose account nor the domain-specificity hypothesis of control. Rather, a hybrid account fits the data best, which also reconciles previous behavioral and neurophysiological findings, suggesting domain-general and specific processes. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":50195,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Human Perception and Performance","volume":"50 1","pages":"2-22"},"PeriodicalIF":2.1,"publicationDate":"2024-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139486545","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Automatic imitation in children: Age-related change and comparison to adults. 儿童的自动模仿:与年龄有关的变化以及与成人的比较
IF 2.1 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-01-01 DOI: 10.1037/xhp0001175
Kae Mukai, Ryoji Onagawa, Katsumi Watanabe

Automatic imitation, in which one person's movement is affected by the observation of another person's movements, has been widely reported. However, it remains unclear how automatic imitation changes over a wide age range, particularly during childhood. In this study, we examined the differences in the tendency for automatic imitation between adults and children and the cross-sectional age-related changes in children aged 5-12 years, using a stimulus-response conflict paradigm. In this task, participants perform a choice-reactive finger movement corresponding to a given response stimulus while observing another participant's compatible or incompatible movement stimuli. The tendency for automatic imitation was assessed based on the reaction time, correct rate, and inverse efficiency score. The results showed that the degree of automatic imitation was weak until the children were 7 years old. Interestingly, our results show that the tendency for automatic imitation during childhood changed to an inverted U-shape, indicating nonlinear changes in automatic imitation during childhood. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

自动模仿,即一个人的动作受到观察到的另一个人的动作的影响,已被广泛报道。然而,人们仍然不清楚自动模仿在不同年龄段,尤其是在儿童时期是如何变化的。在这项研究中,我们采用刺激-反应冲突范式,研究了成人和儿童之间自动模仿倾向的差异,以及 5-12 岁儿童与年龄相关的横断面变化。在这项任务中,被试在观察另一名被试相容或不相容的动作刺激的同时,做出与给定反应刺激相对应的选择反应性手指动作。根据反应时间、正确率和反效率得分来评估自动模仿的倾向。结果显示,儿童在 7 岁之前的自动模仿程度较弱。有趣的是,我们的研究结果表明,儿童时期的自动模仿倾向变成了倒 U 型,这表明儿童时期的自动模仿发生了非线性变化。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Automatic imitation in children: Age-related change and comparison to adults.","authors":"Kae Mukai, Ryoji Onagawa, Katsumi Watanabe","doi":"10.1037/xhp0001175","DOIUrl":"10.1037/xhp0001175","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Automatic imitation, in which one person's movement is affected by the observation of another person's movements, has been widely reported. However, it remains unclear how automatic imitation changes over a wide age range, particularly during childhood. In this study, we examined the differences in the tendency for automatic imitation between adults and children and the cross-sectional age-related changes in children aged 5-12 years, using a stimulus-response conflict paradigm. In this task, participants perform a choice-reactive finger movement corresponding to a given response stimulus while observing another participant's compatible or incompatible movement stimuli. The tendency for automatic imitation was assessed based on the reaction time, correct rate, and inverse efficiency score. The results showed that the degree of automatic imitation was weak until the children were 7 years old. Interestingly, our results show that the tendency for automatic imitation during childhood changed to an inverted U-shape, indicating nonlinear changes in automatic imitation during childhood. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":50195,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Human Perception and Performance","volume":"50 1","pages":"64-73"},"PeriodicalIF":2.1,"publicationDate":"2024-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139486543","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Another dimension! Using dimension weighting to observe integration and retrieval in localization performance. 另一个维度!利用维度加权观察定位性能中的整合和检索。
IF 2.1 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-01-01 DOI: 10.1037/xhp0001176
Lars-Michael Schöpper, Ronja Hoffmann, Christian Frings

According to action control theories, responding to a stimulus leads to the integration of response and stimulus features (e.g., color, shape, etc.) into event files. Upon feature repetition, the event file is retrieved, affecting performance. However, the resulting so-called binding effects are typically absent when participants localize targets in a sequence. Here, only a location change benefit emerges, known as inhibition of return (IOR), unmodulated by feature repetitions and changes. This has often been replicated in attentional orienting research. However, in these experimental designs only the individual stimulus feature is systematically varied to repeat or change. Based on the dimension weighting account from visual search, we hypothesized that the lack of binding effects in localization performance is due to a lack of systematically varying feature dimensions. In three experiments, participants localized targets in the left or right hemifield. The target appeared at multiple possible locations with gray distractors (Experiment 1), at multiple possible locations without distractors (Experiment 2), and at two possible locations without distractors (Experiment 3). Crucially, the nonspatial identity of the target repeated its dimension with or without the individual feature, or it changed its dimension. Whereas a binding effect between response and feature was absent, we found a binding pattern between response and feature dimension. IOR was always present. The results fit well with theories of different research strands-action control, attentional orienting, and visual search-suggesting that these gain impact by incorporating each other's ideas, leading to an all-encompassing understanding of selection and action. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

根据行动控制理论,对刺激做出反应会将反应和刺激特征(如颜色、形状等)整合到事件文件中。当特征重复时,事件文件会被检索出来,从而影响表现。然而,当参与者定位序列中的目标时,通常不会产生所谓的绑定效应。在这种情况下,只有位置变化带来的益处,即所谓的返回抑制(IOR),不受特征重复和变化的影响。这种情况在注意定向研究中也经常出现。然而,在这些实验设计中,只有个别刺激特征被系统地改变为重复或变化。基于视觉搜索中的维度权重理论,我们假设,定位成绩中缺乏结合效应是由于缺乏系统变化的特征维度。在三个实验中,被试在左侧或右侧半视野中定位目标。目标出现在有灰色干扰物的多个可能位置(实验 1)、无干扰物的多个可能位置(实验 2)和无干扰物的两个可能位置(实验 3)。最重要的是,目标的非空间特征会在有或没有单个特征的情况下重复其维度,或者改变其维度。虽然反应与特征之间不存在结合效应,但我们发现反应与特征维度之间存在结合模式。IOR始终存在。研究结果与不同研究领域的理论--行动控制、注意定向和视觉搜索--非常吻合,这表明这些理论通过相互融合产生了影响,从而对选择和行动有了全面的理解。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Another dimension! Using dimension weighting to observe integration and retrieval in localization performance.","authors":"Lars-Michael Schöpper, Ronja Hoffmann, Christian Frings","doi":"10.1037/xhp0001176","DOIUrl":"10.1037/xhp0001176","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>According to action control theories, responding to a stimulus leads to the integration of response and stimulus features (e.g., color, shape, etc.) into event files. Upon feature repetition, the event file is retrieved, affecting performance. However, the resulting so-called binding effects are typically absent when participants localize targets in a sequence. Here, only a location change benefit emerges, known as inhibition of return (IOR), unmodulated by feature repetitions and changes. This has often been replicated in attentional orienting research. However, in these experimental designs only the individual stimulus feature is systematically varied to repeat or change. Based on the dimension weighting account from visual search, we hypothesized that the lack of binding effects in localization performance is due to a lack of systematically varying feature dimensions. In three experiments, participants localized targets in the left or right hemifield. The target appeared at multiple possible locations with gray distractors (Experiment 1), at multiple possible locations without distractors (Experiment 2), and at two possible locations without distractors (Experiment 3). Crucially, the nonspatial identity of the target repeated its dimension with or without the individual feature, or it changed its dimension. Whereas a binding effect between response and feature was absent, we found a binding pattern between response and feature dimension. IOR was always present. The results fit well with theories of different research strands-action control, attentional orienting, and visual search-suggesting that these gain impact by incorporating each other's ideas, leading to an all-encompassing understanding of selection and action. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":50195,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Human Perception and Performance","volume":"50 1","pages":"23-38"},"PeriodicalIF":2.1,"publicationDate":"2024-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139486483","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Parafoveal processing in bilingual readers: Semantic access within but not across languages. 双语读者的副中心凹处理:语言内部而非跨语言的语义访问。
IF 2.1 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2023-12-01 Epub Date: 2023-10-26 DOI: 10.1037/xhp0001161
Liv J Hoversten, Clara D Martin

Prior research has investigated the quality of information a reader can extract from upcoming parafoveal words. However, very few studies have considered parafoveal processing in bilingual readers, who may differ from monolinguals due to slower lexical access and susceptibility to cross-language activation. This eye-tracking experiment, therefore, investigated how bilingual readers process parafoveal semantic information within and across languages. We used the boundary technique to replace a preview word in a sentence with a different target word during the first rightward saccade from the pretarget region. We manipulated both preview language (nonswitch vs. code-switch) and semantic relatedness (synonym/translation vs. unrelated) between previews and targets. Upon fixation, target words always appeared in the same language as the rest of the sentence to create an essentially monolingual language context. Semantic preview benefits emerged for nonswitched synonym previews but not for code-switched translation previews. Furthermore, participants skipped code-switched previews less often than nonswitched previews and no more often than previews that were unfamiliar to them. These data suggest that bilinguals can extract within-language semantic information from the parafovea in both native and nonnative languages, but that cross-language words are not accessible while reading in a monolingual language mode, as per the partial selectivity hypothesis of bilingual language control. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2023 APA, all rights reserved).

先前的研究已经调查了读者可以从即将出现的旁凹词中提取的信息的质量。然而,很少有研究考虑双语读者的旁凹加工,他们可能与单语读者不同,因为词汇访问较慢,容易被跨语言激活。因此,这项眼动追踪实验调查了双语读者如何在语言内部和语言之间处理旁旁语义信息。在第一次从目标前区域向右扫视时,我们使用边界技术将句子中的预览词替换为不同的目标词。我们操纵了预览语言(非切换与代码切换)和预览和目标之间的语义相关性(同义词/翻译与无关)。在固定后,目标词总是出现在与句子其余部分相同的语言中,以创建一个基本上单语的语言上下文。语义预览的好处出现在非切换同义词预览中,但不适用于代码切换翻译预览。此外,参与者跳过代码切换预览的频率低于非切换预览,也不高于他们不熟悉的预览。这些数据表明,在母语和非母语中,双语者都可以从副腔提取语内语义信息,但根据双语语言控制的部分选择性假设,在单语模式下阅读时,跨语言单词是不可访问的。(PsycInfo数据库记录(c)2023 APA,保留所有权利)。
{"title":"Parafoveal processing in bilingual readers: Semantic access within but not across languages.","authors":"Liv J Hoversten, Clara D Martin","doi":"10.1037/xhp0001161","DOIUrl":"10.1037/xhp0001161","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Prior research has investigated the quality of information a reader can extract from upcoming parafoveal words. However, very few studies have considered parafoveal processing in bilingual readers, who may differ from monolinguals due to slower lexical access and susceptibility to cross-language activation. This eye-tracking experiment, therefore, investigated how bilingual readers process parafoveal semantic information within and across languages. We used the boundary technique to replace a preview word in a sentence with a different target word during the first rightward saccade from the pretarget region. We manipulated both preview language (nonswitch vs. code-switch) and semantic relatedness (synonym/translation vs. unrelated) between previews and targets. Upon fixation, target words always appeared in the same language as the rest of the sentence to create an essentially monolingual language context. Semantic preview benefits emerged for nonswitched synonym previews but not for code-switched translation previews. Furthermore, participants skipped code-switched previews less often than nonswitched previews and no more often than previews that were unfamiliar to them. These data suggest that bilinguals can extract within-language semantic information from the parafovea in both native and nonnative languages, but that cross-language words are not accessible while reading in a monolingual language mode, as per the partial selectivity hypothesis of bilingual language control. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2023 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":50195,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Human Perception and Performance","volume":" ","pages":"1564-1578"},"PeriodicalIF":2.1,"publicationDate":"2023-12-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"50163471","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Are upside-down faces perceived as "less human"? 倒置的脸被认为是“不那么人性化”吗?
IF 2.1 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2023-12-01 Epub Date: 2023-10-30 DOI: 10.1037/xhp0001167
Adam Eggleston, Richard Cook, Harriet Over

According to perceptual dehumanization theory (PDT), faces are only perceived as "truly human" when processed in a configural fashion. Consistent with this theory, previous research indicates that when faces are inverted, a manipulation hypothesized to disrupt configural processing, the individuals depicted are attributed fewer uniquely human qualities. In a seminal paper, Hugenberg et al. (2016) reported that faces appeared less creative, less thoughtful, less empathetic, and possessed less "humanness" when inverted. Across four highly powered and preregistered experiments, we demonstrate that inversion does not influence the attribution of uniquely human traits specifically. Rather, in line with research on face processing, inversion impedes face encoding more generally, causing trait attributions to tend toward the mean. Positively valanced faces (i.e., those judged to be trustworthy when presented upright) are perceived to be less creative, considerate, thoughtful, and empathetic when inverted. Conversely, negatively valanced faces (i.e., those judged to be untrustworthy when presented upright) are judged to be more creative, considerate, thoughtful, and empathetic when inverted. Furthermore, we show that the effect of inversion on judgments of "humanness" reflects a general phenomenon that can be replicated with other (nonface) stimulus categories that also possess a canonical orientation. These findings suggest that a key line of evidence for PDT is considerably less convincing than it first appears. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2023 APA, all rights reserved).

根据感知非人化理论(PDT),只有在以结构方式处理人脸时,人脸才会被视为“真正的人类”。与这一理论一致的是,先前的研究表明,当人脸被倒置时,被描绘的个体被赋予的独特人性更少。在一篇具有开创性意义的论文中,Hugenberg等人(2016)报告称,当脸被倒置时,看起来没有那么有创造力,没有那么深思熟虑,没有那么有同情心,也没有那么“人性”。在四个强大且预先注册的实验中,我们证明了反转不会特别影响人类独特特征的归因。相反,与人脸处理的研究一致,反转更普遍地阻碍了人脸编码,导致特征归因倾向于均值。积极有价值的面孔(即那些在直立时被认为是值得信赖的面孔)在倒置时被认为不那么有创造力、体贴、体贴和同理心。相反,负面评价的面孔(即那些在直立时被认为不可信的面孔)在倒置时被认为更有创造力、体贴、体贴和同理心。此外,我们还表明,反转对“人性”判断的影响反映了一种普遍现象,这种现象可以与其他(非致命)刺激类别复制,这些刺激类别也具有规范方向。这些发现表明,PDT的一条关键证据线远没有最初出现的那么令人信服。(PsycInfo数据库记录(c)2023 APA,保留所有权利)。
{"title":"Are upside-down faces perceived as \"less human\"?","authors":"Adam Eggleston, Richard Cook, Harriet Over","doi":"10.1037/xhp0001167","DOIUrl":"10.1037/xhp0001167","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>According to perceptual dehumanization theory (PDT), faces are only perceived as \"truly human\" when processed in a configural fashion. Consistent with this theory, previous research indicates that when faces are inverted, a manipulation hypothesized to disrupt configural processing, the individuals depicted are attributed fewer uniquely human qualities. In a seminal paper, Hugenberg et al. (2016) reported that faces appeared less creative, less thoughtful, less empathetic, and possessed less \"humanness\" when inverted. Across four highly powered and preregistered experiments, we demonstrate that inversion does not influence the attribution of uniquely human traits specifically. Rather, in line with research on face processing, inversion impedes face encoding more generally, causing trait attributions to tend toward the mean. Positively valanced faces (i.e., those judged to be trustworthy when presented upright) are perceived to be less creative, considerate, thoughtful, and empathetic when inverted. Conversely, negatively valanced faces (i.e., those judged to be untrustworthy when presented upright) are judged to be more creative, considerate, thoughtful, and empathetic when inverted. Furthermore, we show that the effect of inversion on judgments of \"humanness\" reflects a general phenomenon that can be replicated with other (nonface) stimulus categories that also possess a canonical orientation. These findings suggest that a key line of evidence for PDT is considerably less convincing than it first appears. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2023 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":50195,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Human Perception and Performance","volume":" ","pages":"1503-1517"},"PeriodicalIF":2.1,"publicationDate":"2023-12-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"71414993","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Visual statistical learning of naturalistic textures. 自然纹理的视觉统计学习。
IF 2.1 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2023-12-01 Epub Date: 2023-10-05 DOI: 10.1037/xhp0001152
Siyuan Cheng, Hailin Ai, Yiran Ge, Yuanyi Luo, Nihong Chen

The visual system continuously adapts to the statistical properties of the environment. In this study, we demonstrated that training significantly enhanced subjects' perceptual sensitivity to co-occurrence statistics in naturalistic textures. The learning effect was specific to the statistical component and spatial location. By examining the time course of learning, we found that learning was accelerated at an untrained location. Our findings establish a link between statistical learning and visual perception, indicating multistage plasticity beyond V1 in the visual hierarchy. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2023 APA, all rights reserved).

视觉系统不断适应环境的统计特性。在这项研究中,我们证明了训练显著提高了受试者对自然纹理中共现统计的感知敏感性。学习效果是特定于统计成分和空间位置的。通过研究学习的时间过程,我们发现在未经训练的地方学习会加速。我们的发现建立了统计学习和视觉感知之间的联系,表明视觉层次中超过V1的多阶段可塑性。(PsycInfo数据库记录(c)2023 APA,保留所有权利)。
{"title":"Visual statistical learning of naturalistic textures.","authors":"Siyuan Cheng, Hailin Ai, Yiran Ge, Yuanyi Luo, Nihong Chen","doi":"10.1037/xhp0001152","DOIUrl":"10.1037/xhp0001152","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The visual system continuously adapts to the statistical properties of the environment. In this study, we demonstrated that training significantly enhanced subjects' perceptual sensitivity to co-occurrence statistics in naturalistic textures. The learning effect was specific to the statistical component and spatial location. By examining the time course of learning, we found that learning was accelerated at an untrained location. Our findings establish a link between statistical learning and visual perception, indicating multistage plasticity beyond V1 in the visual hierarchy. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2023 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":50195,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Human Perception and Performance","volume":" ","pages":"1579-1590"},"PeriodicalIF":2.1,"publicationDate":"2023-12-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"41153560","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The best fitting of three contemporary observer models reveals how participants' strategy influences the window of subjective synchrony. 三个当代观察者模型的最佳拟合揭示了参与者的策略如何影响主观同步窗口。
IF 2.1 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2023-12-01 Epub Date: 2023-11-02 DOI: 10.1037/xhp0001154
Kielan Yarrow, Joshua A Solomon, Derek H Arnold, Warrick Roseboom

When experimenters vary the timing between two intersensory events, and participants judge their simultaneity, an inverse-U-shaped psychometric function is obtained. Typically, this simultaneity function is first fitted with a model for each participant separately, before best-fitting parameters are utilized (e.g., compared across conditions) in the second stage of a two-step inferential procedure. Often, simultaneity-function width is interpreted as representing sensitivity to asynchrony, and/or ascribed theoretical equivalence to a window of multisensory temporal binding. Here, we instead fit a single (principled) multilevel model to data from the entire group and across several conditions at once. By asking 20 participants to sometimes be more conservative in their judgments, we demonstrate how the width of the simultaneity function is prone to strategic change and thus questionable as a measure of either sensitivity to asynchrony or multisensory binding. By repeating our analysis with three different models (two implying a decision based directly on subjective asynchrony, and a third deriving this decision from the correlation between filtered responses to sensory inputs) we find that the first model, which hypothesizes, in particular, Gaussian latency noise and difficulty maintaining the stability of decision criteria across trials, is most plausible for these data. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2023 APA, all rights reserved).

当实验者改变两个传感器间事件的时间,并且参与者判断它们的同时性时,得到了一个倒U型心理测量函数。通常,在两步推理程序的第二阶段中使用最佳拟合参数(例如,在不同条件下进行比较)之前,首先分别为每个参与者对该同时性函数进行模型拟合。通常,同时性函数宽度被解释为表示对异步的敏感性,和/或将理论等价性归因于多感觉时间绑定的窗口。在这里,我们将单个(原则性)多级模型同时适用于来自整个组和多个条件的数据。通过让20名参与者有时在判断中更加保守,我们展示了同时性函数的宽度是如何容易发生战略变化的,因此作为衡量对异步或多感官绑定敏感度的指标是值得怀疑的。通过用三个不同的模型重复我们的分析(两个模型意味着直接基于主观异步的决策,第三个模型从对感觉输入的过滤反应之间的相关性得出该决策),我们发现第一个模型,特别是假设高斯潜伏噪声和难以在试验中保持决策标准的稳定性,对于这些数据来说是最合理的。(PsycInfo数据库记录(c)2023 APA,保留所有权利)。
{"title":"The best fitting of three contemporary observer models reveals how participants' strategy influences the window of subjective synchrony.","authors":"Kielan Yarrow, Joshua A Solomon, Derek H Arnold, Warrick Roseboom","doi":"10.1037/xhp0001154","DOIUrl":"10.1037/xhp0001154","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>When experimenters vary the timing between two intersensory events, and participants judge their simultaneity, an inverse-U-shaped psychometric function is obtained. Typically, this <i>simultaneity function</i> is first fitted with a model for each participant separately, before best-fitting parameters are utilized (e.g., compared across conditions) in the second stage of a two-step inferential procedure. Often, simultaneity-function width is interpreted as representing sensitivity to asynchrony, and/or ascribed theoretical equivalence to a window of multisensory temporal binding. Here, we instead fit a single (principled) multilevel model to data from the entire group and across several conditions at once. By asking 20 participants to sometimes be more conservative in their judgments, we demonstrate how the width of the simultaneity function is prone to strategic change and thus questionable as a measure of either sensitivity to asynchrony or multisensory binding. By repeating our analysis with three different models (two implying a decision based directly on subjective asynchrony, and a third deriving this decision from the correlation between filtered responses to sensory inputs) we find that the first model, which hypothesizes, in particular, Gaussian latency noise and difficulty maintaining the stability of decision criteria across trials, is most plausible for these data. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2023 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":50195,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Human Perception and Performance","volume":" ","pages":"1534-1563"},"PeriodicalIF":2.1,"publicationDate":"2023-12-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"71428607","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
More of me: Self-prioritization of numeric stimuli. 更多的我:数字刺激的自我优先级。
IF 2.1 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2023-12-01 Epub Date: 2023-11-02 DOI: 10.1037/xhp0001165
Johannes Keil, Ayla Barutchu, Clea Desebrock, Charles Spence

People process stimuli that have been arbitrarily associated with the self versus with a stranger preferentially, but congruence effects can modulate self-prioritization, as when the self is paired with, for example, symmetrical versus asymmetrical stimuli. In two experiments, we examined the interaction of self-prioritization with number magnitude when participants associated the self or a stranger with specific number symbols such as "2" presented as natural, negative, and ordinal number types (Experiment 1), or abstract numeric concepts, such as "larger than 5" (Experiment 2). Empathy and personal distance were also assessed. While self-prioritization emerged in both experiments, number type (natural, ordinal, and negative) had no effect on performance. Furthermore, correlations with empathy and personal distance did not emerge consistently. An interaction between number magnitude and self-assignment was observed for the magnitude comparison matching task (e.g., > 5) (Experiment 2), but not in the specific number (e.g., "8") matching task (Experiment 1). The null interaction may reflect the fact that encoding symbol identity, but not number magnitude, was sufficient for the symbol-matching task. The order of numbers and self-associations also had an effect. In sum, this study is the first to show that self-prioritization emerges for symbolic numbers and can even occur with abstract categories, such as a range of numbers (e.g., > 5). Furthermore, congruence effects between number concepts and labels (e.g., for the stranger, less is better) may also affect performance. However, this would appear to depend on the task context, such as whether numeric magnitude was needed to complete the task. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2023 APA, all rights reserved).

与陌生人相比,人们优先处理与自己任意相关的刺激,但一致性效应可以调节自我优先级,例如当自我与对称或不对称刺激配对时。在两个实验中,当参与者将自己或陌生人与特定的数字符号(如自然、负数和序数类型的“2”(实验1))或抽象的数字概念(如“大于5”(实验2))相关联时,我们检验了自我优先级与数字幅度的相互作用。同理心和个人距离也被评估。虽然在两个实验中都出现了自优先级,但数字类型(自然、有序和负)对性能没有影响。此外,与同理心和个人距离的相关性并没有持续出现。在数量级比较匹配任务(例如,>5)(实验2)中观察到数量级和自我分配之间的相互作用,但在特定数量(例如,“8”)匹配任务中没有观察到(实验1)。零交互可以反映这样一个事实,即编码符号身份而不是数字幅度对于符号匹配任务是足够的。数字顺序和自联想也产生了影响。总之,这项研究首次表明,符号数字出现了自优先级,甚至可以出现在抽象类别中,例如一系列数字(例如,>5)。此外,数字概念和标签之间的一致性效应(例如,对于陌生人来说,越少越好)也可能影响性能。然而,这似乎取决于任务上下文,例如是否需要数字幅度来完成任务。(PsycInfo数据库记录(c)2023 APA,保留所有权利)。
{"title":"More of me: Self-prioritization of numeric stimuli.","authors":"Johannes Keil, Ayla Barutchu, Clea Desebrock, Charles Spence","doi":"10.1037/xhp0001165","DOIUrl":"10.1037/xhp0001165","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>People process stimuli that have been arbitrarily associated with the self versus with a stranger preferentially, but congruence effects can modulate self-prioritization, as when the self is paired with, for example, symmetrical versus asymmetrical stimuli. In two experiments, we examined the interaction of self-prioritization with number magnitude when participants associated the self or a stranger with specific number symbols such as \"2\" presented as natural, negative, and ordinal number types (Experiment 1), or abstract numeric concepts, such as \"larger than 5\" (Experiment 2). Empathy and personal distance were also assessed. While self-prioritization emerged in both experiments, number type (natural, ordinal, and negative) had no effect on performance. Furthermore, correlations with empathy and personal distance did not emerge consistently. An interaction between number magnitude and self-assignment was observed for the magnitude comparison matching task (e.g., > 5) (Experiment 2), but not in the specific number (e.g., \"8\") matching task (Experiment 1). The null interaction may reflect the fact that encoding symbol identity, but not number magnitude, was sufficient for the symbol-matching task. The order of numbers and self-associations also had an effect. In sum, this study is the first to show that self-prioritization emerges for symbolic numbers and can even occur with abstract categories, such as a range of numbers (e.g., > 5). Furthermore, congruence effects between number concepts and labels (e.g., for the stranger, less is better) may also affect performance. However, this would appear to depend on the task context, such as whether numeric magnitude was needed to complete the task. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2023 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":50195,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Human Perception and Performance","volume":" ","pages":"1518-1533"},"PeriodicalIF":2.1,"publicationDate":"2023-12-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"71428606","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
Supplemental Material for More of Me: Self-Prioritization of Numeric Stimuli 补充材料更多的我:自我优先级的数字刺激
3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2023-11-02 DOI: 10.1037/xhp0001165.supp
{"title":"Supplemental Material for More of Me: Self-Prioritization of Numeric Stimuli","authors":"","doi":"10.1037/xhp0001165.supp","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/xhp0001165.supp","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":50195,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Human Perception and Performance","volume":"51 11","pages":"0"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2023-11-02","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"135874975","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Journal of Experimental Psychology-Human Perception and Performance
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1