首页 > 最新文献

Journal of Experimental Psychology-Human Perception and Performance最新文献

英文 中文
More of me: Self-prioritization of numeric stimuli. 更多的我:数字刺激的自我优先级。
IF 2.1 3区 心理学 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2023-12-01 Epub Date: 2023-11-02 DOI: 10.1037/xhp0001165
Johannes Keil, Ayla Barutchu, Clea Desebrock, Charles Spence

People process stimuli that have been arbitrarily associated with the self versus with a stranger preferentially, but congruence effects can modulate self-prioritization, as when the self is paired with, for example, symmetrical versus asymmetrical stimuli. In two experiments, we examined the interaction of self-prioritization with number magnitude when participants associated the self or a stranger with specific number symbols such as "2" presented as natural, negative, and ordinal number types (Experiment 1), or abstract numeric concepts, such as "larger than 5" (Experiment 2). Empathy and personal distance were also assessed. While self-prioritization emerged in both experiments, number type (natural, ordinal, and negative) had no effect on performance. Furthermore, correlations with empathy and personal distance did not emerge consistently. An interaction between number magnitude and self-assignment was observed for the magnitude comparison matching task (e.g., > 5) (Experiment 2), but not in the specific number (e.g., "8") matching task (Experiment 1). The null interaction may reflect the fact that encoding symbol identity, but not number magnitude, was sufficient for the symbol-matching task. The order of numbers and self-associations also had an effect. In sum, this study is the first to show that self-prioritization emerges for symbolic numbers and can even occur with abstract categories, such as a range of numbers (e.g., > 5). Furthermore, congruence effects between number concepts and labels (e.g., for the stranger, less is better) may also affect performance. However, this would appear to depend on the task context, such as whether numeric magnitude was needed to complete the task. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2023 APA, all rights reserved).

与陌生人相比,人们优先处理与自己任意相关的刺激,但一致性效应可以调节自我优先级,例如当自我与对称或不对称刺激配对时。在两个实验中,当参与者将自己或陌生人与特定的数字符号(如自然、负数和序数类型的“2”(实验1))或抽象的数字概念(如“大于5”(实验2))相关联时,我们检验了自我优先级与数字幅度的相互作用。同理心和个人距离也被评估。虽然在两个实验中都出现了自优先级,但数字类型(自然、有序和负)对性能没有影响。此外,与同理心和个人距离的相关性并没有持续出现。在数量级比较匹配任务(例如,>5)(实验2)中观察到数量级和自我分配之间的相互作用,但在特定数量(例如,“8”)匹配任务中没有观察到(实验1)。零交互可以反映这样一个事实,即编码符号身份而不是数字幅度对于符号匹配任务是足够的。数字顺序和自联想也产生了影响。总之,这项研究首次表明,符号数字出现了自优先级,甚至可以出现在抽象类别中,例如一系列数字(例如,>5)。此外,数字概念和标签之间的一致性效应(例如,对于陌生人来说,越少越好)也可能影响性能。然而,这似乎取决于任务上下文,例如是否需要数字幅度来完成任务。(PsycInfo数据库记录(c)2023 APA,保留所有权利)。
{"title":"More of me: Self-prioritization of numeric stimuli.","authors":"Johannes Keil, Ayla Barutchu, Clea Desebrock, Charles Spence","doi":"10.1037/xhp0001165","DOIUrl":"10.1037/xhp0001165","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>People process stimuli that have been arbitrarily associated with the self versus with a stranger preferentially, but congruence effects can modulate self-prioritization, as when the self is paired with, for example, symmetrical versus asymmetrical stimuli. In two experiments, we examined the interaction of self-prioritization with number magnitude when participants associated the self or a stranger with specific number symbols such as \"2\" presented as natural, negative, and ordinal number types (Experiment 1), or abstract numeric concepts, such as \"larger than 5\" (Experiment 2). Empathy and personal distance were also assessed. While self-prioritization emerged in both experiments, number type (natural, ordinal, and negative) had no effect on performance. Furthermore, correlations with empathy and personal distance did not emerge consistently. An interaction between number magnitude and self-assignment was observed for the magnitude comparison matching task (e.g., > 5) (Experiment 2), but not in the specific number (e.g., \"8\") matching task (Experiment 1). The null interaction may reflect the fact that encoding symbol identity, but not number magnitude, was sufficient for the symbol-matching task. The order of numbers and self-associations also had an effect. In sum, this study is the first to show that self-prioritization emerges for symbolic numbers and can even occur with abstract categories, such as a range of numbers (e.g., > 5). Furthermore, congruence effects between number concepts and labels (e.g., for the stranger, less is better) may also affect performance. However, this would appear to depend on the task context, such as whether numeric magnitude was needed to complete the task. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2023 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":50195,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Human Perception and Performance","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.1,"publicationDate":"2023-12-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"71428606","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
Supplemental Material for More of Me: Self-Prioritization of Numeric Stimuli 补充材料更多的我:自我优先级的数字刺激
3区 心理学 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2023-11-02 DOI: 10.1037/xhp0001165.supp
{"title":"Supplemental Material for More of Me: Self-Prioritization of Numeric Stimuli","authors":"","doi":"10.1037/xhp0001165.supp","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/xhp0001165.supp","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":50195,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Human Perception and Performance","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2023-11-02","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"135874975","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Are there good days and bad days for hearing? Quantifying day-to-day intraindividual speech perception variability in older and younger adults. 听力有好日子和坏日子吗?量化老年人和年轻人日常个体内言语感知的可变性。
IF 2.1 3区 心理学 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2023-11-01 DOI: 10.1037/xhp0001159
Inka Kuhlmann, Giulia Angonese, Christiane Thiel, Birger Kollmeier, Andrea Hildebrandt

Moment-to-moment variations in hearing and speech perception have long been observed. Depending on the researcher's theoretical position, the observed fluctuations have been attributed to measurement error or to internal, nonsensory factors such as fluctuations in attention. While cognitive performance has been shown to fluctuate from day to day over longer time, such fluctuations have not been quantified for speech perception, despite being well-recognized by clinical audiologists and hearing-impaired patients. In three studies, we aimed to explore and quantify the magnitude of daily variability in speech perception and to investigate whether such variability goes beyond test unreliability. We also asked whether intraindividual variability depends on overall speech perception performance as observed in different groups of individuals. Older adults with objective hearing impairment and mostly hearing aids (N₁ = 45), with subjective hearing problems but no hearing aids (N₂ = 113), and younger adults without hearing problems (N₃ = 20) participated in three ecological momentary assessment studies. They performed a digit-in-noise test two to three times a day for several weeks. Variance heterogeneous linear mixed-effects models indicated reliable intraindividual variability in speech perception and substantial individual differences in daily variability. A protective factor against daily fluctuations is a higher average speech perception. These studies show that day-to-day variations in speech perception cannot simply be attributed to test unreliability and pave the way for investigating how psychological states that do not vary from moment-to-moment, but rather from day to day, predict variations in speech perception. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2023 APA, all rights reserved).

长期以来,人们一直观察到听觉和言语感知的瞬间变化。根据研究人员的理论立场,观察到的波动被归因于测量误差或内部的非感官因素,如注意力的波动。虽然认知表现已被证明在更长的时间内每天都在波动,但尽管临床听力学家和听障患者已经很好地认识到了这种波动,但这种波动还没有被量化用于言语感知。在三项研究中,我们旨在探索和量化语音感知中日常变化的幅度,并调查这种变化是否超出了测试的不可靠性。我们还询问了个体内的变异性是否取决于在不同个体组中观察到的整体言语感知表现。患有客观听力障碍且主要是助听器的老年人(N₁ = 45),有主观听力问题,但没有助听器(N₂ = 113),以及没有听力问题的年轻人(N₃ = 20) 参与了三项生态瞬时评估研究。他们连续几周每天进行两到三次数字噪声测试。方差异质线性混合效应模型表明,言语感知的个体内变异性可靠,日常变异性的个体差异显著。抵御日常波动的一个保护因素是较高的平均语音感知。这些研究表明,言语感知的日常变化不能简单地归因于测试的不可靠性,并为研究心理状态如何预测言语感知的变化铺平了道路。(PsycInfo数据库记录(c)2023 APA,保留所有权利)。
{"title":"Are there good days and bad days for hearing? Quantifying day-to-day intraindividual speech perception variability in older and younger adults.","authors":"Inka Kuhlmann,&nbsp;Giulia Angonese,&nbsp;Christiane Thiel,&nbsp;Birger Kollmeier,&nbsp;Andrea Hildebrandt","doi":"10.1037/xhp0001159","DOIUrl":"10.1037/xhp0001159","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Moment-to-moment variations in hearing and speech perception have long been observed. Depending on the researcher's theoretical position, the observed fluctuations have been attributed to measurement error or to internal, nonsensory factors such as fluctuations in attention. While cognitive performance has been shown to fluctuate from day to day over longer time, such fluctuations have not been quantified for speech perception, despite being well-recognized by clinical audiologists and hearing-impaired patients. In three studies, we aimed to explore and quantify the magnitude of daily variability in speech perception and to investigate whether such variability goes beyond test unreliability. We also asked whether intraindividual variability depends on overall speech perception performance as observed in different groups of individuals. Older adults with objective hearing impairment and mostly hearing aids (N₁ = 45), with subjective hearing problems but no hearing aids (N₂ = 113), and younger adults without hearing problems (N₃ = 20) participated in three ecological momentary assessment studies. They performed a digit-in-noise test two to three times a day for several weeks. Variance heterogeneous linear mixed-effects models indicated reliable intraindividual variability in speech perception and substantial individual differences in daily variability. A protective factor against daily fluctuations is a higher average speech perception. These studies show that day-to-day variations in speech perception cannot simply be attributed to test unreliability and pave the way for investigating how psychological states that do not vary from moment-to-moment, but rather from day to day, predict variations in speech perception. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2023 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":50195,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Human Perception and Performance","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.1,"publicationDate":"2023-11-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"49693508","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Emotional face expressions and group membership: Does affective mismatch induce conflict? 情感面部表情与群体成员关系:情感不匹配会引发冲突吗?
IF 2.1 3区 心理学 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2023-11-01 DOI: 10.1037/xhp0001163
Dirk Wentura, Emre Gurbuz, Andrea Paulus, Michaela Rohr

When asked to judge or react to a facial emotional display of a person, people do not only take the emotion into account, but also other socially important features of the face, such as, for example, ethnicity (Kozlik & Fischer, 2020; Paulus & Wentura, 2014). Importantly, the emotion-related and nonemotion-related features are seemingly not (or not always) processed in a simple, additive manner, but are-in a more functional manner-integrated to provide an "amalgamated signal" on which individuals base their judgment and responses. Whereas Paulus and Wentura (2014) put forward a social-message account of this amalgamated signal, Kozlik and Fischer (2020) recently proposed a processing-conflict explanation. The empirical evidence regarding this issue is, however, mixed. In three experiments, we aimed at replicating and extending Kozlik and Fischer's central experiment to gain further insight into the validity of the social-message versus the processing-conflict account. However, we failed to replicate their findings. The implications of the new evidence for the two accounts are discussed. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2023 APA, all rights reserved).

当被要求判断一个人的面部情绪表现或对其做出反应时,人们不仅会考虑情绪,还会考虑面部的其他社会重要特征,例如种族(Kozlik&Fischer,2020;Paulus&Wentura,2014)。重要的是,情绪相关和非运动相关的特征似乎不是(或不总是)以简单的相加方式进行处理,而是以更具功能性的方式进行整合,以提供一个“合并信号”,个体据此进行判断和反应。Paulus和Wentura(2014)提出了对这种合并信号的社会信息描述,而Kozlik和Fischer(2020)最近提出了处理冲突的解释。然而,关于这一问题的经验证据喜忧参半。在三个实验中,我们旨在复制和扩展Kozlik和Fischer的中心实验,以进一步深入了解社交信息与处理冲突账户的有效性。然而,我们未能复制他们的发现。讨论了新证据对这两种说法的影响。(PsycInfo数据库记录(c)2023 APA,保留所有权利)。
{"title":"Emotional face expressions and group membership: Does affective mismatch induce conflict?","authors":"Dirk Wentura,&nbsp;Emre Gurbuz,&nbsp;Andrea Paulus,&nbsp;Michaela Rohr","doi":"10.1037/xhp0001163","DOIUrl":"10.1037/xhp0001163","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>When asked to judge or react to a facial emotional display of a person, people do not only take the emotion into account, but also other socially important features of the face, such as, for example, ethnicity (Kozlik & Fischer, 2020; Paulus & Wentura, 2014). Importantly, the emotion-related and nonemotion-related features are seemingly not (or not always) processed in a simple, additive manner, but are-in a more functional manner-integrated to provide an \"amalgamated signal\" on which individuals base their judgment and responses. Whereas Paulus and Wentura (2014) put forward a social-message account of this amalgamated signal, Kozlik and Fischer (2020) recently proposed a processing-conflict explanation. The empirical evidence regarding this issue is, however, mixed. In three experiments, we aimed at replicating and extending Kozlik and Fischer's central experiment to gain further insight into the validity of the social-message versus the processing-conflict account. However, we failed to replicate their findings. The implications of the new evidence for the two accounts are discussed. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2023 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":50195,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Human Perception and Performance","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.1,"publicationDate":"2023-11-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"49693426","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Social norm learning alters feature-based visual attention: Evidence from steady-state visual evoked potentials. 社会规范学习改变了基于特征的视觉注意:来自稳态视觉诱发电位的证据。
IF 2.1 3区 心理学 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2023-11-01 Epub Date: 2023-09-28 DOI: 10.1037/xhp0001156
Markus Germar, Thorsten Albrecht, Andreas Mojzisch

Dating back to the seminal studies of Sherif (1935), there is robust evidence that social norm learning is able to shape perceptual decision making in a persistent manner. But what mechanisms underlie this effect? Here, we propose the new attentional alignment hypothesis. According to this hypothesis, norm learning alters feature-based visual attention. In particular, we hypothesize that norm learning increases visual attention toward norm-congruent relative to norm-incongruent stimulus features. Using steady-state visual evoked potentials, our results show for the first time that norm learning can persistently alter early attentional processes in the visual cortex. As predicted by the attentional alignment hypothesis, individuals' feature-based attention was tuned toward norm-congruent and away from norm-incongruent features. This bias persisted even when norm information was no longer available. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2023 APA, all rights reserved).

追溯到Sherif(1935)的开创性研究,有强有力的证据表明,社会规范学习能够以持续的方式塑造感知决策。但这种效应背后的机制是什么?在这里,我们提出了新的注意对齐假说。根据这一假设,规范学习改变了基于特征的视觉注意力。特别地,我们假设,相对于范数不一致的刺激特征,范数学习增加了对范数一致的视觉注意力。利用稳态视觉诱发电位,我们的研究结果首次表明,规范学习可以持续改变视觉皮层的早期注意过程。正如注意力一致假说所预测的那样,个体基于特征的注意力倾向于规范一致和远离规范不一致的特征。即使在规范信息不再可用的情况下,这种偏见仍然存在。(PsycInfo数据库记录(c)2023 APA,保留所有权利)。
{"title":"Social norm learning alters feature-based visual attention: Evidence from steady-state visual evoked potentials.","authors":"Markus Germar,&nbsp;Thorsten Albrecht,&nbsp;Andreas Mojzisch","doi":"10.1037/xhp0001156","DOIUrl":"10.1037/xhp0001156","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Dating back to the seminal studies of Sherif (1935), there is robust evidence that social norm learning is able to shape perceptual decision making in a persistent manner. But what mechanisms underlie this effect? Here, we propose the new attentional alignment hypothesis. According to this hypothesis, norm learning alters feature-based visual attention. In particular, we hypothesize that norm learning increases visual attention toward norm-congruent relative to norm-incongruent stimulus features. Using steady-state visual evoked potentials, our results show for the first time that norm learning can persistently alter early attentional processes in the visual cortex. As predicted by the attentional alignment hypothesis, individuals' feature-based attention was tuned toward norm-congruent and away from norm-incongruent features. This bias persisted even when norm information was no longer available. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2023 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":50195,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Human Perception and Performance","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.1,"publicationDate":"2023-11-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"41154237","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Depersonalization affects self-prioritization of bodily, but not abstract self-related information. 人格解体影响身体信息的自我优先级,但不影响抽象的自我相关信息。
IF 2.1 3区 心理学 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2023-11-01 DOI: 10.1037/xhp0001153
Mateusz Woźniak, Luke McEllin, Jakob Hohwy, Anna Ciaunica

Depersonalization is a common and distressing experience characterized by a feeling of estrangement from one's self, body, and the world. In order to examine the relationship between depersonalization and selfhood we conducted an experimental study comparing processing of three types of self-related information between nonclinical groups of people experiencing high and low levels of depersonalization. Using a sequential matching task, we compared three types of biases for processing of self-related information: prioritization of one's name, of self-associated abstract stimuli (geometrical shapes), and of self-associated bodily stimuli (avatar faces). We found that both groups demonstrated the standard pattern of results for self-prioritization of one's name and geometrical shapes, but they differed with regard to avatar faces. While people with low depersonalization showed the standard prioritization of avatar faces, people with high depersonalization showed overall better response accuracy with avatar faces, and faster response times for stranger-associated avatar faces. These results were complemented by the additional finding that people with high depersonalization reported being more likely to use avatars of a different gender to their own outside of the experimental context. Finally, in this large sample (N = 180) we investigated the relationships between different measures of self-related processing and self-identification, finding no correlation between explicit reports of self-identification with self-associated avatar faces and geometrical shapes, self-prioritization of these stimuli, and prioritization of one's name. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2023 APA, all rights reserved).

人格解体是一种常见而痛苦的经历,其特点是与自我、身体和世界产生隔阂。为了检验人格解体和自我之间的关系,我们进行了一项实验研究,比较了经历高水平和低水平人格解体的非临床人群对三种类型的自我相关信息的处理。使用顺序匹配任务,我们比较了处理自我相关信息的三种类型的偏见:名字的优先顺序、自我相关的抽象刺激(几何形状)和自我相关的身体刺激(化身面部)。我们发现,这两组人都表现出了对自己的名字和几何形状进行自我排序的标准结果模式,但在化身人脸方面有所不同。虽然去个性化程度低的人显示出化身人脸的标准优先级,但去个性化程度高的人总体上对化身人脸的反应准确性更好,对陌生人相关的化身人脸的响应时间更快。这些结果得到了另一项发现的补充,即高度人格解体的人报告说,在实验环境之外,更有可能使用与自己不同性别的化身。最后,在这个大样本(N=180)中,我们调查了自我相关处理和自我识别的不同测量之间的关系,发现自我识别的明确报告与自我相关的化身面孔和几何形状、这些刺激的自我优先级和名字的优先级之间没有相关性。(PsycInfo数据库记录(c)2023 APA,保留所有权利)。
{"title":"Depersonalization affects self-prioritization of bodily, but not abstract self-related information.","authors":"Mateusz Woźniak,&nbsp;Luke McEllin,&nbsp;Jakob Hohwy,&nbsp;Anna Ciaunica","doi":"10.1037/xhp0001153","DOIUrl":"10.1037/xhp0001153","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Depersonalization is a common and distressing experience characterized by a feeling of estrangement from one's self, body, and the world. In order to examine the relationship between depersonalization and selfhood we conducted an experimental study comparing processing of three types of self-related information between nonclinical groups of people experiencing high and low levels of depersonalization. Using a sequential matching task, we compared three types of biases for processing of self-related information: prioritization of one's name, of self-associated abstract stimuli (geometrical shapes), and of self-associated bodily stimuli (avatar faces). We found that both groups demonstrated the standard pattern of results for self-prioritization of one's name and geometrical shapes, but they differed with regard to avatar faces. While people with low depersonalization showed the standard prioritization of avatar faces, people with high depersonalization showed overall better response accuracy with avatar faces, and faster response times for stranger-associated avatar faces. These results were complemented by the additional finding that people with high depersonalization reported being more likely to use avatars of a different gender to their own outside of the experimental context. Finally, in this large sample (N = 180) we investigated the relationships between different measures of self-related processing and self-identification, finding no correlation between explicit reports of self-identification with self-associated avatar faces and geometrical shapes, self-prioritization of these stimuli, and prioritization of one's name. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2023 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":50195,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Human Perception and Performance","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.1,"publicationDate":"2023-11-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"49693425","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Contextual effects on duration perception are modality-specific. 情境对持续时间感知的影响是模态特有的。
IF 2.1 3区 心理学 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2023-11-01 DOI: 10.1037/xhp0001164
Nahal Binur, Bat-Sheva Hadad

The perception of magnitude, crucial for a mental representation of the physical world, is often subject to significant biases. Many of these biases are similar across sensory modalities, implying a generalized perception of magnitude. At the same time, some physical magnitudes might have a dedicated modality-specific calibration mechanism to enhance perceptual sensitivity. We examined this question of generalized versus modality-specific processes testing between- and within-modalities' contextual effects on the perception of magnitude. In a constant stimuli procedure, a central standard was embedded in shorter and longer contextual standards. These contextual standards were sampled in either a relatively wider or narrower range of durations. Participants were asked to determine which of the two consecutive durations was longer. Better perceptual sensitivity was found for narrower contexts, with stronger effects in trials in which the standard was presented first. Interestingly, narrower context enhanced sensitivity for standards within the same modality but had no effect on standards of another modality. A unidirectional transfer of contextual effects was observed under certain conditions from auditory, the dominant modality in performing temporal judgments, to vision. These results suggest that the perceptual system appears to develop modality-specific calibration mechanisms, most likely, to enhance perceptual sensitivity and maintain sensory specialization. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2023 APA, all rights reserved).

对于物理世界的心理表征至关重要的量级感知,往往会受到重大偏见的影响。这些偏差中的许多在感觉模式上是相似的,这意味着对幅度的普遍感知。同时,一些物理幅度可能具有专用的模态特定校准机制,以增强感知灵敏度。我们研究了在模态之间和模态内部测试广义与模态特定过程对幅度感知的上下文影响的问题。在持续刺激过程中,一个中心标准嵌入到更短和更长的上下文标准中。这些上下文标准是在相对较宽或较窄的持续时间范围内采样的。参与者被要求确定两个连续的持续时间中哪一个更长。在较窄的环境中发现了更好的感知敏感性,在首次提出该标准的试验中效果更强。有趣的是,较窄的语境增强了对同一模式中标准的敏感性,但对另一模式的标准没有影响。在某些条件下,观察到上下文效应从听觉(执行时间判断的主要模态)到视觉的单向转移。这些结果表明,感知系统似乎发展了特定于模态的校准机制,很可能是为了增强感知灵敏度和保持感觉专业化。(PsycInfo数据库记录(c)2023 APA,保留所有权利)。
{"title":"Contextual effects on duration perception are modality-specific.","authors":"Nahal Binur,&nbsp;Bat-Sheva Hadad","doi":"10.1037/xhp0001164","DOIUrl":"10.1037/xhp0001164","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The perception of magnitude, crucial for a mental representation of the physical world, is often subject to significant biases. Many of these biases are similar across sensory modalities, implying a generalized perception of magnitude. At the same time, some physical magnitudes might have a dedicated modality-specific calibration mechanism to enhance perceptual sensitivity. We examined this question of generalized versus modality-specific processes testing between- and within-modalities' contextual effects on the perception of magnitude. In a constant stimuli procedure, a central standard was embedded in shorter and longer contextual standards. These contextual standards were sampled in either a relatively wider or narrower range of durations. Participants were asked to determine which of the two consecutive durations was longer. Better perceptual sensitivity was found for narrower contexts, with stronger effects in trials in which the standard was presented first. Interestingly, narrower context enhanced sensitivity for standards within the same modality but had no effect on standards of another modality. A unidirectional transfer of contextual effects was observed under certain conditions from auditory, the dominant modality in performing temporal judgments, to vision. These results suggest that the perceptual system appears to develop modality-specific calibration mechanisms, most likely, to enhance perceptual sensitivity and maintain sensory specialization. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2023 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":50195,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Human Perception and Performance","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.1,"publicationDate":"2023-11-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"49693509","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Exploring the response code in a compatibility effect between physical size and left/right responses: The hand is more important than location. 探索身体大小和左右反应之间的兼容性效应中的反应代码:手比位置更重要。
IF 2.1 3区 心理学 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2023-11-01 DOI: 10.1037/xhp0001162
Christian Seegelke, Melanie Richter, Tobias Heed, Peter Wühr

The spatial-size association of response codes (SSARC) effect refers to the finding of better performance with the left hand to small stimuli and with the right hand to large stimuli, as compared to the reverse mapping. In the present study, we investigated which response coding is responsible for the emergence of the SSARC effect. We observed a SSARC effect only with response selection between hands but not between fingers of one hand, indicating that the responses are coded relative to the body midline. Furthermore, we observed a SSARC effect with parallel arms but not with crossed arms, suggesting that both the anatomical side of the effector and its external spatial position contribute to the response code. However, using a reaching task as compared to keypresses, the SSARC effect followed the arms, suggesting that the crucial spatial response code refers more strongly to the anatomical side of the effector rather than to the external spatial response position. These findings document a strong influence of anatomically- or body-based coding on the SSARC effect, are at odds with the proposition of a generalized magnitude system that utilizes a common, external spatial metric, and point toward a categorical nature of response codes underlying the SSARC effect. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2023 APA, all rights reserved).

响应码的空间大小关联(SSARC)效应是指与反向映射相比,左手对小刺激和右手对大刺激表现更好。在本研究中,我们研究了哪种反应编码是SSARC效应出现的原因。我们观察到SSARC效应仅与手之间的反应选择有关,而与一只手的手指之间的反应无关,这表明反应是相对于身体中线编码的。此外,我们观察到平行臂而非交叉臂的SSARC效应,这表明效应器的解剖侧及其外部空间位置都有助于响应代码。然而,与按键相比,使用到达任务时,SSARC效应跟随手臂,这表明关键的空间响应代码更强烈地指向效应器的解剖侧,而不是外部空间响应位置。这些发现记录了基于解剖学或身体的编码对SSARC效应的强烈影响,与利用通用外部空间度量的广义震级系统的主张不一致,并指出了SSARC效应背后的响应代码的分类性质。(PsycInfo数据库记录(c)2023 APA,保留所有权利)。
{"title":"Exploring the response code in a compatibility effect between physical size and left/right responses: The hand is more important than location.","authors":"Christian Seegelke,&nbsp;Melanie Richter,&nbsp;Tobias Heed,&nbsp;Peter Wühr","doi":"10.1037/xhp0001162","DOIUrl":"10.1037/xhp0001162","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The spatial-size association of response codes (SSARC) effect refers to the finding of better performance with the left hand to small stimuli and with the right hand to large stimuli, as compared to the reverse mapping. In the present study, we investigated which response coding is responsible for the emergence of the SSARC effect. We observed a SSARC effect only with response selection between hands but not between fingers of one hand, indicating that the responses are coded relative to the body midline. Furthermore, we observed a SSARC effect with parallel arms but not with crossed arms, suggesting that both the anatomical side of the effector and its external spatial position contribute to the response code. However, using a reaching task as compared to keypresses, the SSARC effect followed the arms, suggesting that the crucial spatial response code refers more strongly to the anatomical side of the effector rather than to the external spatial response position. These findings document a strong influence of anatomically- or body-based coding on the SSARC effect, are at odds with the proposition of a generalized magnitude system that utilizes a common, external spatial metric, and point toward a categorical nature of response codes underlying the SSARC effect. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2023 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":50195,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Human Perception and Performance","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.1,"publicationDate":"2023-11-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"49693427","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
Revealing object-based cognitive control in a moving object paradigm. 揭示运动对象范式中基于对象的认知控制。
IF 2.1 3区 心理学 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2023-11-01 DOI: 10.1037/xhp0001158
Jackson S Colvett, Blaire J Weidler, Julie M Bugg

Object-based attention and flexible adjustments of cognitive control based on contextual cues signaling the likelihood of distraction are well documented. However, no prior research has conclusively demonstrated that people flexibly adjust cognitive control to minimize distraction based on learned associations between task-irrelevant objects and distraction likelihood (i.e., object-based cognitive control). To fill this gap, we developed a novel paradigm during which participants responded to flanker stimuli appearing in one of multiple locations on two simultaneously presented objects. One object predicted a low likelihood of encountering an incongruent flanker stimulus and the other a high likelihood. After each response, the objects rotated clockwise such that all locations on average were 50% congruent, thereby eliminating confounds between location and likelihood of incongruence. Object-based cognitive control was evidenced by reduced flanker compatibility effects in the high compared to low conflict object. Across four experiments, we demonstrated that object-based cognitive control was dependent on a strong manipulation of the likelihood of conflict between objects and movement of the objects between trials. The novel evidence for object-based cognitive control is important in showing that people exploit not only location as a cue to guide control, but additionally objects, mirroring evidence on object and location-based attention. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2023 APA, all rights reserved).

基于对象的注意力和基于上下文线索的认知控制的灵活调整表明了分心的可能性。然而,先前没有任何研究最终证明,人们可以根据与任务无关的对象和分心可能性之间的习得关联(即基于对象的认知控制),灵活地调整认知控制,以最大限度地减少分心。为了填补这一空白,我们开发了一种新的范式,在这种范式中,参与者对出现在两个同时出现的物体的多个位置之一的侧卫刺激做出反应。一个物体预测遇到不协调的侧卫刺激的可能性很低,另一个则预测遇到不一致的侧卫激励的可能性很高。每次响应后,对象顺时针旋转,使所有位置平均一致50%,从而消除位置和不一致可能性之间的混淆。与低冲突对象相比,高冲突对象的侧卫兼容性效应降低,证明了基于对象的认知控制。在四个实验中,我们证明了基于对象的认知控制依赖于对对象之间冲突的可能性和对象在试验之间移动的强烈操纵。基于对象的认知控制的新证据很重要,表明人们不仅利用位置作为线索来引导控制,而且还利用对象,反映了关于对象和基于位置的注意力的证据。(PsycInfo数据库记录(c)2023 APA,保留所有权利)。
{"title":"Revealing object-based cognitive control in a moving object paradigm.","authors":"Jackson S Colvett,&nbsp;Blaire J Weidler,&nbsp;Julie M Bugg","doi":"10.1037/xhp0001158","DOIUrl":"10.1037/xhp0001158","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Object-based attention and flexible adjustments of cognitive control based on contextual cues signaling the likelihood of distraction are well documented. However, no prior research has conclusively demonstrated that people flexibly adjust cognitive control to minimize distraction based on learned associations between task-irrelevant objects and distraction likelihood (i.e., object-based cognitive control). To fill this gap, we developed a novel paradigm during which participants responded to flanker stimuli appearing in one of multiple locations on two simultaneously presented objects. One object predicted a low likelihood of encountering an incongruent flanker stimulus and the other a high likelihood. After each response, the objects rotated clockwise such that all locations on average were 50% congruent, thereby eliminating confounds between location and likelihood of incongruence. Object-based cognitive control was evidenced by reduced flanker compatibility effects in the high compared to low conflict object. Across four experiments, we demonstrated that object-based cognitive control was dependent on a strong manipulation of the likelihood of conflict between objects and movement of the objects between trials. The novel evidence for object-based cognitive control is important in showing that people exploit not only location as a cue to guide control, but additionally objects, mirroring evidence on object and location-based attention. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2023 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":50195,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Human Perception and Performance","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.1,"publicationDate":"2023-11-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"49693428","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
When "looking at nothing" imparts something: Retrospective gaze cues flexibly direct prioritization in visual working memory. 当“什么都不看”赋予某些东西时:回顾性凝视暗示了视觉工作记忆中灵活的直接优先顺序。
IF 2.1 3区 心理学 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2023-11-01 DOI: 10.1037/xhp0001160
Yingchao Zhang, Shujuan Ye, Wei Chen, Xiaowei Ding

Previous research has shown that retrospective gaze cues direct attention to internally maintained representations in visual working memory (vWM). Here, we aimed to differentiate the dual nature of gaze and accordingly proposed two hypotheses regarding the gaze-induced prioritization in vWM. The directional cueing hypothesis claims a constant attentional shifting to the gazed-at direction. By contrast, the referential cueing hypothesis proposes that gaze cues selectively orient attention toward their referents. To test these hypotheses, we employed an adapted change-detection task wherein gaze cues were presented during the retention interval. Critically, the cue character was positioned between two barriers, which could be either opaque (the blocked condition) or transparent (the unblocked condition). Polygons previously presented at the gazed-at (vs. gazed-away) location were better memorized, but not when the visual perspective of the character was obstructed (i.e., the blocked condition, Experiment 1). Subsequent experiments demonstrated that physical motion cues (Experiment 2) and inverted face cues (Experiment 3), which disrupted the extraction of referential signals, were immune to barrier settings. In Experiment 4, we generalize this selective cueing effect to faces with fearful expressions. These consistent findings support the referential cueing hypothesis and emphasize the distinctiveness of social attention. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2023 APA, all rights reserved).

先前的研究表明,回顾性凝视线索将注意力引导到视觉工作记忆(vWM)中内部保持的表征上。在这里,我们旨在区分凝视的双重性质,并相应地提出了关于vWM中凝视诱导的优先级的两个假设。定向提示假说声称注意力不断地向凝视的方向转移。相比之下,指称线索假说提出,凝视线索有选择地将注意力导向其指称。为了检验这些假设,我们采用了一种自适应的变化检测任务,其中在保持间隔期间呈现凝视线索。至关重要的是,提示字符位于两个屏障之间,这两个屏障可以是不透明的(阻塞条件)或透明的(未阻塞条件)。先前出现在凝视(与凝视远离)位置的多边形被更好地记忆,但当角色的视觉视角被遮挡时(即,遮挡条件,实验1)则不会。随后的实验表明,干扰参考信号提取的身体运动线索(实验2)和倒置面部线索(实验3)对屏障设置免疫。在实验4中,我们将这种选择性提示效应推广到有恐惧表情的人脸上。这些一致的发现支持了指称提示假说,并强调了社会关注的独特性。(PsycInfo数据库记录(c)2023 APA,保留所有权利)。
{"title":"When \"looking at nothing\" imparts something: Retrospective gaze cues flexibly direct prioritization in visual working memory.","authors":"Yingchao Zhang,&nbsp;Shujuan Ye,&nbsp;Wei Chen,&nbsp;Xiaowei Ding","doi":"10.1037/xhp0001160","DOIUrl":"10.1037/xhp0001160","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Previous research has shown that retrospective gaze cues direct attention to internally maintained representations in visual working memory (vWM). Here, we aimed to differentiate the dual nature of gaze and accordingly proposed two hypotheses regarding the gaze-induced prioritization in vWM. The directional cueing hypothesis claims a constant attentional shifting to the gazed-at direction. By contrast, the referential cueing hypothesis proposes that gaze cues selectively orient attention toward their referents. To test these hypotheses, we employed an adapted change-detection task wherein gaze cues were presented during the retention interval. Critically, the cue character was positioned between two barriers, which could be either opaque (the blocked condition) or transparent (the unblocked condition). Polygons previously presented at the gazed-at (vs. gazed-away) location were better memorized, but not when the visual perspective of the character was obstructed (i.e., the blocked condition, Experiment 1). Subsequent experiments demonstrated that physical motion cues (Experiment 2) and inverted face cues (Experiment 3), which disrupted the extraction of referential signals, were immune to barrier settings. In Experiment 4, we generalize this selective cueing effect to faces with fearful expressions. These consistent findings support the referential cueing hypothesis and emphasize the distinctiveness of social attention. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2023 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":50195,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Human Perception and Performance","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.1,"publicationDate":"2023-11-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"49693430","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Journal of Experimental Psychology-Human Perception and Performance
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1