首页 > 最新文献

Journal of Experimental Psychology-Human Perception and Performance最新文献

英文 中文
Proactive suppression is an implicit process that cannot be summoned on demand. 主动抑制是一个隐性过程,无法按需召唤。
IF 2.1 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-06-01 Epub Date: 2024-04-15 DOI: 10.1037/xhp0001206
Christopher Hauck, Eric Ruthruff, Mei-Ching Lien

We examined whether proactive suppression can be applied on demand. A prompt cue indicated the to-be-ignored distractor color for each trial. Participants needed to use this cue to know which of two target shapes to respond to. To assess proactive suppression of the cued distractor color, we presented a probe letter recall task on a minority (25%) of the trials. A letter appeared inside each of the six shapes of the search array and participants recalled as many letters as they could. When the to-be-ignored color was fixed in Experiment 1, probe recall accuracy was lower for probe letters inside to-be-ignored-color distractors than target-color distractors, known as the probe suppression effect. However, when the prompted to-be-ignored color varied from trial to trial, the probe suppression effect disappeared, regardless of whether the prompt was a colored circle (Experiment 2) or a colored word (Experiment 3). Experiment 4 tested the search and destroy hypothesis by shortening the search display duration from 200 to 50 ms. No capture effect by the to-be-ignored color was evident, suggesting that participants did not first search for the to-be-ignored color, prior to suppressing it. We conclude that when rejection of a distractor color is required on demand, one cannot accomplish such suppression proactively but instead must deal with the distractor reactively, incurring a large cost in performance. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

我们研究了主动抑制是否可以按需应用。提示线索显示了每次试验中要忽略的分心颜色。受试者需要通过这一提示才能知道应该对两个目标形状中的哪一个做出反应。为了评估对提示分心颜色的主动抑制,我们在少数(25%)的试验中提出了探究字母回忆任务。在搜索阵列的六个形状中,每个形状内都出现了一个字母,参与者尽可能多地回忆字母。在实验 1 中,当要忽略的颜色固定不变时,要忽略的颜色干扰物内的探针字母的回忆准确率低于目标颜色干扰物内的探针字母的回忆准确率,这就是探针抑制效应。然而,当提示的待忽略颜色在不同试验中变化时,探针抑制效应消失了,无论提示是彩色圆圈(实验 2)还是彩色单词(实验 3)。实验 4 通过将搜索显示时间从 200 毫秒缩短到 50 毫秒来测试搜索和破坏假说。实验结果显示,被试并没有明显地捕捉到要被忽略的颜色,这表明被试在抑制要被忽略的颜色之前,并没有首先搜索要被忽略的颜色。我们的结论是,当需要按需拒绝分心颜色时,人无法主动完成这种抑制,而必须被动地处理分心颜色,这就会在成绩上付出很大代价。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Proactive suppression is an implicit process that cannot be summoned on demand.","authors":"Christopher Hauck, Eric Ruthruff, Mei-Ching Lien","doi":"10.1037/xhp0001206","DOIUrl":"10.1037/xhp0001206","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>We examined whether proactive suppression can be applied on demand. A prompt cue indicated the to-be-ignored distractor color for each trial. Participants needed to use this cue to know which of two target shapes to respond to. To assess proactive suppression of the cued distractor color, we presented a probe letter recall task on a minority (25%) of the trials. A letter appeared inside each of the six shapes of the search array and participants recalled as many letters as they could. When the to-be-ignored color was fixed in Experiment 1, probe recall accuracy was lower for probe letters inside to-be-ignored-color distractors than target-color distractors, known as the probe suppression effect. However, when the prompted to-be-ignored color varied from trial to trial, the probe suppression effect disappeared, regardless of whether the prompt was a colored circle (Experiment 2) or a colored word (Experiment 3). Experiment 4 tested the search and destroy hypothesis by shortening the search display duration from 200 to 50 ms. No capture effect by the to-be-ignored color was evident, suggesting that participants did not first search for the to-be-ignored color, prior to suppressing it. We conclude that when rejection of a distractor color is required on demand, one cannot accomplish such suppression proactively but instead must deal with the distractor reactively, incurring a large cost in performance. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":50195,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Human Perception and Performance","volume":" ","pages":"636-653"},"PeriodicalIF":2.1,"publicationDate":"2024-06-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140853350","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
No evidence in favor of the existence of "intentional" binding. 没有证据证明存在 "有意 "约束。
IF 2.1 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-06-01 Epub Date: 2024-04-18 DOI: 10.1037/xhp0001204
Gaiqing Kong, Cheryne Aberkane, Clément Desoche, Alessandro Farnè, Marine Vernet

Intentional binding refers to the subjective temporal compression between a voluntary action and its subsequent sensory outcome. Despite some studies challenging the link between temporal compression and intentional action, intentional binding is still widely used as an implicit measure for the sense of agency. The debate remains unsettled primarily because the experimental conditions used in previous studies were confounded with various alternative causes for temporal compression, and action intention has not yet been tested comprehensively against all potential alternative causes in a single study. Here, we solve this puzzle by jointly comparing participants' estimates of the interval between three types of triggering events with comparable predictability-voluntary movement, passive movement, and external sensory event-and an external sensory outcome (auditory or visual across experiments). The results failed to show intentional binding, that is, no shorter interval estimation for the voluntary than the passive movement conditions. Instead, we observed temporal (but not intentional) binding when comparing both movement conditions with the external sensory condition. Thus, temporal binding appears to originate from sensory integration and temporal prediction, not from action intention. As such, these findings underscore the need to reconsider the use of "intentional binding" as a reliable proxy of the sense of agency. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

意向束缚指的是自愿行动与随后的感官结果之间的主观时间压缩。尽管一些研究对时间压缩与有意行动之间的联系提出了质疑,但有意束缚仍被广泛用作衡量代理感的隐性指标。这一争论之所以悬而未决,主要是因为以往研究中使用的实验条件与时间压缩的各种替代原因相混淆,而行动意向尚未在一项研究中针对所有潜在的替代原因进行全面测试。在这里,我们通过联合比较参与者对三类具有可比预测性的触发事件--自主运动、被动运动和外部感觉事件--与外部感觉结果(听觉或视觉,不同实验中均有)之间的时间间隔的估计,解决了这一难题。结果未能显示出有意结合,也就是说,自主运动条件下的时间间隔估计没有比被动运动条件下的时间间隔估计短。相反,在将两种运动条件与外部感觉条件进行比较时,我们观察到了时间上的(但不是有意的)约束。因此,时间束缚似乎源于感觉整合和时间预测,而非行动意图。因此,这些发现强调有必要重新考虑使用 "意向束缚 "作为代理感的可靠代表。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved)。
{"title":"No evidence in favor of the existence of \"intentional\" binding.","authors":"Gaiqing Kong, Cheryne Aberkane, Clément Desoche, Alessandro Farnè, Marine Vernet","doi":"10.1037/xhp0001204","DOIUrl":"10.1037/xhp0001204","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Intentional binding refers to the subjective temporal compression between a voluntary action and its subsequent sensory outcome. Despite some studies challenging the link between temporal compression and intentional action, intentional binding is still widely used as an implicit measure for the sense of agency. The debate remains unsettled primarily because the experimental conditions used in previous studies were confounded with various alternative causes for temporal compression, and action intention has not yet been tested comprehensively against all potential alternative causes in a single study. Here, we solve this puzzle by jointly comparing participants' estimates of the interval between three types of triggering events with comparable predictability-voluntary movement, passive movement, and external sensory event-and an external sensory outcome (auditory or visual across experiments). The results failed to show intentional binding, that is, no shorter interval estimation for the voluntary than the passive movement conditions. Instead, we observed temporal (but not intentional) binding when comparing both movement conditions with the external sensory condition. Thus, temporal binding appears to originate from sensory integration and temporal prediction, not from action intention. As such, these findings underscore the need to reconsider the use of \"intentional binding\" as a reliable proxy of the sense of agency. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":50195,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Human Perception and Performance","volume":" ","pages":"626-635"},"PeriodicalIF":2.1,"publicationDate":"2024-06-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140873268","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Gravity's impact on visual search asymmetries: Is visual gravitational motion a distinct visual feature or a familiar dynamic event? 重力对视觉搜索不对称的影响:视觉引力运动是一种独特的视觉特征还是一种熟悉的动态事件?
IF 2.1 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-05-01 Epub Date: 2024-02-29 DOI: 10.1037/xhp0001195
Beatriz Velado, Beatriz Santos Miranda, Francisca Almeida Oliveira, Henrique Marques de Oliveira, Sergio Delle Monache, Nuno De Sá Teixeira

A wealth of converging research lines has led support to the notion that specialized neural processes output a priori information about the expected effects of gravity to fine-tune motor and perceptual responses to dynamic events. Arguably, these putative internal models of gravity might modulate the efficiency in visual search for objects conforming or not to gravitationally coherent dynamics. In the present work, we explored this possibility with a visual search task involving arrays of two to eight objects moving periodically back and forth. The target could be an accelerating/decelerating ball (as if bouncing on earth's surface-1g) with distractors moving at a constant speed (0g) or the reverse. Moreover, the direction of the gravitational pull, as implied by the 1g motion patterns, could be aligned or misaligned with Earth's gravity. Overall, searches for 1g targets were more efficient than 0g targets except, notably, when stimuli displays were congruent with Earth's gravitational pull, in which case the visual search asymmetry is significantly reduced. Outcomes are interpreted as reflecting the joint and mutually cancelling contribution of low-level detection of acceleration patterns and higher level detection of unexpected violations of gravitational motion. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

大量汇集在一起的研究线索支持这样一种观点,即专门的神经过程输出有关重力预期效应的先验信息,以微调对动态事件的运动和知觉反应。可以说,这些假定的重力内部模型可能会调节视觉搜索符合或不符合重力一致性动态的物体的效率。在本研究中,我们通过一项视觉搜索任务来探索这种可能性,该任务涉及 2 到 8 个周期性来回移动的物体阵列。目标可以是一个加速/减速的球(就像在地球表面弹跳一样-1g),分心物以恒定的速度(0g)或相反的速度运动。此外,1g 运动模式所暗示的引力方向可能与地球引力方向一致或不一致。总体而言,搜索 1g 目标比搜索 0g 目标更有效率,但当刺激显示与地球引力一致时除外,在这种情况下,视觉搜索的不对称性会显著降低。结果被解释为反映了对加速度模式的低级检测和对意外违反重力运动的高级检测的共同和相互抵消的贡献。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Gravity's impact on visual search asymmetries: Is visual gravitational motion a distinct visual feature or a familiar dynamic event?","authors":"Beatriz Velado, Beatriz Santos Miranda, Francisca Almeida Oliveira, Henrique Marques de Oliveira, Sergio Delle Monache, Nuno De Sá Teixeira","doi":"10.1037/xhp0001195","DOIUrl":"10.1037/xhp0001195","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>A wealth of converging research lines has led support to the notion that specialized neural processes output a priori information about the expected effects of gravity to fine-tune motor and perceptual responses to dynamic events. Arguably, these putative internal models of gravity might modulate the efficiency in visual search for objects conforming or not to gravitationally coherent dynamics. In the present work, we explored this possibility with a visual search task involving arrays of two to eight objects moving periodically back and forth. The target could be an accelerating/decelerating ball (as if bouncing on earth's surface-1g) with distractors moving at a constant speed (0g) or the reverse. Moreover, the direction of the gravitational pull, as implied by the 1g motion patterns, could be aligned or misaligned with Earth's gravity. Overall, searches for 1g targets were more efficient than 0g targets except, notably, when stimuli displays were congruent with Earth's gravitational pull, in which case the visual search asymmetry is significantly reduced. Outcomes are interpreted as reflecting the joint and mutually cancelling contribution of low-level detection of acceleration patterns and higher level detection of unexpected violations of gravitational motion. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":50195,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Human Perception and Performance","volume":" ","pages":"451-463"},"PeriodicalIF":2.1,"publicationDate":"2024-05-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139998066","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Semantic facilitation in blocked picture categorization: Some data and considerations regarding task selection. 阻塞图片分类中的语义促进:有关任务选择的一些数据和考虑因素
IF 2.1 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-05-01 Epub Date: 2024-03-28 DOI: 10.1037/xhp0001203
Stefan Wöhner, Jana Luckow, Miriam Brandt, Jens Stahlmann, Annika Werwach, Jörg D Jescheniak

Semantic context effects in picture naming and categorization tasks are central to the development and evaluation of current models of word production. When pictures are named in a semantically blocked context, response latencies are delayed. Belke (2013) found that when the naming task was replaced with a semantic categorization task (natural vs. man-made), response latencies were facilitated. From this pattern, she concluded that semantic interference in blocked picture naming has its locus at the lexical level but its origin at the preceding semantic level. However, other studies using the blocking procedure have failed to find facilitation in semantic categorization tasks (Damian et al., 2001; Riley et al., 2015), calling this conclusion into question. In three blocked picture naming and categorization experiments, we investigated different variables that might account for the discrepant results in semantic categorization. We used different semantic categorization tasks, different response modalities, different response set sizes, and different blocking procedures. Semantic facilitation was reliably found in naturalness categorization, but there was no semantic effect in natural size categorization. We discuss the implications of these findings for appropriate task selection. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

图片命名和分类任务中的语义上下文效应是开发和评估当前单词生成模型的核心。当图片在语义受阻的语境中被命名时,反应潜伏期会延迟。Belke(2013 年)发现,当命名任务换成语义分类任务(自然与人造)时,反应潜伏期会加快。根据这一模式,她得出结论:阻断式图片命名中的语义干扰位于词汇层面,但起源于之前的语义层面。然而,其他使用阻断程序的研究未能在语义分类任务中发现促进作用(Damian 等人,2001 年;Riley 等人,2015 年),从而对这一结论提出了质疑。在三个阻断图片命名和分类实验中,我们研究了可能导致语义分类结果差异的不同变量。我们使用了不同的语义分类任务、不同的反应模式、不同的反应集大小和不同的阻断程序。在自然度分类中发现了可靠的语义促进作用,但在自然尺寸分类中却没有发现语义效应。我们讨论了这些发现对适当任务选择的影响。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Semantic facilitation in blocked picture categorization: Some data and considerations regarding task selection.","authors":"Stefan Wöhner, Jana Luckow, Miriam Brandt, Jens Stahlmann, Annika Werwach, Jörg D Jescheniak","doi":"10.1037/xhp0001203","DOIUrl":"10.1037/xhp0001203","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Semantic context effects in picture naming and categorization tasks are central to the development and evaluation of current models of word production. When pictures are named in a semantically blocked context, response latencies are delayed. Belke (2013) found that when the naming task was replaced with a semantic categorization task (natural vs. man-made), response latencies were facilitated. From this pattern, she concluded that semantic interference in blocked picture naming has its locus at the lexical level but its origin at the preceding semantic level. However, other studies using the blocking procedure have failed to find facilitation in semantic categorization tasks (Damian et al., 2001; Riley et al., 2015), calling this conclusion into question. In three blocked picture naming and categorization experiments, we investigated different variables that might account for the discrepant results in semantic categorization. We used different semantic categorization tasks, different response modalities, different response set sizes, and different blocking procedures. Semantic facilitation was reliably found in naturalness categorization, but there was no semantic effect in natural size categorization. We discuss the implications of these findings for appropriate task selection. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":50195,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Human Perception and Performance","volume":" ","pages":"515-530"},"PeriodicalIF":2.1,"publicationDate":"2024-05-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140307675","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
It is a match! Timely response to a specific target boosts concurrent task processing. 这是一种匹配!对特定目标的及时反应可促进同时进行的任务处理。
IF 2.1 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-05-01 Epub Date: 2024-04-04 DOI: 10.1037/xhp0001199
Yi Ni Toh, Vanessa G Lee

Multitasking typically leads to interference. However, responding to attentionally demanding targets in a continuous task paradoxically enhances memory for concurrently presented images, known as the "attentional boost effect" (ABE). Previous research has attributed the ABE to a temporal orienting response induced by the release of norepinephrine from the locus coeruleus when a stimulus is classified as a target. In this study, we tested whether target classification and response decisions act in an all-or-none manner on the ABE, or whether the processes leading up to these decisions also modulate the ABE. Participants encoded objects into memory while monitoring a stream of letters and digits, pressing a key for target letters. To change the process leading to target classification, we asked participants to respond either to a specific target letter or an entire category of letters. To change the process leading to response, we asked participants to either respond immediately to the target or withhold the response until the appearance of the next stimulus. Despite successfully identifying the target and responding to it in all conditions, participants benefited less from target detection in category search than in exact search and less from delayed response than immediate response. These findings suggest that target and response decisions do not act in an all-or-none manner. Instead, the ABE and the temporal orienting response is sensitive to the speed of reaching a perceptual or response decision. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

多任务处理通常会导致干扰。然而,在连续任务中对注意力要求较高的目标做出反应,反而会增强对同时呈现的图像的记忆,这就是所谓的 "注意力增强效应"(ABE)。以往的研究认为,当刺激被归类为目标时,脑室释放去甲肾上腺素,从而诱发了时间定向反应。在这项研究中,我们测试了目标分类和反应决策是否以全或非的方式作用于 ABE,或者这些决策的前导过程是否也会调节 ABE。受试者在监测字母和数字流的同时将对象编码到记忆中,并按下目标字母的按键。为了改变导致目标分类的过程,我们要求参与者对特定的目标字母或整个字母类别做出反应。为了改变作出反应的过程,我们要求参与者要么立即对目标作出反应,要么在下一个刺激出现之前暂不作出反应。尽管受试者在所有条件下都能成功识别目标并做出反应,但在类别搜索中,受试者从目标检测中获益的程度低于精确搜索,而从延迟反应中获益的程度也低于立即反应。这些发现表明,目标和反应决策并不是以一种非此即彼的方式发挥作用的。相反,ABE和时间定向反应对达成知觉或反应决策的速度很敏感。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"It is a match! Timely response to a specific target boosts concurrent task processing.","authors":"Yi Ni Toh, Vanessa G Lee","doi":"10.1037/xhp0001199","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/xhp0001199","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Multitasking typically leads to interference. However, responding to attentionally demanding targets in a continuous task paradoxically enhances memory for concurrently presented images, known as the \"attentional boost effect\" (ABE). Previous research has attributed the ABE to a temporal orienting response induced by the release of norepinephrine from the locus coeruleus when a stimulus is classified as a target. In this study, we tested whether target classification and response decisions act in an all-or-none manner on the ABE, or whether the processes leading up to these decisions also modulate the ABE. Participants encoded objects into memory while monitoring a stream of letters and digits, pressing a key for target letters. To change the process leading to target classification, we asked participants to respond either to a specific target letter or an entire category of letters. To change the process leading to response, we asked participants to either respond immediately to the target or withhold the response until the appearance of the next stimulus. Despite successfully identifying the target and responding to it in all conditions, participants benefited less from target detection in category search than in exact search and less from delayed response than immediate response. These findings suggest that target and response decisions do not act in an all-or-none manner. Instead, the ABE and the temporal orienting response is sensitive to the speed of reaching a perceptual or response decision. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":50195,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Human Perception and Performance","volume":"50 5","pages":"498-514"},"PeriodicalIF":2.1,"publicationDate":"2024-05-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140854250","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Competition between parts and whole: A new approach to Chinese compound word processing. 部分与整体的竞争:中文复合词处理的新方法。
IF 2.1 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-05-01 Epub Date: 2024-03-28 DOI: 10.1037/xhp0001198
Qiwei Zhang, Kuan-Jung Huang, Xingshan Li

How compound words are processed remains a central question in research on Chinese reading. The Chinese reading model assumes that all possible words sharing characters are activated during word processing and these activated words compete for a winner (Li & Pollatsek, 2020). The present studies aimed to examine whether embedded component words compete with whole compound words in Chinese reading. In Study 1, we analyzed two existing lexical decision databases and revealed inhibitory effects of component-word frequency and facilitative effects of character frequency on the first components. In Study 2, we conducted two factorial experiments to further examine the effects of first component-word frequency, with character frequencies controlled. The results consistently indicated significant inhibitory effects of component-word frequency. Collectively, these findings support the theoretical proposition that both component words and compound words are activated and engage in competition during word processing. This provides a new approach to compound word processing in Chinese reading and a possible solution to mixed results of character frequency effects reported in the literature. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

如何处理复合词仍然是中文阅读研究的核心问题。汉语阅读模型假定,在词语处理过程中,所有可能的共用字词都会被激活,这些被激活的词会在竞争中胜出(Li & Pollatsek, 2020)。本研究旨在探讨在中文阅读中,嵌入成分词是否会与整个复合词竞争。在研究 1 中,我们分析了两个现有的词汇决策数据库,发现成分词频率对第一成分词有抑制作用,而字符频率对第一成分词有促进作用。在研究 2 中,我们进行了两个因子实验,在控制字频的情况下进一步考察了第一成分-词频的影响。结果一致表明,成分-词频具有明显的抑制作用。总之,这些研究结果支持了这样一个理论命题,即在单词处理过程中,成分词和复合词都会被激活并参与竞争。这为中文阅读中的复合词处理提供了一种新的方法,也为文献中报道的字频效应的混合结果提供了一种可能的解决方案。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved)。
{"title":"Competition between parts and whole: A new approach to Chinese compound word processing.","authors":"Qiwei Zhang, Kuan-Jung Huang, Xingshan Li","doi":"10.1037/xhp0001198","DOIUrl":"10.1037/xhp0001198","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>How compound words are processed remains a central question in research on Chinese reading. The Chinese reading model assumes that all possible words sharing characters are activated during word processing and these activated words compete for a winner (Li & Pollatsek, 2020). The present studies aimed to examine whether embedded component words compete with whole compound words in Chinese reading. In Study 1, we analyzed two existing lexical decision databases and revealed inhibitory effects of component-word frequency and facilitative effects of character frequency on the first components. In Study 2, we conducted two factorial experiments to further examine the effects of first component-word frequency, with character frequencies controlled. The results consistently indicated significant inhibitory effects of component-word frequency. Collectively, these findings support the theoretical proposition that both component words and compound words are activated and engage in competition during word processing. This provides a new approach to compound word processing in Chinese reading and a possible solution to mixed results of character frequency effects reported in the literature. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":50195,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Human Perception and Performance","volume":" ","pages":"479-497"},"PeriodicalIF":2.1,"publicationDate":"2024-05-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140307674","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
On the timing of overt attention deployment: Eye-movement evidence for the priority accumulation framework. 关于公开注意力部署的时机:优先权积累框架的眼动证据。
IF 2.1 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-05-01 Epub Date: 2024-02-29 DOI: 10.1037/xhp0001192
Daniel Toledano, Mor Sasi, Shlomit Yuval-Greenberg, Dominique Lamy

Most visual-search theories assume that our attention is automatically allocated to the location with the highest priority at any given moment. The Priority Accumulation Framework (PAF) challenges this assumption. It suggests that the priority weight at each location accumulates across sequential events and that evidence for the presence of action-relevant information contributes to determining when attention is deployed to the location with the highest accumulated priority. Here, we tested these hypotheses for overt attention by recording first saccades in a free-viewing spatial-cueing task. We manipulated search difficulty (Experiments 1 and 2) and cue salience (Experiment 2). Standard theories posit that when oculomotor capture by the cue occurs, it is initiated before the search display appears; therefore, these theories predict that the cue's impact on the distribution of first saccades should be independent of search difficulty but influenced by the cue's saliency. By contrast, PAF posits that the cue can bias competition later, after processing of the search display has already started, and therefore predicts that such late impact should increase with both search difficulty and cue salience. The results fully supported PAF's predictions. Our account suggests a distinction between attentional capture and attentional-priority bias that resolves enduring inconsistencies in the attentional-capture literature. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

大多数视觉搜索理论都认为,我们的注意力会在任何特定时刻自动分配到优先级最高的位置。优先权累积框架(PAF)挑战了这一假设。它认为,每个位置的优先权重会在连续事件中不断累积,而行动相关信息的存在有助于确定何时将注意力分配到优先权累积最高的位置。在这里,我们通过记录自由观察空间线索任务中的首次囊回来测试这些关于公开注意的假设。我们操纵了搜索难度(实验 1 和 2)和线索显著性(实验 2)。标准理论认为,当眼球运动被线索捕捉时,它是在搜索显示出现之前开始的;因此,这些理论预测线索对首次注视分布的影响应与搜索难度无关,但受线索显著性的影响。与此相反,PAF 则认为线索会在搜索显示的处理过程开始之后对竞争产生影响,因此预测这种晚期影响会随着搜索难度和线索显著性的增加而增加。结果完全支持 PAF 的预测。我们的解释提出了注意捕捉和注意优先偏差之间的区别,解决了注意捕捉文献中长期存在的不一致问题。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"On the timing of overt attention deployment: Eye-movement evidence for the priority accumulation framework.","authors":"Daniel Toledano, Mor Sasi, Shlomit Yuval-Greenberg, Dominique Lamy","doi":"10.1037/xhp0001192","DOIUrl":"10.1037/xhp0001192","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Most visual-search theories assume that our attention is automatically allocated to the location with the highest priority at any given moment. The Priority Accumulation Framework (PAF) challenges this assumption. It suggests that the priority weight at each location accumulates across sequential events and that evidence for the presence of action-relevant information contributes to determining when attention is deployed to the location with the highest accumulated priority. Here, we tested these hypotheses for overt attention by recording first saccades in a free-viewing spatial-cueing task. We manipulated search difficulty (Experiments 1 and 2) and cue salience (Experiment 2). Standard theories posit that when oculomotor capture by the cue occurs, it is initiated before the search display appears; therefore, these theories predict that the cue's impact on the distribution of first saccades should be independent of search difficulty but influenced by the cue's saliency. By contrast, PAF posits that the cue can bias competition later, after processing of the search display has already started, and therefore predicts that such late impact should increase with both search difficulty and cue salience. The results fully supported PAF's predictions. Our account suggests a distinction between attentional capture and attentional-priority bias that resolves enduring inconsistencies in the attentional-capture literature. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":50195,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Human Perception and Performance","volume":" ","pages":"431-450"},"PeriodicalIF":2.1,"publicationDate":"2024-05-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139998068","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
A discrete component in visual working memory encoding. 视觉工作记忆编码中的离散成分
IF 2.1 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-05-01 Epub Date: 2024-02-29 DOI: 10.1037/xhp0001196
Hyung-Bum Park, Weiwei Zhang

Working memory (WM) is a central cognitive bottleneck, which has primarily been attributed to its well-known storage limit. However, relatively little is known about the processing limit during the initial memory encoding stage, which may also constrain various cognitive processes. The present study introduces a novel method using dynamic stimulus presentation and hierarchical Bayesian modeling to quantitatively estimate visual WM encoding speed. Participants performed a delayed-estimation task with two memory items continuously changing color hues in perceptually unnoticeable steps. Across three experiments, the recall errors systematically shifted toward the direction of color change, providing a proxy measure of encoding speed. Importantly, the observed shifts were best characterized by a temporal lag during the encoding of different items, supported by a mixture of two distributions with credibly distinct encoding times. A supplementary model-free analysis further confirmed the discrete encoding component in visual WM for multiple items. These findings shed light on the temporal dynamics of WM encoding processes. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

工作记忆(WM)是认知的核心瓶颈,其主要原因是众所周知的存储极限。然而,人们对初始记忆编码阶段的处理限制知之甚少,而这一限制也可能制约各种认知过程。本研究介绍了一种使用动态刺激呈现和分层贝叶斯建模来定量估计视觉 WM 编码速度的新方法。受试者进行了一项延迟估计任务,两个记忆项目以知觉上不易察觉的步骤不断改变颜色色调。在三次实验中,回忆错误系统地向颜色变化的方向移动,从而提供了编码速度的替代测量。重要的是,所观察到的偏移的最佳特征是不同项目编码过程中的时间滞后,这得到了编码时间截然不同的两种分布的混合支持。一项无模型补充分析进一步证实了视觉 WM 中多个项目的离散编码部分。这些发现揭示了 WM 编码过程的时间动态。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"A discrete component in visual working memory encoding.","authors":"Hyung-Bum Park, Weiwei Zhang","doi":"10.1037/xhp0001196","DOIUrl":"10.1037/xhp0001196","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Working memory (WM) is a central cognitive bottleneck, which has primarily been attributed to its well-known storage limit. However, relatively little is known about the processing limit during the initial memory encoding stage, which may also constrain various cognitive processes. The present study introduces a novel method using dynamic stimulus presentation and hierarchical Bayesian modeling to quantitatively estimate visual WM encoding speed. Participants performed a delayed-estimation task with two memory items continuously changing color hues in perceptually unnoticeable steps. Across three experiments, the recall errors systematically shifted toward the direction of color change, providing a proxy measure of encoding speed. Importantly, the observed shifts were best characterized by a temporal lag during the encoding of different items, supported by a mixture of two distributions with credibly distinct encoding times. A supplementary model-free analysis further confirmed the discrete encoding component in visual WM for multiple items. These findings shed light on the temporal dynamics of WM encoding processes. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":50195,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Human Perception and Performance","volume":" ","pages":"464-478"},"PeriodicalIF":2.1,"publicationDate":"2024-05-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139998065","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Use one system for all results to avoid contradiction: Advice for using significance tests, equivalence tests, and Bayes factors. 对所有结果使用一个系统,以避免矛盾:使用显著性检验、等效性检验和贝叶斯系数的建议。
IF 2.1 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-05-01 DOI: 10.1037/xhp0001202
Zoltan Dienes

A nonsignificant result against an H0 of no effect does not distinguish evidence for no effect from no evidence at all one way or the other. Thus, a researcher engaged primarily in significance testing may decide to follow up just the nonsignificant results with a test from another system of inference, such as equivalence tests (more generally, inference by intervals) or Bayes factors. However, selectively using two systems of inference in this way, can lead to inferential inconsistency because different tests are based on different principles, and therefore a researcher can be tempted to select the way each system is used to get the results the researcher wants for just the tests that system is applied to. For a related set of tests, one system of inference should be consistently used. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

针对 "无效应 H0 "的非显著性结果并不能以某种方式区分 "无效应 "和 "无证据"。因此,主要从事显著性检验的研究人员可能会决定仅对不显著的结果进行后续检验,而采用另一种推理系统,如等效检验(更广泛地说,区间推理)或贝叶斯因子。然而,以这种方式有选择地使用两种推论系统,可能会导致推论的不一致性,因为不同的检验基于不同的原理,因此,研究人员可能会倾向于选择使用每种系统的方式,以便仅在应用该系统的检验中获得研究人员想要的结果。对于一组相关的测试,应始终使用一种推论系统。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Use one system for all results to avoid contradiction: Advice for using significance tests, equivalence tests, and Bayes factors.","authors":"Zoltan Dienes","doi":"10.1037/xhp0001202","DOIUrl":"10.1037/xhp0001202","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>A nonsignificant result against an H0 of no effect does not distinguish evidence for no effect from no evidence at all one way or the other. Thus, a researcher engaged primarily in significance testing may decide to follow up just the nonsignificant results with a test from another system of inference, such as equivalence tests (more generally, inference by intervals) or Bayes factors. However, selectively using two systems of inference in this way, can lead to inferential inconsistency because different tests are based on different principles, and therefore a researcher can be tempted to select the way each system is used to get the results the researcher wants for just the tests that system is applied to. For a related set of tests, one system of inference should be consistently used. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":50195,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Human Perception and Performance","volume":"50 5","pages":"531-534"},"PeriodicalIF":2.1,"publicationDate":"2024-05-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140867248","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Do accent and input modality modulate processing of language switches in bilingual language comprehension? 口音和输入模式会调节双语语言理解中的语言转换处理吗?
IF 2.1 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-04-01 Epub Date: 2024-02-22 DOI: 10.1037/xhp0001190
Marion Coumel, Cong Liu, Danijela Trenkic, Angela de Bruin

We examined how bilinguals process language switches between their first (L1) and second language (L2). Language switching costs (slower responses to language switch than nonswitch trials) appear to arise more systematically in production than in comprehension, possibly because the latter context might sometimes elicit less language coactivation (Declerck et al., 2019). This might reduce language competition and in turn the need for bilinguals to apply language control when processing language switches. Yet even in comprehension, language coactivation may vary depending on variables such as the accent of the speaker (e.g., whether the L2 words are pronounced with an L1 or L2 accent) and input modality (spoken or written). In three experiments conducted during 2021-2022, we tested how unbalanced Mandarin-English bilinguals processed language switches during comprehension and the potential influence of a speaker's accent and input modality. Overall, across settings, participants experienced significant language switching costs. In some conditions, switching costs were larger to L1-Mandarin than to L2-English, an asymmetry consistent with the participants' dominance in L1-Mandarin and the application of language control. However, manipulating accent and input modality did not influence language switches, suggesting they did not impact language coactivation sufficiently to modulate language control. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

我们研究了双语者如何处理第一语言(L1)和第二语言(L2)之间的语言转换。语言转换成本(对语言转换的反应慢于非转换试验)似乎在生产中比在理解中更系统地出现,这可能是因为后一种语境有时可能引起较少的语言共激活(Declerck 等人,2019)。这可能会减少语言竞争,进而减少二语者在处理语言转换时应用语言控制的需要。然而,即使在理解过程中,语言共激活也会因说话者的口音(例如,L2单词的发音是L1口音还是L2口音)和输入模式(口语还是书面语)等变量的不同而变化。在 2021-2022 年期间进行的三项实验中,我们测试了不平衡的普通话-英语二语者在理解过程中如何处理语言转换,以及说话者的口音和输入模式的潜在影响。总体而言,在各种环境下,参与者都经历了显著的语言转换成本。在某些情况下,L1-普通话的转换成本大于 L2-英语的转换成本,这种不对称性与受试者的 L1-普通话优势和语言控制的应用相一致。然而,对口音和输入模式的操作并不影响语言转换,这表明它们对语言共激活的影响不足以调节语言控制。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Do accent and input modality modulate processing of language switches in bilingual language comprehension?","authors":"Marion Coumel, Cong Liu, Danijela Trenkic, Angela de Bruin","doi":"10.1037/xhp0001190","DOIUrl":"10.1037/xhp0001190","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>We examined how bilinguals process language switches between their first (L1) and second language (L2). Language switching costs (slower responses to language switch than nonswitch trials) appear to arise more systematically in production than in comprehension, possibly because the latter context might sometimes elicit less language coactivation (Declerck et al., 2019). This might reduce language competition and in turn the need for bilinguals to apply language control when processing language switches. Yet even in comprehension, language coactivation may vary depending on variables such as the accent of the speaker (e.g., whether the L2 words are pronounced with an L1 or L2 accent) and input modality (spoken or written). In three experiments conducted during 2021-2022, we tested how unbalanced Mandarin-English bilinguals processed language switches during comprehension and the potential influence of a speaker's accent and input modality. Overall, across settings, participants experienced significant language switching costs. In some conditions, switching costs were larger to L1-Mandarin than to L2-English, an asymmetry consistent with the participants' dominance in L1-Mandarin and the application of language control. However, manipulating accent and input modality did not influence language switches, suggesting they did not impact language coactivation sufficiently to modulate language control. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":50195,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Human Perception and Performance","volume":" ","pages":"395-415"},"PeriodicalIF":2.1,"publicationDate":"2024-04-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139933891","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Journal of Experimental Psychology-Human Perception and Performance
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1