首页 > 最新文献

Journal of Experimental Psychology-Human Perception and Performance最新文献

英文 中文
Age-related effects of immediate and delayed task switching in a targeted stepping task. 目标步法任务中即时和延迟任务切换对年龄的影响
IF 2.1 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-10-01 Epub Date: 2024-08-22 DOI: 10.1037/xhp0001237
Kyungwan Kim, Lena Deller, Marie Vinent, Wiebren Zijlstra

The ability to quickly adapt steps while walking is pivotal for safe mobility. In a previous study of immediate switching between the two stepping tasks, older adults (OAs) performed worse than young adults (YAs). However, it remained unclear whether this difference was due to an inability to learn the tasks or an inability to quickly switch. Therefore, this study investigated treadmill walking while performing two targeted stepping tasks in conditions with immediate task switching (ITS) versus delayed task switching (DTS). Thirty YAs (aged 26.9 ± 3.1 years) and 32 OAs (aged 70.7 ± 7.3 years) were randomly assigned to either the ITS (ITS_YAs and ITS_OAs) or the DTS (DTS_YAs and DTS_OAs) group. Each group repeatedly switched between Task A (easy) and Task B (difficult) and completed three blocks (ABAB). Delayed switching involved 1-min breaks between both tasks. Results showed that ITS_OAs exhibited significantly more step errors and worse step accuracy, but that DTS_OAs were able to achieve a similar performance as YAs. Our findings underline an inability for quick gait adaptation during targeted stepping tasks in OAs, but the possibility to learn when delayed switching reduces task interference. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

行走时快速调整步法的能力对于安全移动至关重要。在之前的一项关于在两种迈步任务之间立即切换的研究中,老年人(OAs)的表现比年轻人(YAs)差。然而,这种差异究竟是由于无法学习任务还是无法快速切换造成的,目前仍不清楚。因此,本研究调查了在立即任务切换(ITS)和延迟任务切换(DTS)条件下进行两项目标步法任务时的跑步机行走情况。研究人员将 30 名 YAs(年龄为 26.9 ± 3.1 岁)和 32 名 OAs(年龄为 70.7 ± 7.3 岁)随机分配到 ITS 组(ITS_YAs 和 ITS_OAs)或 DTS 组(DTS_YAs 和 DTS_OAs)。每组在任务 A(简单)和任务 B(困难)之间反复切换,并完成三个组块(ABAB)。延迟切换包括在两个任务之间休息 1 分钟。结果显示,ITS_OAs 表现出明显更多的步法错误和更差的步法准确性,但 DTS_OAs 能够达到与 YAs 相似的成绩。我们的研究结果表明,OA在执行有针对性的步法任务时无法快速适应步态,但在延迟切换减少任务干扰时有可能学会步法。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Age-related effects of immediate and delayed task switching in a targeted stepping task.","authors":"Kyungwan Kim, Lena Deller, Marie Vinent, Wiebren Zijlstra","doi":"10.1037/xhp0001237","DOIUrl":"10.1037/xhp0001237","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The ability to quickly adapt steps while walking is pivotal for safe mobility. In a previous study of immediate switching between the two stepping tasks, older adults (OAs) performed worse than young adults (YAs). However, it remained unclear whether this difference was due to an inability to learn the tasks or an inability to quickly switch. Therefore, this study investigated treadmill walking while performing two targeted stepping tasks in conditions with immediate task switching (ITS) versus delayed task switching (DTS). Thirty YAs (aged 26.9 ± 3.1 years) and 32 OAs (aged 70.7 ± 7.3 years) were randomly assigned to either the ITS (ITS_YAs and ITS_OAs) or the DTS (DTS_YAs and DTS_OAs) group. Each group repeatedly switched between Task A (easy) and Task B (difficult) and completed three blocks (ABAB). Delayed switching involved 1-min breaks between both tasks. Results showed that ITS_OAs exhibited significantly more step errors and worse step accuracy, but that DTS_OAs were able to achieve a similar performance as YAs. Our findings underline an inability for quick gait adaptation during targeted stepping tasks in OAs, but the possibility to learn when delayed switching reduces task interference. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":50195,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Human Perception and Performance","volume":" ","pages":"1023-1032"},"PeriodicalIF":2.1,"publicationDate":"2024-10-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142019436","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The role of selection history in the learned predictiveness effect. 选择历史在习得预测效应中的作用。
IF 2.1 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-10-01 Epub Date: 2024-08-22 DOI: 10.1037/xhp0001240
Paula Balea, Sara Molinero, Miguel A Vadillo, David Luque

Previous research has shown that cues that are good predictors of relevant outcomes receive more attention than nonpredictive cues. This attentional bias is thought to stem from the different predictive value of cues. However, because successful performance requires more attention to predictive cues, the bias may be a lingering effect of previous attention to cues (i.e., a selection history effect) instead. Two experiments assessed the contribution of predictive value and selection history to the bias produced by learned predictiveness. In a first task, participants responded to pairs of cues, only one of which predicted the correct response. A second task was superficially very similar, but the correct response was determined randomly on each trial and participants responded based on some physical characteristic of a target stimulus in each compound. Hence, in this latter task, participants had to pay more attention to the target stimuli, but these stimuli were not consistently associated with a specific response. Results revealed no differences in the attentional bias toward the relevant stimuli in the two tasks, suggesting that the bias induced by learned predictiveness is a consequence of deploying more attention to predictive stimuli during training. Thus, predictiveness may not bias attention by itself, adding nothing over and above the effect expected by selection history. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

以往的研究表明,能够很好预测相关结果的线索会比非预测性线索受到更多关注。这种注意偏差被认为源于线索的不同预测价值。然而,由于成功的表现需要对预测性线索给予更多的注意,这种偏差可能是以前对线索的注意所产生的一种挥之不去的影响(即选择历史效应)。有两个实验评估了预测价值和选择历史对学习预测性产生的偏差的贡献。在第一个任务中,参与者对成对的线索做出反应,其中只有一个线索能预测出正确的反应。第二项任务从表面上看非常相似,但正确的反应是在每次试验中随机决定的,而参与者是根据每个化合物中目标刺激的某些物理特征做出反应的。因此,在后一项任务中,参与者必须更加关注目标刺激物,但这些刺激物并不总是与特定的反应相关联。结果显示,在这两项任务中,对相关刺激的注意偏向没有差异,这表明学习预测性所引起的偏向是在训练过程中对预测性刺激投入更多注意力的结果。因此,预测性本身可能不会使注意力产生偏向,不会在选择史预期的效果之外增加任何效果。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"The role of selection history in the learned predictiveness effect.","authors":"Paula Balea, Sara Molinero, Miguel A Vadillo, David Luque","doi":"10.1037/xhp0001240","DOIUrl":"10.1037/xhp0001240","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Previous research has shown that cues that are good predictors of relevant outcomes receive more attention than nonpredictive cues. This attentional bias is thought to stem from the different predictive value of cues. However, because successful performance requires more attention to predictive cues, the bias may be a lingering effect of previous attention to cues (i.e., a selection history effect) instead. Two experiments assessed the contribution of predictive value and selection history to the bias produced by learned predictiveness. In a first task, participants responded to pairs of cues, only one of which predicted the correct response. A second task was superficially very similar, but the correct response was determined randomly on each trial and participants responded based on some physical characteristic of a target stimulus in each compound. Hence, in this latter task, participants had to pay more attention to the target stimuli, but these stimuli were not consistently associated with a specific response. Results revealed no differences in the attentional bias toward the relevant stimuli in the two tasks, suggesting that the bias induced by learned predictiveness is a consequence of deploying more attention to predictive stimuli during training. Thus, predictiveness may not bias attention by itself, adding nothing over and above the effect expected by selection history. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":50195,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Human Perception and Performance","volume":" ","pages":"1010-1022"},"PeriodicalIF":2.1,"publicationDate":"2024-10-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142019440","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Trial history contributes to the optimal tuning of attention. 试验历史有助于优化注意力的调整。
IF 2.1 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-10-01 Epub Date: 2024-08-15 DOI: 10.1037/xhp0001235
Dirk Kerzel, Stanislas Huynh Cong

In visual search tasks, targets are difficult to find when they are similar to the surrounding nontargets. In this scenario, it is optimal to tune attention to target features that maximize the difference between target and nontargets. We investigated whether the optimal tuning of attention is driven by biases arising from previously attended stimuli (i.e., trial history). Consistent with the effects of trial history, we found that optimal tuning was stronger when a single target-nontarget relation was repeated than when two target-nontarget relations alternated randomly. Detailed analysis of blocks with random alternation showed that optimal tuning was stronger when the target-nontarget relation probed in the current trial matched the relation in the previous trial. We evaluated several mechanisms that may underlie the effects of trial history, such as priming of attentional set, switch costs, and sensory adaptation. However, none of the accounts was able to fully account for the pattern of results. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

在视觉搜索任务中,如果目标与周围的非目标相似,就很难找到。在这种情况下,将注意力调整到能最大化目标与非目标之间差异的目标特征上是最佳选择。我们研究了注意力的最佳调整是否受先前注意的刺激(即试验历史)所产生的偏差的驱动。与试验历史的影响相一致,我们发现,当单一目标-非目标关系重复出现时,最佳调谐比两个目标-非目标关系随机交替出现时更强。对随机交替区块的详细分析显示,当当前试验中探测的目标-非目标关系与前一试验中的关系相匹配时,最佳调谐效果更强。我们评估了几种可能导致试验历史影响的机制,如注意集的启动、转换成本和感觉适应。然而,没有一种说法能够完全解释这种结果模式。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Trial history contributes to the optimal tuning of attention.","authors":"Dirk Kerzel, Stanislas Huynh Cong","doi":"10.1037/xhp0001235","DOIUrl":"10.1037/xhp0001235","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>In visual search tasks, targets are difficult to find when they are similar to the surrounding nontargets. In this scenario, it is optimal to tune attention to target features that maximize the difference between target and nontargets. We investigated whether the optimal tuning of attention is driven by biases arising from previously attended stimuli (i.e., trial history). Consistent with the effects of trial history, we found that optimal tuning was stronger when a single target-nontarget relation was repeated than when two target-nontarget relations alternated randomly. Detailed analysis of blocks with random alternation showed that optimal tuning was stronger when the target-nontarget relation probed in the current trial matched the relation in the previous trial. We evaluated several mechanisms that may underlie the effects of trial history, such as priming of attentional set, switch costs, and sensory adaptation. However, none of the accounts was able to fully account for the pattern of results. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":50195,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Human Perception and Performance","volume":" ","pages":"1000-1009"},"PeriodicalIF":2.1,"publicationDate":"2024-10-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141989409","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Haptic touch modulates size adaptation aftereffects on the hand. 触觉能调节手部的大小适应后效。
IF 2.1 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-10-01 Epub Date: 2024-08-15 DOI: 10.1037/xhp0001231
Souta Hidaka, Raffaele Tucciarelli, Salma Yusuf, Fabiana Memmolo, Sampath Rajapakse, Elena Azañón, Matthew R Longo

When we interact with objects using our hands, we derive their size through our skin. Prolonged exposure to an object leads to a perceptual size aftereffect: adapting to a larger/smaller object makes a subsequently perceived object to appear smaller/larger than its actual size. This phenomenon has been described as haptic as tactile sensations with kinesthetic feedback are involved. However, the exact role of different haptic components in generating this aftereffect remains largely underexplored. Here, we investigated how different aspects of haptic touch influence size perception. After adaptation to a large sphere with one hand and a small sphere with the other, participants touched two test spheres of equal or different sizes and judged which one felt larger. Similar haptic size adaption aftereffects were observed (a) when participants repeatedly grasped on and off the adapters, (b) when they simply continued to grasp the adapters without further hand movements, and (c) when the adapters were grasped without involving the fingers. All these conditions produced stronger aftereffects than a condition where the palms were simply resting on the adapter. Our findings suggest that the inclusion of grasp markedly increased the aftereffects, highlighting the pivotal role of haptic interactions in determining perceptual size adaptation. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

当我们用手与物体互动时,我们通过皮肤来推测物体的大小。长时间接触物体会导致感知尺寸后遗效应:适应一个更大/更小的物体会使随后感知到的物体看起来比实际尺寸更小/更大。这种现象被描述为触觉效应,因为它涉及到具有运动反馈的触觉。然而,不同触觉成分在产生这种后效中的确切作用在很大程度上仍未得到充分探索。在这里,我们研究了触觉的不同方面如何影响大小感知。在用一只手适应了一个大球体和用另一只手适应了一个小球体之后,参与者触摸了两个大小相同或不同的测试球体,并判断哪一个感觉更大。在以下情况下也观察到了类似的触觉大小适应后遗效应:(a)当参与者反复抓握适配器时;(b)当参与者只是继续抓握适配器而没有进一步的手部动作时;以及(c)当参与者抓握适配器而没有手指参与时。与手掌仅仅停留在适配器上的情况相比,所有这些情况都会产生更强烈的后遗效应。我们的研究结果表明,抓握的加入明显增加了后发效应,突出了触觉相互作用在决定知觉大小适应中的关键作用。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA,保留所有权利)。
{"title":"Haptic touch modulates size adaptation aftereffects on the hand.","authors":"Souta Hidaka, Raffaele Tucciarelli, Salma Yusuf, Fabiana Memmolo, Sampath Rajapakse, Elena Azañón, Matthew R Longo","doi":"10.1037/xhp0001231","DOIUrl":"10.1037/xhp0001231","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>When we interact with objects using our hands, we derive their size through our skin. Prolonged exposure to an object leads to a perceptual size aftereffect: adapting to a larger/smaller object makes a subsequently perceived object to appear smaller/larger than its actual size. This phenomenon has been described as haptic as tactile sensations with kinesthetic feedback are involved. However, the exact role of different haptic components in generating this aftereffect remains largely underexplored. Here, we investigated how different aspects of haptic touch influence size perception. After adaptation to a large sphere with one hand and a small sphere with the other, participants touched two test spheres of equal or different sizes and judged which one felt larger. Similar haptic size adaption aftereffects were observed (a) when participants repeatedly grasped on and off the adapters, (b) when they simply continued to grasp the adapters without further hand movements, and (c) when the adapters were grasped without involving the fingers. All these conditions produced stronger aftereffects than a condition where the palms were simply resting on the adapter. Our findings suggest that the inclusion of grasp markedly increased the aftereffects, highlighting the pivotal role of haptic interactions in determining perceptual size adaptation. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":50195,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Human Perception and Performance","volume":" ","pages":"989-999"},"PeriodicalIF":2.1,"publicationDate":"2024-10-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141989407","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Evidence against the low-salience account of attentional suppression. 反证注意力抑制的低体验观点
IF 2.1 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-10-01 Epub Date: 2024-08-22 DOI: 10.1037/xhp0001234
Brad T Stilwell, Howard E Egeth, Nicholas Gaspelin

Do salient distractors have the power to automatically capture attention? This question has led to a heated debate concerning the role of salience in attentional control. A potential resolution, called the signal suppression hypothesis, has proposed that salient items produce a bottom-up signal that vies for attention, but that salient stimuli can be suppressed via top-down control to prevent the capture of attention. This hypothesis, however, has been criticized on the grounds that the distractors used in initial studies of support were weakly salient. It has been difficult to know how seriously to take this low-salience criticism because assertions about high and low salience were made in the absence of a common (or any) measure of salience. The current study used a recently developed psychophysical technique to compare the salience of distractors from two previous studies at the center of this debate. Surprisingly, we found that the original stimuli criticized as having low salience were, if anything, more salient than stimuli from the later studies that purported to increase salience. Follow-up experiments determined exactly why the original stimuli were more salient and tested whether further improving salience could cause attentional capture as predicted by the low-salience account. Ultimately, these findings challenge purely stimulus-driven accounts of attentional control. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

突出的分心物是否能够自动吸引注意力?这个问题引发了关于显著性在注意控制中的作用的激烈争论。一种潜在的解决方案,即信号抑制假说,认为显著性项目会产生一种自下而上的信号来争夺注意力,但可以通过自上而下的控制来抑制显著性刺激,以防止注意力被俘获。然而,这一假说受到了批评,理由是最初的支持研究中使用的分心物的显著性很弱。我们很难知道该如何认真对待这种低显著性的批评,因为关于高显著性和低显著性的论断是在缺乏通用(或任何)显著性测量方法的情况下提出的。当前的研究使用了一种最新开发的心理物理技术,来比较处于这一争论中心的两项先前研究中的干扰物的显著性。出乎意料的是,我们发现原来被批评为低显著性的刺激物,竟然比后来声称提高显著性的研究中的刺激物更显著。后续实验确定了原始刺激物更显著的确切原因,并测试了进一步提高显著性是否会导致低显著性理论所预测的注意力捕获。最终,这些发现对注意力控制的纯刺激驱动理论提出了挑战。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Evidence against the low-salience account of attentional suppression.","authors":"Brad T Stilwell, Howard E Egeth, Nicholas Gaspelin","doi":"10.1037/xhp0001234","DOIUrl":"10.1037/xhp0001234","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Do salient distractors have the power to automatically capture attention? This question has led to a heated debate concerning the role of salience in attentional control. A potential resolution, called the signal suppression hypothesis, has proposed that salient items produce a bottom-up signal that vies for attention, but that salient stimuli can be suppressed via top-down control to prevent the capture of attention. This hypothesis, however, has been criticized on the grounds that the distractors used in initial studies of support were weakly salient. It has been difficult to know how seriously to take this low-salience criticism because assertions about high and low salience were made in the absence of a common (or any) measure of salience. The current study used a recently developed psychophysical technique to compare the salience of distractors from two previous studies at the center of this debate. Surprisingly, we found that the original stimuli criticized as having low salience were, if anything, more salient than stimuli from the later studies that purported to increase salience. Follow-up experiments determined exactly why the original stimuli were more salient and tested whether further improving salience could cause attentional capture as predicted by the low-salience account. Ultimately, these findings challenge purely stimulus-driven accounts of attentional control. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":50195,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Human Perception and Performance","volume":" ","pages":"1033-1047"},"PeriodicalIF":2.1,"publicationDate":"2024-10-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142019437","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The influence of affective voice on sound distance perception. 情感声音对声音距离感的影响。
IF 2.1 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-09-01 Epub Date: 2024-08-05 DOI: 10.1037/xhp0001222
Leon O H Kroczek, Sarah Roßkopf, Felix Stärz, Matthias Blau, Steven van de Par, Andreas Mühlberger

Affective stimuli in our environment indicate reward or threat and thereby relate to approach and avoidance behavior. Previous findings suggest that affective stimuli may bias visual perception, but it remains unclear whether similar biases exist in the auditory domain. Therefore, we asked whether affective auditory voices (angry vs. neutral) influence sound distance perception. Two VR experiments (data collection 2021-2022) were conducted in which auditory stimuli were presented via loudspeakers located at positions unknown to the participants. In the first experiment (N = 44), participants actively placed a visually presented virtual agent or virtual loudspeaker in an empty room at the perceived sound source location. In the second experiment (N = 32), participants were standing in front of several virtual agents or virtual loudspeakers and had to indicate the sound source by directing their gaze toward the perceived sound location. Results in both preregistered experiments consistently showed that participants estimated the location of angry voice stimuli at greater distances than the location of neutral voice stimuli. We discuss that neither emotional nor motivational biases can account for these results. Instead, distance estimates seem to rely on listeners' representations regarding the relationship between vocal affect and acoustic characteristics. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

环境中的情感刺激表示奖励或威胁,从而与接近和回避行为相关。以前的研究结果表明,情感刺激可能会使视觉感知产生偏差,但听觉领域是否存在类似的偏差仍不清楚。因此,我们提出了情感性听觉声音(愤怒与中性)是否会影响声音距离感知的问题。我们进行了两次 VR 实验(数据收集时间为 2021 年至 2022 年),通过位于参与者未知位置的扬声器呈现听觉刺激。在第一个实验中(N = 44),参与者主动将视觉呈现的虚拟代理或虚拟扬声器放置在感知声源位置的空房间中。在第二个实验中(32 人),参与者站在多个虚拟代理或虚拟扬声器前,必须将视线投向感知到的声源位置来指示声源。这两项预先登记的实验结果一致表明,参与者估计愤怒声音刺激位置的距离大于估计中性声音刺激位置的距离。我们讨论发现,情绪或动机偏差都不能解释这些结果。相反,距离估计似乎依赖于听者对声音情感和声学特征之间关系的表征。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"The influence of affective voice on sound distance perception.","authors":"Leon O H Kroczek, Sarah Roßkopf, Felix Stärz, Matthias Blau, Steven van de Par, Andreas Mühlberger","doi":"10.1037/xhp0001222","DOIUrl":"10.1037/xhp0001222","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Affective stimuli in our environment indicate reward or threat and thereby relate to approach and avoidance behavior. Previous findings suggest that affective stimuli may bias visual perception, but it remains unclear whether similar biases exist in the auditory domain. Therefore, we asked whether affective auditory voices (angry vs. neutral) influence sound distance perception. Two VR experiments (data collection 2021-2022) were conducted in which auditory stimuli were presented via loudspeakers located at positions unknown to the participants. In the first experiment (<i>N</i> = 44), participants actively placed a visually presented virtual agent or virtual loudspeaker in an empty room at the perceived sound source location. In the second experiment (<i>N</i> = 32), participants were standing in front of several virtual agents or virtual loudspeakers and had to indicate the sound source by directing their gaze toward the perceived sound location. Results in both preregistered experiments consistently showed that participants estimated the location of angry voice stimuli at greater distances than the location of neutral voice stimuli. We discuss that neither emotional nor motivational biases can account for these results. Instead, distance estimates seem to rely on listeners' representations regarding the relationship between vocal affect and acoustic characteristics. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":50195,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Human Perception and Performance","volume":" ","pages":"918-933"},"PeriodicalIF":2.1,"publicationDate":"2024-09-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141890688","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Action plan discarding leads to unbinding of action features. 丢弃行动计划会导致取消行动功能的绑定。
IF 2.1 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-09-01 Epub Date: 2024-07-25 DOI: 10.1037/xhp0001219
Viola Mocke, Christian Beste, Bernhard Pastötter, Wilfried Kunde

Action planning can be construed as the temporary binding of action features to form a representation known as an action file. This file is distinct from other possible, but currently not required actions of the behavioral repertoire. To further this action file approach, we investigated what happens with an initially planned action, which however, is discarded before execution. In two experiments we found consistent evidence for a quick unbinding of action features with discarding. Other possible mechanisms that action discarding might invoke, be it the paradox strengthening of a discarded action plan, the selective suppression of the otherwise intact plan, or the global suppression of all subsequent action, were not or at least less consistently supported. These findings provide a novel perspective on inhibitory action control, which we discuss with respect to its applications to other instances of such inhibitory control as studied in multitasking, stop-signal, directed forgetting, or response-reprogramming paradigms. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

行动规划可以理解为临时绑定行动特征,形成一个被称为行动文件的表征。这个文件有别于其他可能的、但目前还不是必须的行为。为了进一步研究这种行动档案方法,我们研究了最初计划好的行动会发生什么情况,然而这个行动在执行之前就被放弃了。在两个实验中,我们发现了一致的证据,即在丢弃动作时,动作特征会迅速解除绑定。丢弃动作可能引发的其他机制,无论是被丢弃动作计划的悖论强化、对原本完整计划的选择性抑制,还是对所有后续动作的全面抑制,都没有得到支持,至少支持的一致性较低。这些发现为抑制性行动控制提供了一个新的视角,我们将讨论它在多任务、停止信号、定向遗忘或反应编程范式中研究的抑制性控制的其他实例中的应用。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved)。
{"title":"Action plan discarding leads to unbinding of action features.","authors":"Viola Mocke, Christian Beste, Bernhard Pastötter, Wilfried Kunde","doi":"10.1037/xhp0001219","DOIUrl":"10.1037/xhp0001219","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Action planning can be construed as the temporary binding of action features to form a representation known as an action file. This file is distinct from other possible, but currently not required actions of the behavioral repertoire. To further this action file approach, we investigated what happens with an initially planned action, which however, is discarded before execution. In two experiments we found consistent evidence for a quick unbinding of action features with discarding. Other possible mechanisms that action discarding might invoke, be it the paradox strengthening of a discarded action plan, the selective suppression of the otherwise intact plan, or the global suppression of all subsequent action, were not or at least less consistently supported. These findings provide a novel perspective on inhibitory action control, which we discuss with respect to its applications to other instances of such inhibitory control as studied in multitasking, stop-signal, directed forgetting, or response-reprogramming paradigms. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":50195,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Human Perception and Performance","volume":" ","pages":"903-917"},"PeriodicalIF":2.1,"publicationDate":"2024-09-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141762245","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
"Leap before you look": Conditions that suppress explicit, knowledge-based learning during visuomotor adaptation. "先跃后看":在视觉运动适应过程中,抑制基于知识的显性学习的条件。
IF 2.1 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-08-01 Epub Date: 2024-05-16 DOI: 10.1037/xhp0001210
Tejas Savalia, Rosemary A Cowell, David E Huber

When learning a novel visuomotor mapping (e.g., mirror writing), accuracy can improve quickly through explicit, knowledge-based learning (e.g., aim left to go right), but after practice, implicit or procedural learning takes over, producing fast, natural movements. This procedural learning occurs automatically, whereas it has recently been found that knowledge-based learning can be suppressed by the gradual introduction of the novel mapping when participants must make fast movements and visuomotor perturbations are small (e.g., 30° rotations). We explored the range of task instructions, perturbation parameters, and feedback that preclude or encourage this suppression. Using a reaching task with a rotation between screen position and movement direction, we found that knowledge-based learning could be suppressed even for an extreme 90° rotation, but only if it was introduced gradually and only under instructions to move quickly. If the rotation was introduced abruptly or if instructions emphasized accuracy over speed, knowledge-based learning occurred. A second experiment indicated that knowledge-based learning always occurred in the absence of continuous motion feedback, evidenced by the time course of learning, the aftereffects of learning when the rotation was abruptly removed, and the outcome of formal model comparison between a dual-state (procedural and knowledge-based) versus a single-state (procedural only) learning model of the data. A third experiment replicated the findings and verified that the knowledge-based component of the dual-state model corresponded to explicit aiming, whereas the procedural component was slow to unlearn. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

在学习新的视觉运动映射(如镜像书写)时,通过显性的、以知识为基础的学习(如向左瞄准向右走),可以迅速提高准确性,但在练习之后,隐性或程序性学习就会占据上风,产生快速、自然的动作。这种程序性学习是自动发生的,而最近的研究发现,当参与者必须做出快速动作且视觉运动扰动较小时(如 30° 旋转),基于知识的学习可以通过逐步引入新的映射而被抑制。我们探索了排除或鼓励这种抑制的任务指令、扰动参数和反馈的范围。通过使用屏幕位置和移动方向之间存在旋转的伸手任务,我们发现即使是极端的 90° 旋转,也能抑制基于知识的学习,但前提是旋转必须是逐渐引入的,而且必须是在指示快速移动的情况下。如果突然引入旋转,或者指示强调准确性而非速度,则会出现基于知识的学习。第二个实验表明,在没有持续运动反馈的情况下,基于知识的学习总是会发生,这一点可以从学习的时间过程、突然取消旋转时的学习后效应以及数据的双状态(程序和知识)与单状态(仅程序)学习模型之间的正式模型比较结果中得到证明。第三个实验重复了这些发现,并验证了双状态模型中以知识为基础的部分与显性瞄准相对应,而以程序为基础的部分则会缓慢地解除学习。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"\"Leap before you look\": Conditions that suppress explicit, knowledge-based learning during visuomotor adaptation.","authors":"Tejas Savalia, Rosemary A Cowell, David E Huber","doi":"10.1037/xhp0001210","DOIUrl":"10.1037/xhp0001210","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>When learning a novel visuomotor mapping (e.g., mirror writing), accuracy can improve quickly through explicit, knowledge-based learning (e.g., aim left to go right), but after practice, implicit or procedural learning takes over, producing fast, natural movements. This procedural learning occurs automatically, whereas it has recently been found that knowledge-based learning can be suppressed by the gradual introduction of the novel mapping when participants must make fast movements and visuomotor perturbations are small (e.g., 30° rotations). We explored the range of task instructions, perturbation parameters, and feedback that preclude or encourage this suppression. Using a reaching task with a rotation between screen position and movement direction, we found that knowledge-based learning could be suppressed even for an extreme 90° rotation, but only if it was introduced gradually and only under instructions to move quickly. If the rotation was introduced abruptly or if instructions emphasized accuracy over speed, knowledge-based learning occurred. A second experiment indicated that knowledge-based learning always occurred in the absence of continuous motion feedback, evidenced by the time course of learning, the aftereffects of learning when the rotation was abruptly removed, and the outcome of formal model comparison between a dual-state (procedural and knowledge-based) versus a single-state (procedural only) learning model of the data. A third experiment replicated the findings and verified that the knowledge-based component of the dual-state model corresponded to explicit aiming, whereas the procedural component was slow to unlearn. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":50195,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Human Perception and Performance","volume":" ","pages":"785-807"},"PeriodicalIF":2.1,"publicationDate":"2024-08-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11412309/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140946486","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Why are some individuals better at using negative attentional templates to suppress distractors? Exploration of interindividual differences in cognitive control efficiency. 为什么有些人更善于使用消极注意模板来抑制分心?认知控制效率的个体间差异探索。
IF 2.1 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-08-01 Epub Date: 2024-06-20 DOI: 10.1037/xhp0001214
Matthieu Chidharom, Nancy B Carlisle

Negative templates are based on foreknowledge of distractor features and can lead to more efficient visual search at the group level. However, large individual differences exist in the size of benefits induced by negative cues. The cognitive factors underlying these interindividual differences remain unknown. Previous research has suggested higher engagement of proactive control for negative templates compared to positive templates. We thus hypothesized that interindividual differences in proactive control efficiency may explain the large variability in negative cue benefits. A large data set made up of data from two previously published studies was reanalyzed (N = 139), with eye movements recorded in 36 participants. Individual proactive control efficiency was measured through reaction time (RT) variability. Participants with higher proactive control efficiency exhibited larger benefits after negative cues across two critical measures: Individuals with higher proactive control showed larger RT benefits following negative compared to neutral cues; similarly, individuals with higher proactive control exhibited lower first saccades to cued distractor items. No such relationship was observed for positive cues. Our results confirmed the existence of large interindividual differences in the benefits induced by negative attentional templates. Critically, we show that proactive control drives these interindividual differences in negative template use. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

负面模板是基于对干扰物特征的预知,可以在群体水平上提高视觉搜索的效率。然而,负面提示所带来的益处的大小存在着巨大的个体差异。这些个体间差异背后的认知因素仍然未知。以往的研究表明,与积极模板相比,消极模板的主动控制参与度更高。因此,我们假设,主动控制效率的个体间差异可以解释负面线索收益的巨大差异。我们重新分析了由之前发表的两项研究数据组成的大型数据集(N = 139),其中记录了 36 名参与者的眼球运动。个人主动控制效率通过反应时间(RT)变异性进行测量。在两个关键测量指标上,主动控制效率较高的参与者在负面提示后表现出更大的收益:与中性线索相比,主动控制能力更强的个体在出现消极线索后表现出更大的反应时间收益;同样,主动控制能力更强的个体在出现线索分心项目时表现出更低的首次囊回。而积极线索则没有这种关系。我们的研究结果证实,负面注意模板所带来的益处存在很大的个体差异。重要的是,我们证明了主动控制驱动了消极模板使用的个体间差异。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Why are some individuals better at using negative attentional templates to suppress distractors? Exploration of interindividual differences in cognitive control efficiency.","authors":"Matthieu Chidharom, Nancy B Carlisle","doi":"10.1037/xhp0001214","DOIUrl":"10.1037/xhp0001214","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Negative templates are based on foreknowledge of distractor features and can lead to more efficient visual search at the group level. However, large individual differences exist in the size of benefits induced by negative cues. The cognitive factors underlying these interindividual differences remain unknown. Previous research has suggested higher engagement of proactive control for negative templates compared to positive templates. We thus hypothesized that interindividual differences in proactive control efficiency may explain the large variability in negative cue benefits. A large data set made up of data from two previously published studies was reanalyzed (<i>N</i> = 139), with eye movements recorded in 36 participants. Individual proactive control efficiency was measured through reaction time (RT) variability. Participants with higher proactive control efficiency exhibited larger benefits after negative cues across two critical measures: Individuals with higher proactive control showed larger RT benefits following negative compared to neutral cues; similarly, individuals with higher proactive control exhibited lower first saccades to cued distractor items. No such relationship was observed for positive cues. Our results confirmed the existence of large interindividual differences in the benefits induced by negative attentional templates. Critically, we show that proactive control drives these interindividual differences in negative template use. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":50195,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Human Perception and Performance","volume":" ","pages":"808-818"},"PeriodicalIF":2.1,"publicationDate":"2024-08-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11571072/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141428183","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Temporal segmentation and "look ahead" simulation: Physical events structure visual perception of intuitive physics. 时间分割和 "前瞻 "模拟:物理事件构造了直观物理的视觉感知。
IF 2.1 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-08-01 Epub Date: 2024-06-20 DOI: 10.1037/xhp0001218
Tristan S Yates, Shannon Yasuda, Ilker Yildirim

How we perceive the physical world is not only organized in terms of objects, but also structured in time as sequences of events. This is especially evident in intuitive physics, with temporally bounded dynamics such as falling, occlusion, and bouncing demarcating the continuous flow of sensory inputs. While the spatial structure and attentional consequences of physical objects have been well-studied, much less is known about the temporal structure and attentional consequences of physical events in visual perception. Previous work has recognized physical events as units in the mind, and used presegmented object interactions to explore physical representations. However, these studies did not address whether and how perception imposes the kind of temporal structure that carves these physical events to begin with, and the attentional consequences of such segmentation during intuitive physics. Here, we use performance-based tasks to address this gap. In Experiment 1, we find that perception not only spontaneously separates visual input in time into physical events, but also, this segmentation occurs in a nonlinear manner within a few hundred milliseconds at the moment of the event boundary. In Experiment 2, we find that event representations, once formed, use coarse "look ahead" simulations to selectively prioritize those objects that are predictively part of the unfolding dynamics. This rich temporal and predictive structure of physical event representations, formed during vision, should inform models of intuitive physics. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

我们对物理世界的感知不仅以物体为单位,还以事件序列为时间结构。这一点在直观物理学中尤为明显,有时间限制的动态(如坠落、闭塞和弹跳)划分了感官输入的连续流。虽然对物理对象的空间结构和注意后果已有深入研究,但对视觉感知中物理事件的时间结构和注意后果却知之甚少。以前的研究已经认识到物理事件是思维中的单元,并使用预分割对象交互来探索物理表征。然而,这些研究并没有解决感知是否以及如何施加时间结构来刻画这些物理事件,也没有解决在直观物理过程中这种分割的注意后果。在此,我们使用基于表现的任务来弥补这一不足。在实验 1 中,我们发现感知不仅会自发地将视觉输入的时间分割成物理事件,而且这种分割会在事件边界的几百毫秒内以非线性方式发生。在实验 2 中,我们发现事件表征一旦形成,就会使用粗略的 "前瞻性 "模拟,有选择性地优先选择那些可以预测动态发展的物体。在视觉过程中形成的物理事件表征的这种丰富的时间和预测结构,应能为直觉物理学模型提供信息。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Temporal segmentation and \"look ahead\" simulation: Physical events structure visual perception of intuitive physics.","authors":"Tristan S Yates, Shannon Yasuda, Ilker Yildirim","doi":"10.1037/xhp0001218","DOIUrl":"10.1037/xhp0001218","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>How we perceive the physical world is not only organized in terms of objects, but also structured in time as sequences of events. This is especially evident in intuitive physics, with temporally bounded dynamics such as falling, occlusion, and bouncing demarcating the continuous flow of sensory inputs. While the spatial structure and attentional consequences of physical objects have been well-studied, much less is known about the temporal structure and attentional consequences of physical events in visual perception. Previous work has recognized physical events as units in the mind, and used presegmented object interactions to explore physical representations. However, these studies did not address whether and how perception imposes the kind of temporal structure that carves these physical events to begin with, and the attentional consequences of such segmentation during intuitive physics. Here, we use performance-based tasks to address this gap. In Experiment 1, we find that perception not only spontaneously separates visual input in time into physical events, but also, this segmentation occurs in a nonlinear manner within a few hundred milliseconds at the moment of the event boundary. In Experiment 2, we find that event representations, once formed, use coarse \"look ahead\" simulations to selectively prioritize those objects that are predictively part of the unfolding dynamics. This rich temporal and predictive structure of physical event representations, formed during vision, should inform models of intuitive physics. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":50195,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Human Perception and Performance","volume":" ","pages":"859-874"},"PeriodicalIF":2.1,"publicationDate":"2024-08-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141428182","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Journal of Experimental Psychology-Human Perception and Performance
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1