首页 > 最新文献

Journal of Experimental Psychology-Human Perception and Performance最新文献

英文 中文
Use one system for all results to avoid contradiction: Advice for using significance tests, equivalence tests, and Bayes factors. 对所有结果使用一个系统,以避免矛盾:使用显著性检验、等效性检验和贝叶斯系数的建议。
IF 2.1 3区 心理学 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2024-05-01 DOI: 10.1037/xhp0001202
Zoltan Dienes

A nonsignificant result against an H0 of no effect does not distinguish evidence for no effect from no evidence at all one way or the other. Thus, a researcher engaged primarily in significance testing may decide to follow up just the nonsignificant results with a test from another system of inference, such as equivalence tests (more generally, inference by intervals) or Bayes factors. However, selectively using two systems of inference in this way, can lead to inferential inconsistency because different tests are based on different principles, and therefore a researcher can be tempted to select the way each system is used to get the results the researcher wants for just the tests that system is applied to. For a related set of tests, one system of inference should be consistently used. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

针对 "无效应 H0 "的非显著性结果并不能以某种方式区分 "无效应 "和 "无证据"。因此,主要从事显著性检验的研究人员可能会决定仅对不显著的结果进行后续检验,而采用另一种推理系统,如等效检验(更广泛地说,区间推理)或贝叶斯因子。然而,以这种方式有选择地使用两种推论系统,可能会导致推论的不一致性,因为不同的检验基于不同的原理,因此,研究人员可能会倾向于选择使用每种系统的方式,以便仅在应用该系统的检验中获得研究人员想要的结果。对于一组相关的测试,应始终使用一种推论系统。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Use one system for all results to avoid contradiction: Advice for using significance tests, equivalence tests, and Bayes factors.","authors":"Zoltan Dienes","doi":"10.1037/xhp0001202","DOIUrl":"10.1037/xhp0001202","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>A nonsignificant result against an H0 of no effect does not distinguish evidence for no effect from no evidence at all one way or the other. Thus, a researcher engaged primarily in significance testing may decide to follow up just the nonsignificant results with a test from another system of inference, such as equivalence tests (more generally, inference by intervals) or Bayes factors. However, selectively using two systems of inference in this way, can lead to inferential inconsistency because different tests are based on different principles, and therefore a researcher can be tempted to select the way each system is used to get the results the researcher wants for just the tests that system is applied to. For a related set of tests, one system of inference should be consistently used. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":50195,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Human Perception and Performance","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.1,"publicationDate":"2024-05-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140867248","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Exploring individual differences in native phonetic perception and their link to nonnative phonetic perception. 探索母语语音感知的个体差异及其与非母语语音感知的联系。
IF 2.1 3区 心理学 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2024-04-01 Epub Date: 2024-02-01 DOI: 10.1037/xhp0001191
Claire T Honda, Meghan Clayards, Shari R Baum

Adults differ considerably in their perception of both native and nonnative phonemes. For instance, when presented with continua of native phonemes on two-alternative forced choice (2AFC) or visual analog scaling (VAS) tasks, some people show sudden changes in responses (i.e., steep identification slopes) and others show gradual changes (i.e., shallow identification slopes). Moreover, some adults are more successful than others at learning unfamiliar phonemes. The predictors of these individual differences and the relationships between them are poorly understood. It also remains unclear to what extent different tasks (2AFC vs. VAS) may reflect distinct individual differences in perception. In two experiments, we addressed these questions by examining the relationships between individual differences in performance on native and nonnative phonetic perception tasks. We found that shallow 2AFC identification slopes were not related to shallow VAS identification slopes but were related to inconsistent VAS responses. Additionally, our results suggest that consistent native perception may play a role in promoting successful nonnative perception. These findings help characterize the nature of individual differences in phonetic perception and contribute to our understanding of how to measure such differences. This work also has implications for encouraging successful acquisition of new languages in adulthood. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

成年人对母语和非母语音素的感知存在很大差异。例如,当在两选一强迫选择(2AFC)或视觉模拟标度(VAS)任务中出现连续的母语音素时,有些人的反应会突然发生变化(即陡峭的识别斜坡),而有些人的反应则会逐渐发生变化(即较浅的识别斜坡)。此外,一些成年人在学习陌生音素时比其他人更成功。对于这些个体差异的预测因素以及它们之间的关系,人们还知之甚少。此外,不同的任务(2AFC 与 VAS)在多大程度上反映了感知方面的个体差异也仍不清楚。在两个实验中,我们通过研究母语和非母语语音感知任务中个体差异之间的关系来解决这些问题。我们发现,浅 2AFC 识别斜率与浅 VAS 识别斜率无关,但与不一致的 VAS 反应有关。此外,我们的研究结果表明,一致的母语感知可能对成功的非母语感知起到促进作用。这些发现有助于描述语音感知个体差异的性质,并有助于我们理解如何测量这种差异。这项研究还对鼓励人们在成年后成功学习新语言具有重要意义。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Exploring individual differences in native phonetic perception and their link to nonnative phonetic perception.","authors":"Claire T Honda, Meghan Clayards, Shari R Baum","doi":"10.1037/xhp0001191","DOIUrl":"10.1037/xhp0001191","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Adults differ considerably in their perception of both native and nonnative phonemes. For instance, when presented with continua of native phonemes on two-alternative forced choice (2AFC) or visual analog scaling (VAS) tasks, some people show sudden changes in responses (i.e., steep identification slopes) and others show gradual changes (i.e., shallow identification slopes). Moreover, some adults are more successful than others at learning unfamiliar phonemes. The predictors of these individual differences and the relationships between them are poorly understood. It also remains unclear to what extent different tasks (2AFC vs. VAS) may reflect distinct individual differences in perception. In two experiments, we addressed these questions by examining the relationships between individual differences in performance on native and nonnative phonetic perception tasks. We found that shallow 2AFC identification slopes were not related to shallow VAS identification slopes but were related to inconsistent VAS responses. Additionally, our results suggest that consistent native perception may play a role in promoting successful nonnative perception. These findings help characterize the nature of individual differences in phonetic perception and contribute to our understanding of how to measure such differences. This work also has implications for encouraging successful acquisition of new languages in adulthood. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":50195,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Human Perception and Performance","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.1,"publicationDate":"2024-04-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139652066","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Do accent and input modality modulate processing of language switches in bilingual language comprehension? 口音和输入模式会调节双语语言理解中的语言转换处理吗?
IF 2.1 3区 心理学 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2024-04-01 Epub Date: 2024-02-22 DOI: 10.1037/xhp0001190
Marion Coumel, Cong Liu, Danijela Trenkic, Angela de Bruin

We examined how bilinguals process language switches between their first (L1) and second language (L2). Language switching costs (slower responses to language switch than nonswitch trials) appear to arise more systematically in production than in comprehension, possibly because the latter context might sometimes elicit less language coactivation (Declerck et al., 2019). This might reduce language competition and in turn the need for bilinguals to apply language control when processing language switches. Yet even in comprehension, language coactivation may vary depending on variables such as the accent of the speaker (e.g., whether the L2 words are pronounced with an L1 or L2 accent) and input modality (spoken or written). In three experiments conducted during 2021-2022, we tested how unbalanced Mandarin-English bilinguals processed language switches during comprehension and the potential influence of a speaker's accent and input modality. Overall, across settings, participants experienced significant language switching costs. In some conditions, switching costs were larger to L1-Mandarin than to L2-English, an asymmetry consistent with the participants' dominance in L1-Mandarin and the application of language control. However, manipulating accent and input modality did not influence language switches, suggesting they did not impact language coactivation sufficiently to modulate language control. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

我们研究了双语者如何处理第一语言(L1)和第二语言(L2)之间的语言转换。语言转换成本(对语言转换的反应慢于非转换试验)似乎在生产中比在理解中更系统地出现,这可能是因为后一种语境有时可能引起较少的语言共激活(Declerck 等人,2019)。这可能会减少语言竞争,进而减少二语者在处理语言转换时应用语言控制的需要。然而,即使在理解过程中,语言共激活也会因说话者的口音(例如,L2单词的发音是L1口音还是L2口音)和输入模式(口语还是书面语)等变量的不同而变化。在 2021-2022 年期间进行的三项实验中,我们测试了不平衡的普通话-英语二语者在理解过程中如何处理语言转换,以及说话者的口音和输入模式的潜在影响。总体而言,在各种环境下,参与者都经历了显著的语言转换成本。在某些情况下,L1-普通话的转换成本大于 L2-英语的转换成本,这种不对称性与受试者的 L1-普通话优势和语言控制的应用相一致。然而,对口音和输入模式的操作并不影响语言转换,这表明它们对语言共激活的影响不足以调节语言控制。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Do accent and input modality modulate processing of language switches in bilingual language comprehension?","authors":"Marion Coumel, Cong Liu, Danijela Trenkic, Angela de Bruin","doi":"10.1037/xhp0001190","DOIUrl":"10.1037/xhp0001190","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>We examined how bilinguals process language switches between their first (L1) and second language (L2). Language switching costs (slower responses to language switch than nonswitch trials) appear to arise more systematically in production than in comprehension, possibly because the latter context might sometimes elicit less language coactivation (Declerck et al., 2019). This might reduce language competition and in turn the need for bilinguals to apply language control when processing language switches. Yet even in comprehension, language coactivation may vary depending on variables such as the accent of the speaker (e.g., whether the L2 words are pronounced with an L1 or L2 accent) and input modality (spoken or written). In three experiments conducted during 2021-2022, we tested how unbalanced Mandarin-English bilinguals processed language switches during comprehension and the potential influence of a speaker's accent and input modality. Overall, across settings, participants experienced significant language switching costs. In some conditions, switching costs were larger to L1-Mandarin than to L2-English, an asymmetry consistent with the participants' dominance in L1-Mandarin and the application of language control. However, manipulating accent and input modality did not influence language switches, suggesting they did not impact language coactivation sufficiently to modulate language control. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":50195,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Human Perception and Performance","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.1,"publicationDate":"2024-04-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139933891","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Moving stimuli enhance beat timing and sensorimotor coupling in vision. 移动刺激增强了视觉中的节拍计时和感觉运动耦合。
IF 2.1 3区 心理学 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2024-04-01 Epub Date: 2024-02-29 DOI: 10.1037/xhp0001193
Liang Zhou, Lianzi Xing, Chenhao Zheng, Shouxin Li

Vision has long been known for its inefficiency in beat perception and synchronization. However, this has been challenged by the finding that moving stimuli (bouncing ball or moving bar) can significantly improve visual beat synchronization. The present study examined two possible mechanisms for this phenomenon: visual motion facilitates temporal processing or promotes sensorimotor coupling. Instead of a single visual object (such as a ball or bar), random-dot kinematograms (RDKs) were used to construct visual motion sequences to avoid confounding factors, such as changes in trajectory and velocity. Experiment 1 showed that RDKs improved beat-timing discrimination compared with visual flashes, but auditory tones were still superior to RDKs. In Experiment 2, synchronized movements improved auditory-tone beat timing but impaired visual-flash beat timing, with no effect on RDK beat timing. Experiment 3 indicated that the regression slope of the phase correction response in RDKs was higher than that in visual flashes but still lower than that in auditory tones. The results showed that moving stimuli enhances both temporal processing (Experiment 1) and sensorimotor coupling (Experiments 2 and 3) in vision, but to a lesser degree, with audition retaining an advantage. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

长期以来,人们一直认为视觉在节拍感知和同步方面效率低下。然而,移动刺激(弹跳球或移动棒)能显著提高视觉节拍同步性,这一发现对上述观点提出了挑战。本研究探讨了这一现象的两种可能机制:视觉运动促进了时间处理或促进了感觉运动耦合。为了避免轨迹和速度变化等干扰因素,我们使用随机点运动图(RDK)来构建视觉运动序列,而不是单一的视觉对象(如球或杆)。实验 1 显示,与视觉闪光相比,随机点运动图提高了节拍时间辨别能力,但听觉音调仍优于随机点运动图。在实验 2 中,同步运动改善了听觉音调节拍计时,但损害了视觉闪光节拍计时,对 RDK 节拍计时没有影响。实验 3 表明,RDK 的相位校正反应回归斜率高于视觉闪光,但仍低于听觉音调。结果表明,移动刺激增强了视觉的时间处理(实验 1)和感觉运动耦合(实验 2 和 3),但程度较低,听觉仍保持优势。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Moving stimuli enhance beat timing and sensorimotor coupling in vision.","authors":"Liang Zhou, Lianzi Xing, Chenhao Zheng, Shouxin Li","doi":"10.1037/xhp0001193","DOIUrl":"10.1037/xhp0001193","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Vision has long been known for its inefficiency in beat perception and synchronization. However, this has been challenged by the finding that moving stimuli (bouncing ball or moving bar) can significantly improve visual beat synchronization. The present study examined two possible mechanisms for this phenomenon: visual motion facilitates temporal processing or promotes sensorimotor coupling. Instead of a single visual object (such as a ball or bar), random-dot kinematograms (RDKs) were used to construct visual motion sequences to avoid confounding factors, such as changes in trajectory and velocity. Experiment 1 showed that RDKs improved beat-timing discrimination compared with visual flashes, but auditory tones were still superior to RDKs. In Experiment 2, synchronized movements improved auditory-tone beat timing but impaired visual-flash beat timing, with no effect on RDK beat timing. Experiment 3 indicated that the regression slope of the phase correction response in RDKs was higher than that in visual flashes but still lower than that in auditory tones. The results showed that moving stimuli enhances both temporal processing (Experiment 1) and sensorimotor coupling (Experiments 2 and 3) in vision, but to a lesser degree, with audition retaining an advantage. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":50195,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Human Perception and Performance","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.1,"publicationDate":"2024-04-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139998067","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Social priming of speech perception: The role of individual differences in implicit racial and ethnic associations. 言语感知的社会诱导:个体差异在内隐性种族和民族联想中的作用。
IF 2.1 3区 心理学 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2024-04-01 Epub Date: 2024-02-08 DOI: 10.1037/xhp0001187
Drew J McLaughlin, Kristin J Van Engen

Prior research has shown that visual information, such as a speaker's perceived race or ethnicity, prompts listeners to expect a specific sociophonetic pattern ("social priming"). Indeed, a picture of an East Asian face may facilitate perception of second language (L2) Mandarin Chinese-accented English but interfere with perception of first language- (L1-) accented English. The present study builds on this line of inquiry, addressing the relationship between social priming effects and implicit racial/ethnic associations for L1- and L2-accented speech. For L1-accented speech, we found no priming effects when comparing White versus East Asian or Latina primes. For L2- (Mandarin Chinese-) accented speech, however, transcription accuracy was slightly better following an East Asian prime than a White prime. Across all experiments, a relationship between performance and individual differences in implicit associations emerged, but in no cases did this relationship interact with the priming manipulation. Ultimately, exploring social priming effects with additional methodological approaches, and in different populations of listeners, will help to determine whether these effects operate differently in the context of L1- and L2-accented speech. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

先前的研究表明,视觉信息(如说话者的种族或民族)会促使听者期待特定的社会语音模式("社会引物")。事实上,一张东亚人面孔的图片可能会促进对第二语言(L2)汉语普通话口音英语的感知,但会干扰对第一语言(L1)口音英语的感知。本研究以这一研究思路为基础,探讨了 L1 和 L2 口音语音的社会引物效应与内隐种族/民族关联之间的关系。对于 L1 口音的语音,在比较白人与东亚人或拉丁裔人的引物时,我们没有发现引物效应。但是,对于 L2(中国普通话)重音语音,东亚语素比白人语素的转录准确率略高。在所有实验中,成绩与内隐联想的个体差异之间都存在着某种关系,但在任何情况下,这种关系都不会与引物操作产生交互作用。最终,通过更多的方法和在不同的听者群体中探索社会引物效应,将有助于确定这些效应在 L1- 和 L2- 口音语音中是否有不同的作用。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Social priming of speech perception: The role of individual differences in implicit racial and ethnic associations.","authors":"Drew J McLaughlin, Kristin J Van Engen","doi":"10.1037/xhp0001187","DOIUrl":"10.1037/xhp0001187","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Prior research has shown that visual information, such as a speaker's perceived race or ethnicity, prompts listeners to expect a specific sociophonetic pattern (\"social priming\"). Indeed, a picture of an East Asian face may facilitate perception of second language (L2) Mandarin Chinese-accented English but interfere with perception of first language- (L1-) accented English. The present study builds on this line of inquiry, addressing the relationship between social priming effects and implicit racial/ethnic associations for L1- and L2-accented speech. For L1-accented speech, we found no priming effects when comparing White versus East Asian or Latina primes. For L2- (Mandarin Chinese-) accented speech, however, transcription accuracy was slightly better following an East Asian prime than a White prime. Across all experiments, a relationship between performance and individual differences in implicit associations emerged, but in no cases did this relationship interact with the priming manipulation. Ultimately, exploring social priming effects with additional methodological approaches, and in different populations of listeners, will help to determine whether these effects operate differently in the context of L1- and L2-accented speech. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":50195,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Human Perception and Performance","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.1,"publicationDate":"2024-04-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139708340","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
What drives the automatic retrieval of real-world object size knowledge? 是什么驱动了现实世界物体尺寸知识的自动检索?
IF 2.1 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-04-01 Epub Date: 2024-02-01 DOI: 10.1037/xhp0001189
Simen Hagen, Yuanfang Zhao, Lydia Moonen, Neele Ulken, Marius V Peelen

Real-world object size is a behaviorally relevant object property that is automatically retrieved when viewing object images: participants are faster to indicate the bigger of two object images when this object is also bigger in the real world. What drives this size Stroop effect? One possibility is that it reflects the automatic retrieval of real-world size after objects are recognized at the basic level (e.g., recognizing an object as a plane activates large real-world size). An alternative possibility is that the size Stroop effect is driven by automatic associations between low-/mid-level visual features (e.g., rectilinearity) and real-world size, bypassing object recognition. Here, we tested both accounts. In Experiment 1, objects were displayed upright and inverted, slowing down recognition while equating visual features. Inversion strongly reduced the Stroop effect, indicating that object recognition contributed to the Stroop effect. Independently of inversion, however, trial-wise differences in rectilinearity also contributed to the Stroop effect. In Experiment 2, the Stroop effect was compared between manmade objects (for which rectilinearity was associated with size) and animals (no association between rectilinearity and size). The Stroop effect was larger for animals than for manmade objects, indicating that rectilinear feature differences were not necessary for the Stroop effect. Finally, in Experiment 3, unrecognizable "texform" objects that maintained size-related visual feature differences were displayed upright and inverted. Results revealed a small Stroop effect for both upright and inverted conditions. Altogether, these results indicate that the size Stroop effect partly follows object recognition with an additional contribution from visual feature associations. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

现实世界中的物体大小是一种与行为相关的物体属性,在观看物体图像时会自动检索:当两个物体图像中的一个物体在现实世界中也较大时,参与者会更快地指出该物体较大。是什么驱动了这种大小斯特罗普效应?一种可能是,它反映了在基本水平上识别物体后对现实世界大小的自动检索(例如,将物体识别为平面会激活现实世界的大尺寸)。另一种可能是,大小 Stroop 效应是由中低级视觉特征(如直线性)和真实世界大小之间的自动关联驱动的,绕过了物体识别。在这里,我们测试了这两种说法。在实验 1 中,物体被直立和倒置显示,在等同视觉特征的同时减慢识别速度。倒置大大降低了 Stroop 效应,表明物体识别促成了 Stroop 效应。然而,与倒置无关的是,试验过程中的直线度差异也会导致斯特罗普效应。在实验 2 中,对人造物体(直线度与大小相关)和动物(直线度与大小无关)的 Stroop 效应进行了比较。动物的 Stroop 效应大于人造物体,这表明直角特征差异并非 Stroop 效应的必要条件。最后,在实验 3 中,对无法识别的 "texform "物体进行了直立和倒置显示,这些物体保持了与尺寸相关的视觉特征差异。结果显示,在直立和倒置的条件下都出现了轻微的 Stroop 效应。总之,这些结果表明,尺寸的 Stroop 效应部分是在物体识别之后产生的,另外还有视觉特征联想的贡献。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"What drives the automatic retrieval of real-world object size knowledge?","authors":"Simen Hagen, Yuanfang Zhao, Lydia Moonen, Neele Ulken, Marius V Peelen","doi":"10.1037/xhp0001189","DOIUrl":"10.1037/xhp0001189","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Real-world object size is a behaviorally relevant object property that is automatically retrieved when viewing object images: participants are faster to indicate the bigger of two object images when this object is also bigger in the real world. What drives this size Stroop effect? One possibility is that it reflects the automatic retrieval of real-world size after objects are recognized at the basic level (e.g., recognizing an object as a plane activates large real-world size). An alternative possibility is that the size Stroop effect is driven by automatic associations between low-/mid-level visual features (e.g., rectilinearity) and real-world size, bypassing object recognition. Here, we tested both accounts. In Experiment 1, objects were displayed upright and inverted, slowing down recognition while equating visual features. Inversion strongly reduced the Stroop effect, indicating that object recognition contributed to the Stroop effect. Independently of inversion, however, trial-wise differences in rectilinearity also contributed to the Stroop effect. In Experiment 2, the Stroop effect was compared between manmade objects (for which rectilinearity was associated with size) and animals (no association between rectilinearity and size). The Stroop effect was larger for animals than for manmade objects, indicating that rectilinear feature differences were not necessary for the Stroop effect. Finally, in Experiment 3, unrecognizable \"texform\" objects that maintained size-related visual feature differences were displayed upright and inverted. Results revealed a small Stroop effect for both upright and inverted conditions. Altogether, these results indicate that the size Stroop effect partly follows object recognition with an additional contribution from visual feature associations. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":50195,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Human Perception and Performance","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.1,"publicationDate":"2024-04-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC7616435/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139652067","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Object-based attention is accentuated by object reward association. 基于物体的注意力会因物体奖励联想而增强。
IF 2.1 3区 心理学 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2024-03-01 DOI: 10.1037/xhp0001177
Damiano Grignolio, David J Acunzo, Clayton Hickey

Humans use selective attention to prioritize visual features, like color or shape, as well as discrete spatial locations, and these effects are sensitive to the experience of reward. Reward-associated features and locations are accordingly prioritized from early in the visual hierarchy. Attention is also sensitive to the establishment of visual objects: selection of one constituent object part often leads to prioritization of other locations on that object. But very little is known about the influence of reward on this object-based control of attention. Here we show in four experiments that reward prioritization and object prioritization interact in visual cognition to guide selection. Experiment 1 establishes groundwork for this investigation, showing that reward feedback does not negate object prioritization. In Experiment 2, we corroborate the hypothesis that reward prioritization and object prioritization emerge concurrently. In Experiment 3, we find that reward prioritization and object prioritization sustain and interact in extinction, when reward feedback is discontinued. We verify this interaction in Experiment 4, linking it to task experience rather than the strategic utility of the reward association. Results suggest that information gathered from locations on reward-associated objects gains preferential access to cognition. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

人类利用选择性注意来确定视觉特征(如颜色或形状)以及离散空间位置的优先顺序,这些效应对奖励体验非常敏感。因此,与奖励相关的特征和位置在视觉层次结构的早期就被优先考虑。注意力对视觉对象的建立也很敏感:选择一个组成对象的部分往往会导致优先选择该对象上的其他位置。但是,人们对奖励对这种基于对象的注意力控制的影响知之甚少。在这里,我们通过四个实验证明,奖励优先和对象优先在视觉认知中相互作用,从而引导选择。实验 1 为这一研究奠定了基础,表明奖励反馈不会否定对象优先化。在实验 2 中,我们证实了奖励优先和对象优先同时出现的假设。在实验 3 中,我们发现当奖赏反馈停止时,奖赏优先化和对象优先化在消退过程中会持续并相互作用。我们在实验 4 中验证了这种相互作用,并将其与任务经验而非奖励关联的策略效用联系起来。结果表明,从奖励相关对象的位置收集到的信息会优先进入认知。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Object-based attention is accentuated by object reward association.","authors":"Damiano Grignolio, David J Acunzo, Clayton Hickey","doi":"10.1037/xhp0001177","DOIUrl":"10.1037/xhp0001177","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Humans use selective attention to prioritize visual features, like color or shape, as well as discrete spatial locations, and these effects are sensitive to the experience of reward. Reward-associated features and locations are accordingly prioritized from early in the visual hierarchy. Attention is also sensitive to the establishment of visual objects: selection of one constituent object part often leads to prioritization of other locations on that object. But very little is known about the influence of reward on this object-based control of attention. Here we show in four experiments that reward prioritization and object prioritization interact in visual cognition to guide selection. Experiment 1 establishes groundwork for this investigation, showing that reward feedback does not negate object prioritization. In Experiment 2, we corroborate the hypothesis that reward prioritization and object prioritization emerge concurrently. In Experiment 3, we find that reward prioritization and object prioritization sustain and interact in extinction, when reward feedback is discontinued. We verify this interaction in Experiment 4, linking it to task experience rather than the strategic utility of the reward association. Results suggest that information gathered from locations on reward-associated objects gains preferential access to cognition. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":50195,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Human Perception and Performance","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.1,"publicationDate":"2024-03-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139998128","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Separating facilitation and interference in backward crosstalk. 分离后向串扰中的促进和干扰。
IF 2.1 3区 心理学 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2024-03-01 DOI: 10.1037/xhp0001184
Valentin Koob, Carlotta Sauerbier, Hannes Schröter, Rolf Ulrich, Markus Janczyk

When two speeded tasks have spatially overlapping responses, preactivated Task 2 (T2) response information influences Task 1 (T1) response selection, a phenomenon known as the backward crosstalk effect (BCE). Current models of the BCE implicitly assume that T2 response information is equally present in trials requiring compatible or incompatible responses, such that T1 performance improves when T2 requires a compatible response and deteriorates when T2 requires an incompatible response. Thus, T2 response information should have a facilitatory and an interfering effect on T1. Interestingly, this hypothesis has never been tested, and the present study (conducted between 2021 and 2023) attempts to fill this gap by using neutral trials in which T2 responses did not spatially overlap with those in T1. The results suggest that the BCE (in T1) reflects both facilitation and interference effects of comparable magnitude, thus corroborating current conceptualizations of the BCE. We also observed an unexpected pattern of effects for T2, with only an interference effect, but no facilitation effect. Additional experiments led us to conclude that the T2 result was sensitive to the specific task characteristics. Conclusions about how the crosstalk transfers from T1 to T2 when switching tasks are therefore not possible. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

当两个加速任务的反应在空间上重叠时,预先激活的任务 2(T2)反应信息会影响任务 1(T1)的反应选择,这种现象被称为后向串扰效应(BCE)。目前的 BCE 模型隐含的假设是,T2 反应信息在需要相容或不相容反应的试验中同样存在,因此当 T2 需要相容反应时,T1 成绩会提高,而当 T2 需要不相容反应时,T1 成绩会下降。因此,T2 反应信息应该对 T1 有促进和干扰作用。有趣的是,这一假设从未得到过验证,而本研究(2021 年至 2023 年进行)试图通过使用 T2 反应与 T1 反应在空间上不重叠的中性试验来填补这一空白。结果表明,(T1 中的)BCE 反映了程度相当的促进和干扰效应,从而证实了当前对 BCE 的概念化。在 T2 中,我们还观察到一种意想不到的效应模式,即只有干扰效应,而没有促进效应。通过更多的实验,我们得出结论:T2 的结果对特定的任务特征很敏感。因此,我们无法就切换任务时串扰如何从 T1 转移到 T2 得出结论。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA,保留所有权利)。
{"title":"Separating facilitation and interference in backward crosstalk.","authors":"Valentin Koob, Carlotta Sauerbier, Hannes Schröter, Rolf Ulrich, Markus Janczyk","doi":"10.1037/xhp0001184","DOIUrl":"10.1037/xhp0001184","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>When two speeded tasks have spatially overlapping responses, preactivated Task 2 (T2) response information influences Task 1 (T1) response selection, a phenomenon known as the backward crosstalk effect (BCE). Current models of the BCE implicitly assume that T2 response information is equally present in trials requiring compatible or incompatible responses, such that T1 performance improves when T2 requires a compatible response and deteriorates when T2 requires an incompatible response. Thus, T2 response information should have a facilitatory and an interfering effect on T1. Interestingly, this hypothesis has never been tested, and the present study (conducted between 2021 and 2023) attempts to fill this gap by using neutral trials in which T2 responses did not spatially overlap with those in T1. The results suggest that the BCE (in T1) reflects both facilitation and interference effects of comparable magnitude, thus corroborating current conceptualizations of the BCE. We also observed an unexpected pattern of effects for T2, with only an interference effect, but no facilitation effect. Additional experiments led us to conclude that the T2 result was sensitive to the specific task characteristics. Conclusions about how the crosstalk transfers from T1 to T2 when switching tasks are therefore not possible. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":50195,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Human Perception and Performance","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.1,"publicationDate":"2024-03-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139998129","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Flexibility by association? No evidence for an influence of cue-transition associations on voluntary task switching. 联想的灵活性?没有证据表明线索-过渡联想对自愿任务转换有影响。
IF 2.1 3区 心理学 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2024-03-01 DOI: 10.1037/xhp0001186
Jonathan Mendl, Kerstin Fröber, Gesine Dreisbach

Some situations require cognitive flexibility, whereas others call for cognitive stability. Recent theories posit lower-level associative learning processes as the basis of contextual control. The present study incorporates six experiments to investigate whether cognitive flexibility can be triggered by task-irrelevant color cues in the task-switching paradigm. In the first learning phase, the cue colors were repeatedly paired with certain task transitions (repetition, switch) without explicit instruction. In the following test phase, voluntary trials were intermixed (where participants can freely choose the task) to measure the voluntary switch rate (VSR) in response to the color cues. For Experiment 2a, cue size and duration were increased, and the learning phase was extended. Additionally, in Experiment 2b, the second half of the test phase consisted of 100% free choices. Experiment 3 contained catch trials to ensure cue processing. In Experiment 4, two tasks of unequal difficulty were used. Experiments 1-4 provided evidence for the null hypothesis indicating no effect of the transition association on the VSR (all BF₁₀ < 0.265). The control Experiment 5 ruled out that the null effect was due to the insensitivity of the paradigm. Therefore, flexibility by association appears to be harder to achieve than recent accounts suggest. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

有些情况需要认知的灵活性,而另一些情况则需要认知的稳定性。最近的理论认为,低级联想学习过程是情境控制的基础。本研究通过六项实验来研究在任务转换范式中,与任务无关的颜色线索是否能触发认知灵活性。在第一个学习阶段,在没有明确指令的情况下,提示颜色与某些任务转换(重复、切换)反复配对。在接下来的测试阶段,自愿试验(参与者可以自由选择任务)被混合在一起,以测量参与者对颜色线索的自愿转换率(VSR)。实验 2a 增加了提示的大小和持续时间,并延长了学习阶段。此外,在实验 2b 中,测试阶段的后半段由 100% 的自由选择组成。实验 3 包含捕捉试验,以确保对线索的处理。在实验 4 中,使用了两个难度不等的任务。实验 1-4 为零假设提供了证据,表明过渡联想对 VSR 没有影响(所有 BF₁₀ < 0.265)。对照实验 5 排除了由于范式不敏感而导致的无效效应。因此,联想灵活性似乎比最近的说法更难实现。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved)。
{"title":"Flexibility by association? No evidence for an influence of cue-transition associations on voluntary task switching.","authors":"Jonathan Mendl, Kerstin Fröber, Gesine Dreisbach","doi":"10.1037/xhp0001186","DOIUrl":"10.1037/xhp0001186","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Some situations require cognitive flexibility, whereas others call for cognitive stability. Recent theories posit lower-level associative learning processes as the basis of contextual control. The present study incorporates six experiments to investigate whether cognitive flexibility can be triggered by task-irrelevant color cues in the task-switching paradigm. In the first learning phase, the cue colors were repeatedly paired with certain task transitions (repetition, switch) without explicit instruction. In the following test phase, voluntary trials were intermixed (where participants can freely choose the task) to measure the voluntary switch rate (VSR) in response to the color cues. For Experiment 2a, cue size and duration were increased, and the learning phase was extended. Additionally, in Experiment 2b, the second half of the test phase consisted of 100% free choices. Experiment 3 contained catch trials to ensure cue processing. In Experiment 4, two tasks of unequal difficulty were used. Experiments 1-4 provided evidence for the null hypothesis indicating no effect of the transition association on the VSR (all BF₁₀ < 0.265). The control Experiment 5 ruled out that the null effect was due to the insensitivity of the paradigm. Therefore, flexibility by association appears to be harder to achieve than recent accounts suggest. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":50195,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Human Perception and Performance","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.1,"publicationDate":"2024-03-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139998071","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Expected events dilate subjective duration in the auditory modality: Effects of predictability and expectation on time perception. 预期事件会延长听觉模式中的主观持续时间:可预测性和预期对时间感知的影响。
IF 2.1 3区 心理学 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2024-03-01 DOI: 10.1037/xhp0001179
Nariman Utegaliyev, Christoph von Castell

In timing research, repeated stimuli have been shown to have a shortening effect on time perception compared to novel stimuli. This finding had been attributed to repeated stimuli being more expected and, thus, less arousing and/or attended, or eliciting less neuronal activation due to repetition suppression, which results in temporal underestimation. However, more recent studies in the visual domain that disentangled effects of repetition and expectation suggest a more nuanced interpretation. In these studies, repetition led to temporal contraction while expectation caused subjective dilation of time. It was argued that expectations increase the perceptual strength of the stimulus, which leads to temporal overestimation, while repetitions reduce perceptual strength, which then leads to temporal underestimation. In the present study, we sought to further elaborate on these findings using auditory stimuli. In Experiment 1, we used an implicit method to induce expectation and manipulated the probability of stimulus repetition block-wise in a two-stimulus paradigm with auditory tones. Our findings were in line with the recent findings. When repetitions were less frequent, that is, less expected, we found clear evidence for perceived temporal contraction of repetitions. In contrast, when repetitions were more expected, the shortening effect of stimulus repetition on subjective duration disappeared. In Experiment 2, participants explicitly generated expectations about an upcoming tone in a temporal bisection paradigm. In trials, where expectations were fulfilled, presentation durations were perceived longer compared to trials with unfulfilled expectations. Our findings suggest that factors that increase the perceptual strength of a stimulus contribute to subjective temporal dilation. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

在定时研究中,与新刺激相比,重复刺激对时间知觉的影响被证明是缩短的。这一研究结果被认为是由于重复刺激的预期性更强,因此唤起和/或注意的程度更低,或者由于重复抑制而引起的神经元激活更少,从而导致时间估计不足。然而,最近在视觉领域进行的研究将重复和期望的影响区分开来,提出了一种更细致的解释。在这些研究中,重复会导致时间收缩,而期望则会导致时间的主观扩张。有观点认为,期望会增加刺激的知觉强度,从而导致时间上的高估,而重复会降低知觉强度,从而导致时间上的低估。在本研究中,我们试图利用听觉刺激来进一步阐述这些发现。在实验 1 中,我们使用了一种内隐方法来诱导期望,并在听觉音调的双刺激范式中分块操纵刺激重复的概率。我们的研究结果与最近的研究结果一致。当重复的频率较低,即预期较低时,我们发现了明显的证据表明重复的时间收缩感知。相反,当重复的预期次数较多时,刺激重复对主观持续时间的缩短效应就消失了。在实验 2 中,受试者在时间分割范式中明确地对即将到来的音调产生了预期。在预期实现的试验中,与预期未实现的试验相比,呈现持续时间被认为更长。我们的研究结果表明,增加刺激感知强度的因素有助于主观时间扩张。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Expected events dilate subjective duration in the auditory modality: Effects of predictability and expectation on time perception.","authors":"Nariman Utegaliyev, Christoph von Castell","doi":"10.1037/xhp0001179","DOIUrl":"10.1037/xhp0001179","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>In timing research, repeated stimuli have been shown to have a shortening effect on time perception compared to novel stimuli. This finding had been attributed to repeated stimuli being more expected and, thus, less arousing and/or attended, or eliciting less neuronal activation due to repetition suppression, which results in temporal underestimation. However, more recent studies in the visual domain that disentangled effects of repetition and expectation suggest a more nuanced interpretation. In these studies, repetition led to temporal contraction while expectation caused subjective dilation of time. It was argued that expectations increase the perceptual strength of the stimulus, which leads to temporal overestimation, while repetitions reduce perceptual strength, which then leads to temporal underestimation. In the present study, we sought to further elaborate on these findings using auditory stimuli. In Experiment 1, we used an implicit method to induce expectation and manipulated the probability of stimulus repetition block-wise in a two-stimulus paradigm with auditory tones. Our findings were in line with the recent findings. When repetitions were less frequent, that is, less expected, we found clear evidence for perceived temporal contraction of repetitions. In contrast, when repetitions were more expected, the shortening effect of stimulus repetition on subjective duration disappeared. In Experiment 2, participants explicitly generated expectations about an upcoming tone in a temporal bisection paradigm. In trials, where expectations were fulfilled, presentation durations were perceived longer compared to trials with unfulfilled expectations. Our findings suggest that factors that increase the perceptual strength of a stimulus contribute to subjective temporal dilation. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":50195,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Human Perception and Performance","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.1,"publicationDate":"2024-03-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139998070","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Journal of Experimental Psychology-Human Perception and Performance
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1