首页 > 最新文献

Journal of Experimental Psychology-Human Perception and Performance最新文献

英文 中文
The one exception: The impact of statistical regularities on explicit sense of agency. 一个例外:统计规律对明确代入感的影响。
IF 2.1 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-11-01 Epub Date: 2024-09-19 DOI: 10.1037/xhp0001243
Olivia Seubert, Robrecht van der Wel, Moritz Reis, Roland Pfister, Katharina A Schwarz

Establishing causal beliefs by observing regularities between actions and events in the environment is a crucial part of goal-directed behavior. Sense of agency (SoA) describes the corresponding experience of generating and controlling actions and subsequent events. Investigating how SoA adapts to situational changes in action-effect contingency, we observed even singular disturbances of perfect action-effect contingencies to yield a striking impact on SoA formation. Moreover, we additionally included disturbances of regularity that are not directly linked to one's own actions. Doing so allowed us to investigate how SoA might be a concept that goes beyond own actions toward a more generalized, subjective representation of control regarding environmental events. Indeed, the present experiments establish that, while SoA is highly tuned toward action-effect relations, it is also sensitive to events that occur without one's own action contribution. SoA thus appears to be exceptionally sensitive to singular breakpoints of perfect control with agents disproportionally incorporating such events during SoA formation while at the same time building on a rich situation model. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

通过观察环境中行动与事件之间的规律性来建立因果信念,是目标导向行为的重要组成部分。代理感(SoA)描述了产生和控制行动及后续事件的相应体验。在研究 SoA 如何适应行动-效果或然性的情境变化时,我们观察到即使是完美行动-效果或然性的单一干扰也会对 SoA 的形成产生显著影响。此外,我们还加入了与个人行为没有直接联系的规律性干扰。这样一来,我们就可以研究 SoA 这个概念是如何超越自身行动,成为一种更普遍、更主观的对环境事件控制的表征。事实上,目前的实验证实,虽然 SoA 高度关注行动-效果关系,但它对没有自身行动参与的事件也很敏感。因此,SoA 似乎对完美控制的奇异断点异常敏感,在形成 SoA 的过程中,行为主体会不成比例地将此类事件纳入其中,同时建立一个丰富的情境模型。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved)。
{"title":"The one exception: The impact of statistical regularities on explicit sense of agency.","authors":"Olivia Seubert, Robrecht van der Wel, Moritz Reis, Roland Pfister, Katharina A Schwarz","doi":"10.1037/xhp0001243","DOIUrl":"10.1037/xhp0001243","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Establishing causal beliefs by observing regularities between actions and events in the environment is a crucial part of goal-directed behavior. Sense of agency (SoA) describes the corresponding experience of generating and controlling actions and subsequent events. Investigating how SoA adapts to situational changes in action-effect contingency, we observed even singular disturbances of perfect action-effect contingencies to yield a striking impact on SoA formation. Moreover, we additionally included disturbances of regularity that are not directly linked to one's own actions. Doing so allowed us to investigate how SoA might be a concept that goes beyond own actions toward a more generalized, subjective representation of control regarding environmental events. Indeed, the present experiments establish that, while SoA is highly tuned toward action-effect relations, it is also sensitive to events that occur without one's own action contribution. SoA thus appears to be exceptionally sensitive to singular breakpoints of perfect control with agents disproportionally incorporating such events during SoA formation while at the same time building on a rich situation model. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":50195,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Human Perception and Performance","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.1,"publicationDate":"2024-11-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142299849","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Toward a better approach for measuring visual-search slopes. 为测量视觉搜索斜率提供更好的方法。
IF 2.1 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-11-01 Epub Date: 2024-09-19 DOI: 10.1037/xhp0001238
Talke Michaelsen, Markus Janczyk, Heinrich R Liesefeld

The slope of the function relating response times to the number of stimuli in a visual-search display is commonly considered a measure of search speed and is extensively used to test theories of visual cognition. Unfortunately, this important measure is confounded in multiple ways so that many classical findings in the literature must be called into question. As a way out of this predicament, we here develop a new technique to measure search speed (data collected in 2022 and 2023): Instead of manipulating the number of stimuli that need to be searched via a set-size manipulation, we achieve the intended purpose by placing the search target at different spatial positions with respect to an a-priori-known search order. Reliably inducing such a search order is the main achievement of the present study, but we also report several additional data patterns that might turn out instrumental for future research on visual attention. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

在视觉搜索显示中,反应时间与刺激物数量之间的函数斜率通常被认为是搜索速度的衡量标准,并被广泛用于检验视觉认知理论。遗憾的是,这一重要的测量指标受到多方面的干扰,因此许多经典的文献研究结果都必须受到质疑。为了摆脱这一困境,我们在此开发了一种测量搜索速度的新技术(数据收集于 2022 年和 2023 年):我们不是通过设定大小来操纵需要搜索的刺激物的数量,而是通过将搜索目标放置在不同的空间位置,并按照事先已知的搜索顺序来达到预期目的。可靠地诱导这种搜索顺序是本研究的主要成就,但我们也报告了几种额外的数据模式,它们可能会对未来的视觉注意力研究有所帮助。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Toward a better approach for measuring visual-search slopes.","authors":"Talke Michaelsen, Markus Janczyk, Heinrich R Liesefeld","doi":"10.1037/xhp0001238","DOIUrl":"10.1037/xhp0001238","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The slope of the function relating response times to the number of stimuli in a visual-search display is commonly considered a measure of search speed and is extensively used to test theories of visual cognition. Unfortunately, this important measure is confounded in multiple ways so that many classical findings in the literature must be called into question. As a way out of this predicament, we here develop a new technique to measure search speed (data collected in 2022 and 2023): Instead of manipulating the number of stimuli that need to be searched via a set-size manipulation, we achieve the intended purpose by placing the search target at different spatial positions with respect to an a-priori-known search order. Reliably inducing such a search order is the main achievement of the present study, but we also report several additional data patterns that might turn out instrumental for future research on visual attention. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":50195,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Human Perception and Performance","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.1,"publicationDate":"2024-11-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142299850","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Determinants of shared and idiosyncratic contributions to judgments of faces. 对人脸判断的共同贡献和特异贡献的决定因素。
IF 2.1 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-11-01 Epub Date: 2024-09-19 DOI: 10.1037/xhp0001239
Daniel N Albohn, Joel E Martinez, Alexander Todorov

Recent work has shown that the idiosyncrasies of the observer can contribute more to the variance of social judgments of faces than the features of the faces. However, it is unclear what conditions determine the relative contributions of shared and idiosyncratic variance. Here, we examine two conditions: type of judgment and diversity of face stimuli. First, we show that for simpler, directly observable judgments that are consistent across observers (e.g., masculinity) shared exceeds idiosyncratic variance, whereas for more complex and less directly observable judgments (e.g., trustworthiness), idiosyncratic exceeds shared variance. Second, we show that judgments of more diverse face images increase the amount of shared variance. Finally, using machine-learning methods, we examine how stimulus (e.g., incidental emotion resemblance, skin luminosity) and observer variables (e.g., race, age) contribute to shared and idiosyncratic variance of judgments. Overall, our results indicate that an observer's age is the most consistent and best predictor of idiosyncratic variance contributions to face judgments measured in the current research. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

最近的研究表明,观察者的特异性比人脸的特征对人脸的社会判断差异的贡献更大。然而,目前还不清楚是什么条件决定了共同变异和特异变异的相对贡献。在这里,我们研究了两个条件:判断类型和人脸刺激的多样性。首先,我们表明,对于较简单的、可直接观察到的、不同观察者之间一致的判断(如男性气质),共享方差超过了特异方差;而对于较复杂的、可直接观察到的较少的判断(如可信度),特异方差超过了共享方差。其次,我们表明,对更多样化的人脸图像的判断会增加共享方差的数量。最后,我们使用机器学习方法,研究了刺激(如偶然的情感相似性、皮肤亮度)和观察者变量(如种族、年龄)如何导致判断的共享方差和特异方差。总之,我们的研究结果表明,在目前的研究中,观察者的年龄是对人脸判断的特异性差异贡献最一致和最好的预测因素。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Determinants of shared and idiosyncratic contributions to judgments of faces.","authors":"Daniel N Albohn, Joel E Martinez, Alexander Todorov","doi":"10.1037/xhp0001239","DOIUrl":"10.1037/xhp0001239","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Recent work has shown that the idiosyncrasies of the observer can contribute more to the variance of social judgments of faces than the features of the faces. However, it is unclear what conditions determine the relative contributions of shared and idiosyncratic variance. Here, we examine two conditions: type of judgment and diversity of face stimuli. First, we show that for simpler, directly observable judgments that are consistent across observers (e.g., masculinity) shared exceeds idiosyncratic variance, whereas for more complex and less directly observable judgments (e.g., trustworthiness), idiosyncratic exceeds shared variance. Second, we show that judgments of more diverse face images increase the amount of shared variance. Finally, using machine-learning methods, we examine how stimulus (e.g., incidental emotion resemblance, skin luminosity) and observer variables (e.g., race, age) contribute to shared and idiosyncratic variance of judgments. Overall, our results indicate that an observer's age is the most consistent and best predictor of idiosyncratic variance contributions to face judgments measured in the current research. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":50195,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Human Perception and Performance","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.1,"publicationDate":"2024-11-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142299847","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Mind wandering is associated with worsening attentional vigilance. 思维游离与注意力警觉性的下降有关。
IF 2.1 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-11-01 Epub Date: 2024-08-22 DOI: 10.1037/xhp0001233
Anthony P Zanesco, Ekaterina Denkova, Jordan Barry, Amishi P Jha

The tendency for our minds to wander is a pervasive and disruptive influence on continued task performance. Models of sustained attention have implicated mind wandering, moments when attention has turned inwards toward task-unrelated thought, in characteristic patterns of worsening performance with greater time-on-task, known as the vigilance decrement. Despite their theoretical connection, associations between mind wandering and the vigilance decrement have not been investigated systematically. Across two studies (N = 730), we evaluated covariance between within-task change in rates of probe-caught mind wandering and patterns of worsening behavioral task performance that characterize the vigilance decrement. Bivariate growth curve models characterized patterns of intraindividual linear change in mind wandering alongside concomitant changes in task accuracy, response time (RT), and RT variability. Importantly, models assessing the covariance between intraindividual change in mind wandering and behavioral outcome measures confirmed that increases in mind wandering are associated with patterns of worsening behavioral performance with greater time-on-task. In addition, we investigated the role of several moderating factors associated with patterns of within-task change: self-reported task interest and motivation, and individuals' propensity for mind wandering, and mindfulness in their daily lives. These factors moderated either the overall level or rate of within-task change in mind wandering. Our results provide support for models of sustained attention that directly implicate mind wandering in worsening behavioral performance with greater time-on-task in continuous performance tasks requiring sustained attention. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

我们的思维容易游离,这对持续完成任务具有普遍的破坏性影响。持续注意力模型认为,当注意力转向与任务无关的内向思维时,就会出现思维游离,而随着任务时间的延长,这种思维游离会导致任务表现的下降,这就是所谓的警觉性下降(t vigilance decrement)。尽管两者之间存在理论联系,但注意力游离和警觉性下降之间的关联尚未得到系统研究。在两项研究(N = 730)中,我们评估了任务内探针捕捉到的思维游移率的变化与作为警觉性下降特征的行为任务表现恶化模式之间的协方差。二元增长曲线模型描述了思维游移的个体内线性变化模式,以及任务准确性、反应时间(RT)和 RT 变异性的伴随变化。重要的是,评估思维游移个体内变化与行为结果测量之间协方差的模型证实,思维游移的增加与任务时间越长行为表现越差的模式相关。此外,我们还研究了与任务内变化模式相关的几个调节因素的作用:自我报告的任务兴趣和动机、思维游离倾向以及日常生活中的正念。这些因素对思维游移的总体水平或任务内变化率起着调节作用。我们的研究结果为持续注意力模型提供了支持,该模型直接指出,在需要持续注意力的连续性任务中,随着任务时间的延长,思维游离会导致行为表现的恶化。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Mind wandering is associated with worsening attentional vigilance.","authors":"Anthony P Zanesco, Ekaterina Denkova, Jordan Barry, Amishi P Jha","doi":"10.1037/xhp0001233","DOIUrl":"10.1037/xhp0001233","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The tendency for our minds to wander is a pervasive and disruptive influence on continued task performance. Models of sustained attention have implicated mind wandering, moments when attention has turned inwards toward task-unrelated thought, in characteristic patterns of worsening performance with greater time-on-task, known as the vigilance decrement. Despite their theoretical connection, associations between mind wandering and the vigilance decrement have not been investigated systematically. Across two studies (<i>N</i> = 730), we evaluated covariance between within-task change in rates of probe-caught mind wandering and patterns of worsening behavioral task performance that characterize the vigilance decrement. Bivariate growth curve models characterized patterns of intraindividual linear change in mind wandering alongside concomitant changes in task accuracy, response time (RT), and RT variability. Importantly, models assessing the covariance between intraindividual change in mind wandering and behavioral outcome measures confirmed that increases in mind wandering are associated with patterns of worsening behavioral performance with greater time-on-task. In addition, we investigated the role of several moderating factors associated with patterns of within-task change: self-reported task interest and motivation, and individuals' propensity for mind wandering, and mindfulness in their daily lives. These factors moderated either the overall level or rate of within-task change in mind wandering. Our results provide support for models of sustained attention that directly implicate mind wandering in worsening behavioral performance with greater time-on-task in continuous performance tasks requiring sustained attention. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":50195,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Human Perception and Performance","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.1,"publicationDate":"2024-11-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142019438","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Salience effects on attentional selection are enabled by task relevance. 任务相关性会对注意选择产生显著性效应。
IF 2.1 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-11-01 Epub Date: 2024-09-23 DOI: 10.1037/xhp0001241
Yue Zhang, Nicholas Gaspelin

Attention is a limited resource that must be carefully controlled to prevent distraction. Much research has demonstrated that distraction can be prevented by proactively suppressing salient stimuli to prevent them from capturing attention. It has been suggested, however, that prior studies showing evidence of suppression may have used stimuli that were not truly salient. This claim has been difficult to test because there are currently no agreed-upon methods to demonstrate that an object is salient. The current study aims to help resolve this by introducing a new technique to test the role of salience in attentional capture. Low- and high-salience singletons were generated via a manipulation of color contrast. An initial experiment then verified the manipulation of salience using a search task where the color singleton was the target and could only be found via its bottom-up popout. High-salience singletons were found much more easily than low-salience singletons, suggesting that salience powerfully influenced attention when task relevant. A following experiment then used the same stimulus displays but adapted the task so that the singletons were task-irrelevant distractors. Both low- and high-salience singletons were suppressed, suggesting neither was able to capture attention. These results challenge purely stimulus-driven accounts by showing that improving salience only enhances attentional allocation in situations where the object is also task relevant. The results are instead consistent with the signal suppression hypothesis, which predicts that task-irrelevant singletons can be suppressed. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

注意力是一种有限的资源,必须加以谨慎控制,以防止注意力分散。许多研究表明,可以通过主动抑制突出刺激来防止注意力分散。然而,有人认为,之前有证据显示抑制作用的研究可能使用了并非真正突出的刺激物。这种说法一直难以验证,因为目前还没有公认的方法来证明一个物体是突出的。本研究旨在通过引入一种新技术来测试显著性在注意力捕捉中的作用,从而帮助解决这一问题。通过对颜色对比度的操纵,产生了低显著性和高显著性的单子。最初的实验使用搜索任务验证了对显著性的操纵,在该任务中,颜色单子是目标,只能通过其自下而上的弹出窗口找到。高显著性的单体比低显著性的单体更容易被发现,这表明当任务相关时,显著性会对注意力产生强大的影响。接下来的实验使用了相同的刺激显示,但对任务进行了调整,使单子成为与任务无关的干扰物。低显著性和高显著性单子都被抑制了,这表明两者都无法吸引注意力。这些结果对纯刺激驱动的说法提出了质疑,表明只有在对象也与任务相关的情况下,提高显著性才能增强注意力分配。这些结果反而与信号抑制假说相一致,该假说预测与任务无关的单子会被抑制。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Salience effects on attentional selection are enabled by task relevance.","authors":"Yue Zhang, Nicholas Gaspelin","doi":"10.1037/xhp0001241","DOIUrl":"10.1037/xhp0001241","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Attention is a limited resource that must be carefully controlled to prevent distraction. Much research has demonstrated that distraction can be prevented by proactively suppressing salient stimuli to prevent them from capturing attention. It has been suggested, however, that prior studies showing evidence of suppression may have used stimuli that were not truly salient. This claim has been difficult to test because there are currently no agreed-upon methods to demonstrate that an object is salient. The current study aims to help resolve this by introducing a new technique to test the role of salience in attentional capture. Low- and high-salience singletons were generated via a manipulation of color contrast. An initial experiment then verified the manipulation of salience using a search task where the color singleton was the target and could only be found via its bottom-up popout. High-salience singletons were found much more easily than low-salience singletons, suggesting that salience powerfully influenced attention when task relevant. A following experiment then used the same stimulus displays but adapted the task so that the singletons were task-irrelevant distractors. Both low- and high-salience singletons were suppressed, suggesting neither was able to capture attention. These results challenge purely stimulus-driven accounts by showing that improving salience only enhances attentional allocation in situations where the object is also task relevant. The results are instead consistent with the signal suppression hypothesis, which predicts that task-irrelevant singletons can be suppressed. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":50195,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Human Perception and Performance","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.1,"publicationDate":"2024-11-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142299848","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Ordinal information, but not metric information, matters in binding feature with depth location in three-dimensional contexts. 在三维语境中,将特征与深度位置结合起来的关键是排序信息,而不是度量信息。
IF 2.1 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-11-01 Epub Date: 2024-08-22 DOI: 10.1037/xhp0001228
Jiehui Qian, Tian Zheng, Binglong Li

A basic function of human visual perception is the ability to recognize and locate objects in the environment. It has been shown that two-dimensional (2D) location can reliably bias judgments on object identity (spatial congruency bias; Golomb et al., 2014), suggesting that 2D location information is automatically bound with object features to induce such a bias. Although the binding problem of feature and location has been vigorously studied under various 2D settings, it remains unclear how depth location can be bound with object features in a three-dimensional (3D) setting. Here we conducted five experiments in various 3D contexts using the congruency bias paradigm, and found that changes of object's depth location could influence perceptual judgments on object features differently depending on whether its relative depth order with respect to others changed or not. Experiments 1 and 2 showed that the judgments on an object's color could be affected by changes in its ordinal depth, but not by changes in its absolute metric depth. Experiment 3 showed that the bias was asymmetric-changes in an object's color did bias the judgments on metric-depth location, but not if its depth order had changed. Experiments 4 and 5 investigated whether these findings could be generalized to a peripersonal near space and a large-scale far space, respectively, using more ecological virtual environments. Our findings suggest that ordinal depth plays a special role in feature-location binding: an object may be automatically bound with its relative depth relation, but not with its absolute metric-depth location. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

人类视觉感知的一个基本功能是识别和定位环境中的物体。有研究表明,二维(2D)位置会对物体特征的判断产生可靠的偏差(空间一致性偏差;Golomb 等人,2014 年),这表明二维位置信息会自动与物体特征绑定,从而诱发这种偏差。虽然在各种二维环境下,特征与位置的结合问题已经得到了深入研究,但深度位置如何与三维(3D)环境下的物体特征结合,目前仍不清楚。在此,我们使用一致性偏差范式在各种三维环境下进行了五项实验,发现物体深度位置的变化会根据其相对于其他物体的深度顺序是否发生变化而对物体特征的知觉判断产生不同的影响。实验 1 和 2 表明,对物体颜色的判断会受到物体深度顺序变化的影响,但不会受到物体绝对度量深度变化的影响。实验 3 表明,这种偏差是不对称的--物体颜色的变化确实会使对度量深度位置的判断产生偏差,但如果物体的深度顺序发生了变化,这种偏差就不会产生。实验 4 和实验 5 利用更加生态化的虚拟环境,研究了这些发现是否可以分别推广到近距离空间和远距离空间。我们的研究结果表明,深度顺序在特征-位置绑定中起着特殊作用:一个物体可能会自动与其相对深度关系绑定,但不会与其绝对度量深度位置绑定。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved)。
{"title":"Ordinal information, but not metric information, matters in binding feature with depth location in three-dimensional contexts.","authors":"Jiehui Qian, Tian Zheng, Binglong Li","doi":"10.1037/xhp0001228","DOIUrl":"10.1037/xhp0001228","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>A basic function of human visual perception is the ability to recognize and locate objects in the environment. It has been shown that two-dimensional (2D) location can reliably bias judgments on object identity (spatial congruency bias; Golomb et al., 2014), suggesting that 2D location information is automatically bound with object features to induce such a bias. Although the binding problem of feature and location has been vigorously studied under various 2D settings, it remains unclear how depth location can be bound with object features in a three-dimensional (3D) setting. Here we conducted five experiments in various 3D contexts using the congruency bias paradigm, and found that changes of object's depth location could influence perceptual judgments on object features differently depending on whether its relative depth order with respect to others changed or not. Experiments 1 and 2 showed that the judgments on an object's color could be affected by changes in its ordinal depth, but not by changes in its absolute metric depth. Experiment 3 showed that the bias was asymmetric-changes in an object's color did bias the judgments on metric-depth location, but not if its depth order had changed. Experiments 4 and 5 investigated whether these findings could be generalized to a peripersonal near space and a large-scale far space, respectively, using more ecological virtual environments. Our findings suggest that ordinal depth plays a special role in feature-location binding: an object may be automatically bound with its relative depth relation, but not with its absolute metric-depth location. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":50195,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Human Perception and Performance","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.1,"publicationDate":"2024-11-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142019439","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Perceived duration of visual stimuli contracts due to crowding. 由于拥挤,视觉刺激的感知持续时间会缩短。
IF 2.1 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-10-31 DOI: 10.1037/xhp0001246
Sofia Nagisa, Ikuya Murakami

Recent research on duration perception suggests that duration encoding is not a single general process but involves several separate processes, some of which are specific to visual modality. Moreover, different functional aspects of visual processing can influence duration perception in distinct ways. One of the most important functions of the visual system is to identify and recognize features, shapes, and objects. However, it is still unclear whether and how computations related to these processes affect duration perception. To clarify this issue, we used a spatial crowding phenomenon, which allows the dissociation of low-level feature extraction from high-level processes such as object recognition. We created letter and vernier stimuli matched for their low-level properties but different in their discriminability due to spatial crowding. Here, we show that stimuli that became more difficult to discriminate appeared shorter in duration (data collected in 2019-2023). This difference in perceived duration could not be explained by low-level stimulus properties, cognitive bias due to discriminability, or perceived stimulus onsets or offsets. These results suggest the existence of time-sensitive structures specific to visual processing of features, shapes, and objects that is affected by crowding. These findings support the notion of distributed timing mechanisms in the visual system. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

最近关于时长感知的研究表明,时长编码不是一个单一的一般过程,而是涉及多个独立的过程,其中一些过程是视觉模式所特有的。此外,视觉处理的不同功能方面会以不同的方式影响时长感知。视觉系统最重要的功能之一是识别和辨认特征、形状和物体。然而,与这些过程相关的计算是否会影响时长感知以及如何影响时长感知,目前仍不清楚。为了澄清这个问题,我们使用了空间拥挤现象,这种现象可以将低级特征提取与高级过程(如物体识别)分离开来。我们创造了字母和游标刺激物,这两种刺激物的低级特征相匹配,但由于空间拥挤,它们的可辨别性却不同。在这里,我们表明,越难辨别的刺激物持续时间越短(数据收集于 2019-2023 年)。这种感知持续时间上的差异无法用低级刺激属性、可分辨性导致的认知偏差或感知刺激的起始或终止来解释。这些结果表明,在对特征、形状和物体进行视觉处理时,存在受拥挤影响的时间敏感结构。这些发现支持了视觉系统中分布式定时机制的概念。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Perceived duration of visual stimuli contracts due to crowding.","authors":"Sofia Nagisa, Ikuya Murakami","doi":"10.1037/xhp0001246","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/xhp0001246","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Recent research on duration perception suggests that duration encoding is not a single general process but involves several separate processes, some of which are specific to visual modality. Moreover, different functional aspects of visual processing can influence duration perception in distinct ways. One of the most important functions of the visual system is to identify and recognize features, shapes, and objects. However, it is still unclear whether and how computations related to these processes affect duration perception. To clarify this issue, we used a spatial crowding phenomenon, which allows the dissociation of low-level feature extraction from high-level processes such as object recognition. We created letter and vernier stimuli matched for their low-level properties but different in their discriminability due to spatial crowding. Here, we show that stimuli that became more difficult to discriminate appeared shorter in duration (data collected in 2019-2023). This difference in perceived duration could not be explained by low-level stimulus properties, cognitive bias due to discriminability, or perceived stimulus onsets or offsets. These results suggest the existence of time-sensitive structures specific to visual processing of features, shapes, and objects that is affected by crowding. These findings support the notion of distributed timing mechanisms in the visual system. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":50195,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Human Perception and Performance","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.1,"publicationDate":"2024-10-31","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142548667","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Double training reveals an interval-invariant subsecond temporal structure in the brain. 双重训练揭示了大脑中间隔不变的亚秒级时间结构。
IF 2.1 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-10-31 DOI: 10.1037/xhp0001254
Shu-Chen Guan, Ying-Zi Xiong, Cong Yu

Subsecond temporal perception is critical for understanding time-varying events. Many studies suggest that subsecond timing is an intrinsic property of neural dynamics, distributed across sensory modalities and brain areas. However, our recent finding of the transfer of temporal interval discrimination (TID) learning across sensory modalities supports the existence of a more abstract and conceptual representation of subsecond time that guides the temporal processing of distributed mechanisms. One major challenge to this hypothesis is that TID learning consistently fails to transfer from trained intervals to untrained intervals. To address this issue, here, we examined whether this interval specificity can be removed with double training, a procedure originally developed to eliminate various specificities in visual perceptual learning. Specifically, participants practiced the primary TID task, the learning of which per se was specific to the trained interval (e.g., 100 ms). In addition, they also received exposure to a new interval (e.g., 200 ms) through a secondary and functionally independent tone-frequency discrimination task. This double training successfully enabled complete transfer of TID learning to the new interval, indicating that training improved an interval-invariant component of temporal interval perception, which supports our proposal of an abstract and conceptual representation of subsecond time in the brain. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

亚秒时间感知对于理解时变事件至关重要。许多研究表明,亚秒计时是神经动态的固有属性,分布于不同的感官模式和脑区。然而,我们最近发现的时间间隔辨别(TID)学习在不同感官模式间的转移支持了亚秒时间存在一种更抽象、更概念化的表征,这种表征指导着分布式机制的时间处理。这一假设面临的一个主要挑战是,时间间隔辨别学习始终无法从训练有素的时间间隔转移到未经训练的时间间隔。为了解决这个问题,我们在这里研究了是否可以通过双重训练来消除这种时间间隔特异性,这种训练方法最初是为了消除视觉感知学习中的各种特异性而开发的。具体来说,参与者练习主要的 TID 任务,其学习本身就是针对训练过的时间间隔(例如 100 毫秒)。此外,他们还通过一个次要的、功能独立的音调-频率辨别任务接触了一个新的间隔(如 200 毫秒)。这种双重训练成功地使TID学习完全转移到了新的时间间隔上,表明训练提高了时间间隔感知的时间间隔不变成分,这支持了我们提出的亚秒时间在大脑中的抽象和概念表征。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Double training reveals an interval-invariant subsecond temporal structure in the brain.","authors":"Shu-Chen Guan, Ying-Zi Xiong, Cong Yu","doi":"10.1037/xhp0001254","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/xhp0001254","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Subsecond temporal perception is critical for understanding time-varying events. Many studies suggest that subsecond timing is an intrinsic property of neural dynamics, distributed across sensory modalities and brain areas. However, our recent finding of the transfer of temporal interval discrimination (TID) learning across sensory modalities supports the existence of a more abstract and conceptual representation of subsecond time that guides the temporal processing of distributed mechanisms. One major challenge to this hypothesis is that TID learning consistently fails to transfer from trained intervals to untrained intervals. To address this issue, here, we examined whether this interval specificity can be removed with double training, a procedure originally developed to eliminate various specificities in visual perceptual learning. Specifically, participants practiced the primary TID task, the learning of which per se was specific to the trained interval (e.g., 100 ms). In addition, they also received exposure to a new interval (e.g., 200 ms) through a secondary and functionally independent tone-frequency discrimination task. This double training successfully enabled complete transfer of TID learning to the new interval, indicating that training improved an interval-invariant component of temporal interval perception, which supports our proposal of an abstract and conceptual representation of subsecond time in the brain. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":50195,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Human Perception and Performance","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.1,"publicationDate":"2024-10-31","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142548666","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Task-irrelevant inputs alter ensemble representations of faces within the spatial focus of attention. 与任务无关的输入会改变注意力空间焦点内的人脸集合表征。
IF 2.1 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-10-31 DOI: 10.1037/xhp0001249
Kevin Sayed, Viola S Störmer

Spatial attention enhances processing of information, but how does unattended and task-irrelevant information influence visual processing within the spatial focus of attention? We tested this by asking participants to extract the average emotional expression of a set of sequentially presented faces while simultaneously presenting task-irrelevant faces at a spatially unattended and task-irrelevant location. Across several experiments, we found that participants' reports of the emotional expression of faces at the attended location were biased toward the task-irrelevant faces. For example, when happier faces were presented at the unattended location, participants were biased to perceive the attended faces as happier. A control experiment in which participants were asked to also detect probes at cued and uncued locations showed that spatial attention was directed towards the cued location as instructed. Together, these results reveal that unattended and task-irrelevant inputs do not only affect the efficiency of target processing, for example by slowing responses or lowering accuracies, but that they can systematically bias ensemble representations within the spatial focus of attention. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

空间注意力能增强信息处理,但未关注和任务无关的信息如何影响注意力空间焦点内的视觉处理?我们通过让参与者提取一组顺序呈现的人脸的平均情绪表达来测试这一点,同时在空间上无人注意且与任务无关的位置呈现与任务无关的人脸。在多个实验中,我们发现参与者对被关注位置上的人脸的情绪表达的报告偏向于任务无关的人脸。例如,当较快乐的面孔出现在无人注意的位置时,参与者会偏向于认为有人注意的面孔更快乐。在一项对照实验中,参与者被要求同时检测提示位置和未提示位置上的探针,结果表明,空间注意力按照指示被导向了提示位置。这些结果共同揭示出,未被注意和与任务无关的输入不仅会影响目标处理的效率,例如减慢反应速度或降低准确率,而且会系统性地偏移注意空间焦点内的集合表征。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Task-irrelevant inputs alter ensemble representations of faces within the spatial focus of attention.","authors":"Kevin Sayed, Viola S Störmer","doi":"10.1037/xhp0001249","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/xhp0001249","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Spatial attention enhances processing of information, but how does unattended and task-irrelevant information influence visual processing within the spatial focus of attention? We tested this by asking participants to extract the average emotional expression of a set of sequentially presented faces while simultaneously presenting task-irrelevant faces at a spatially unattended and task-irrelevant location. Across several experiments, we found that participants' reports of the emotional expression of faces at the attended location were biased toward the task-irrelevant faces. For example, when happier faces were presented at the unattended location, participants were biased to perceive the attended faces as happier. A control experiment in which participants were asked to also detect probes at cued and uncued locations showed that spatial attention was directed towards the cued location as instructed. Together, these results reveal that unattended and task-irrelevant inputs do not only affect the efficiency of target processing, for example by slowing responses or lowering accuracies, but that they can systematically bias ensemble representations within the spatial focus of attention. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":50195,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Human Perception and Performance","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.1,"publicationDate":"2024-10-31","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142548668","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Integrated encoding of relations and objects in visual working memory. 视觉工作记忆中关系和对象的综合编码
IF 2.1 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-10-10 DOI: 10.1037/xhp0001248
Jianzhe Xu, Haokui Xu, Jing Chen, Chenya Gu, Jifan Zhou, Hui Chen, Mowei Shen

Comprehensive understanding of visual scenes necessitates grasping the relations among visual objects. Given the potentially pivotal role of visual working memory (VWM) in processing visual relations, it is important to investigate the representation of relations in VWM. In our previous study, we proposed the integrated storage hypothesis, postulating that relations and objects are stored together as an integrated structured representation in VWM. The present study aimed to test this hypothesis against the alternative separate encoding hypothesis by probing the irrelevant-distracting effect. Across three experiments, where participants memorized object shapes/colors while disregarding relations, an irrelevant-distracting effect was consistently observed across varying types of changes in relation and set sizes. Critically, recombining the probe with irrelevant relation from another memory item (Experiment 2) or reversing the relational roles of probed objects relative to the memory item (Experiment 3) were perceived as inconsistency with stored representations and impaired change detection. These findings supported the integrated storage hypothesis, indicating that the dynamic relations between the objects are automatically encoded alongside object identities to form an integrated structured representation. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

要全面理解视觉场景,就必须掌握视觉对象之间的关系。鉴于视觉工作记忆(VWM)在处理视觉关系中的潜在关键作用,研究关系在视觉工作记忆中的表征就显得尤为重要。在之前的研究中,我们提出了综合存储假说,认为在视觉工作记忆中,关系和对象作为一个综合的结构化表征被存储在一起。本研究旨在通过探究无关-分心效应来检验这一假说与独立编码假说之间的差异。在三项实验中,被试在记忆物体形状/颜色的同时忽略了关系,在关系和集合大小的不同变化类型中,都持续观察到了无关-分散效应。重要的是,将探针与另一个记忆项目中的无关关系重新组合(实验 2),或将探针对象相对于记忆项目的关系角色颠倒(实验 3),都会被视为与存储表征不一致,从而影响对变化的检测。这些发现支持了整合存储假说,表明对象之间的动态关系与对象的身份一起被自动编码,形成一个整合的结构化表征。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Integrated encoding of relations and objects in visual working memory.","authors":"Jianzhe Xu, Haokui Xu, Jing Chen, Chenya Gu, Jifan Zhou, Hui Chen, Mowei Shen","doi":"10.1037/xhp0001248","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/xhp0001248","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Comprehensive understanding of visual scenes necessitates grasping the relations among visual objects. Given the potentially pivotal role of visual working memory (VWM) in processing visual relations, it is important to investigate the representation of relations in VWM. In our previous study, we proposed the integrated storage hypothesis, postulating that relations and objects are stored together as an integrated structured representation in VWM. The present study aimed to test this hypothesis against the alternative separate encoding hypothesis by probing the irrelevant-distracting effect. Across three experiments, where participants memorized object shapes/colors while disregarding relations, an irrelevant-distracting effect was consistently observed across varying types of changes in relation and set sizes. Critically, recombining the probe with irrelevant relation from another memory item (Experiment 2) or reversing the relational roles of probed objects relative to the memory item (Experiment 3) were perceived as inconsistency with stored representations and impaired change detection. These findings supported the integrated storage hypothesis, indicating that the dynamic relations between the objects are automatically encoded alongside object identities to form an integrated structured representation. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":50195,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Human Perception and Performance","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.1,"publicationDate":"2024-10-10","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142479514","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Journal of Experimental Psychology-Human Perception and Performance
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1