首页 > 最新文献

Journal of Experimental Psychology-Human Perception and Performance最新文献

英文 中文
Characterising the role of awareness in ensemble perception. 描述意识在集合感知中的作用。
IF 2.1 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-07-01 Epub Date: 2024-04-25 DOI: 10.1037/xhp0001201
Patxi Elosegi, Ning Mei, David Soto

Ensemble representations are efficient codes that the brain generates effortlessly even under noisy conditions. However, the role of visual awareness for computing ensemble representations remains unclear. We present two psychophysical experiments (N = 15 × 2) using a bias-free paradigm to investigate the contribution of conscious and unconscious processing to ensemble perception. Here, we show that ensemble perception can unfold without awareness of the relevant features that define the ensemble. Computational modeling of the type-1 and type-2 drift-rates further suggest that awareness lags well behind the categorization processes that support ensemble perception. Additional evidence indicates that the dissociation between type-1 from type-2 sensitivity, was not driven by the type-2 inefficiency or a systematic disadvantage in type-2 decision making. The present study demonstrates the utility of robust measures for studying the role of visual consciousness and metacognition in stimuli and tasks of increasing complexity, crucially, without underestimating the contribution of unconscious processing in an otherwise visible stimulus. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

集合表征是大脑即使在嘈杂条件下也能毫不费力地生成的高效代码。然而,视觉意识在计算集合表征中的作用仍不清楚。我们展示了两个心理物理实验(N = 15 × 2),使用无偏差范式来研究有意识和无意识处理对集合感知的贡献。在这里,我们证明了集合感知可以在没有意识到定义集合的相关特征的情况下展开。对第一类和第二类漂移率的计算建模进一步表明,意识远远落后于支持集合感知的分类过程。更多证据表明,第一类与第二类敏感性之间的差异并非由第二类的低效率或第二类决策的系统性劣势所驱动。本研究表明,在研究视觉意识和元认知在日益复杂的刺激和任务中的作用时,稳健的测量方法非常有用,关键是不会低估无意识处理在其他可见刺激中的贡献。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Characterising the role of awareness in ensemble perception.","authors":"Patxi Elosegi, Ning Mei, David Soto","doi":"10.1037/xhp0001201","DOIUrl":"10.1037/xhp0001201","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Ensemble representations are efficient codes that the brain generates effortlessly even under noisy conditions. However, the role of visual awareness for computing ensemble representations remains unclear. We present two psychophysical experiments (<i>N</i> = 15 × 2) using a bias-free paradigm to investigate the contribution of conscious and unconscious processing to ensemble perception. Here, we show that ensemble perception can unfold without awareness of the relevant features that define the ensemble. Computational modeling of the type-1 and type-2 drift-rates further suggest that awareness lags well behind the categorization processes that support ensemble perception. Additional evidence indicates that the dissociation between type-1 from type-2 sensitivity, was not driven by the type-2 inefficiency or a systematic disadvantage in type-2 decision making. The present study demonstrates the utility of robust measures for studying the role of visual consciousness and metacognition in stimuli and tasks of increasing complexity, crucially, without underestimating the contribution of unconscious processing in an otherwise visible stimulus. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":50195,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Human Perception and Performance","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.1,"publicationDate":"2024-07-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140873267","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Statistical relationships between surface form and sensory meanings of English words influence lexical processing. 英语单词表面形式和感官意义之间的统计关系影响词汇加工。
IF 2.1 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-07-01 Epub Date: 2024-05-06 DOI: 10.1037/xhp0001209
Greig I de Zubicaray, Elaine Kearney, Frank Guenther, Katie L McMahon, Joanne Arciuli

Across spoken languages, there are some words whose acoustic features resemble the meanings of their referents by evoking perceptual imagery, i.e., they are iconic (e.g., in English, "splash" imitates the sound of an object hitting water). While these sound symbolic form-meaning relationships are well-studied, relatively little work has explored whether the sensory properties of English words also involve systematic (i.e., statistical) form-meaning mappings. We first test the prediction that surface form properties can predict sensory experience ratings for over 5,000 monosyllabic and disyllabic words (Juhasz & Yap, 2013), confirming they explain a significant proportion of variance. Next, we show that iconicity and sensory form typicality, a statistical measure of how well a word's form aligns with its sensory experience rating, are only weakly related to each other, indicating they are likely to be distinct constructs. To determine whether form typicality influences processing of sensory words, we conducted regression analyses using lexical decision, word recognition, naming and semantic decision tasks from behavioral megastudy data sets. Across the data sets, sensory form typicality was able to predict more variance in performance than sensory experience or iconicity ratings. Further, the effects of typicality were consistently inhibitory in comprehension (i.e., more typical forms were responded to more slowly and less accurately), whereas for production the effect was facilitatory. These findings are the first evidence that systematic form-meaning mappings in English sensory words influence their processing. We discuss how language processing models incorporating Bayesian prediction mechanisms might be able to account for form typicality in the lexicon. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

在各种口语中,有些词的声音特征通过唤起知觉意象而与其所指的意义相似,即它们具有标志性(例如,在英语中,"splash "模仿物体撞击水面的声音)。虽然这些声音符号的形式-意义关系已经得到了充分的研究,但对于英语单词的感官属性是否也涉及系统性(即统计性)的形式-意义映射,研究相对较少。我们首先检验了表面形式属性可以预测 5000 多个单音节和双音节单词的感官体验评分这一预测(Juhasz & Yap, 2013),证实它们可以解释很大一部分变异。接下来,我们证明了标志性和感官形式典型性(一种衡量单词形式与其感官体验评分一致性的统计量度)之间的关系很弱,这表明它们很可能是不同的建构。为了确定形式典型性是否会影响对感官词汇的处理,我们使用行为大研究数据集中的词汇决策、词汇识别、命名和语义决策任务进行了回归分析。在所有数据集中,感官形式典型性比感官经验或图标性评级更能预测成绩差异。此外,典型性对理解的影响始终是抑制性的(即对更典型的形式反应更慢、更不准确),而对生产的影响则是促进性的。这些发现首次证明了英语感觉词中的系统形式-意义映射会影响其处理过程。我们讨论了包含贝叶斯预测机制的语言处理模型如何能够解释词典中的形式典型性。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Statistical relationships between surface form and sensory meanings of English words influence lexical processing.","authors":"Greig I de Zubicaray, Elaine Kearney, Frank Guenther, Katie L McMahon, Joanne Arciuli","doi":"10.1037/xhp0001209","DOIUrl":"10.1037/xhp0001209","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Across spoken languages, there are some words whose acoustic features resemble the meanings of their referents by evoking perceptual imagery, i.e., they are iconic (e.g., in English, \"splash\" imitates the sound of an object hitting water). While these sound symbolic form-meaning relationships are well-studied, relatively little work has explored whether the sensory properties of English words also involve systematic (i.e., statistical) form-meaning mappings. We first test the prediction that surface form properties can predict sensory experience ratings for over 5,000 monosyllabic and disyllabic words (Juhasz & Yap, 2013), confirming they explain a significant proportion of variance. Next, we show that iconicity and sensory form typicality, a statistical measure of how well a word's form aligns with its sensory experience rating, are only weakly related to each other, indicating they are likely to be distinct constructs. To determine whether form typicality influences processing of sensory words, we conducted regression analyses using lexical decision, word recognition, naming and semantic decision tasks from behavioral megastudy data sets. Across the data sets, sensory form typicality was able to predict more variance in performance than sensory experience or iconicity ratings. Further, the effects of typicality were consistently inhibitory in comprehension (i.e., more typical forms were responded to more slowly and less accurately), whereas for production the effect was facilitatory. These findings are the first evidence that systematic form-meaning mappings in English sensory words influence their processing. We discuss how language processing models incorporating Bayesian prediction mechanisms might be able to account for form typicality in the lexicon. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":50195,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Human Perception and Performance","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.1,"publicationDate":"2024-07-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140873132","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Attentional set and explicit expectations of perceptual load determine flanker interference. 注意集和对知觉负荷的明确预期决定了侧翼干扰。
IF 2.1 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-07-01 Epub Date: 2024-05-09 DOI: 10.1037/xhp0001217
Joshua O Eayrs, Nanne Kukkonen, Nicoleta Prutean, S Tabitha Steendam, C Nico Boehler, Jan R Wiersema, Ruth M Krebs, Wim Notebaert

Task-irrelevant stimuli often capture our attention despite our best efforts to ignore them. It has been noted that tasks involving perceptually complex displays can lead to reduced interference from distractors. The mechanism behind this effect is debated, with some accounts emphasizing the "perceptual load" of the stimuli themselves and others emphasizing the role of proactive control. Here, in three experiments, we investigated the roles of perceptual load, proactive control, and reward motivation in determining distractor interference. Participants performed a visual search task of high, low, or intermediate load, with flanking task-irrelevant distractors. Each trial was preceded by a cue indicating the level of perceptual load (Experiments 1-3) as well as the potential reward that could be earned (Experiments 2 and 3). In all three experiments, the attentional set induced by the preceding trial and cued proactive expectation of perceptual load interacted to determine flanker interference, which was significant for all trial types except trials cued as high load which were also preceded by high load. These effects were not modulated by reward motivation, although in the final experiment reward did significantly improve performance overall. Thus, successful distractor exclusion does not depend upon motivation or load per se but does require an expectation of high load. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

与任务无关的刺激物往往会吸引我们的注意力,尽管我们会尽力忽略它们。有人指出,涉及知觉复杂显示的任务会导致分心物的干扰减少。关于这种效应背后的机制存在争议,一些观点强调刺激物本身的 "知觉负荷",而另一些观点则强调主动控制的作用。在这里,我们通过三项实验研究了知觉负荷、主动控制和奖励动机在决定分心干扰中的作用。受试者进行了一项高、低或中等负荷的视觉搜索任务,任务两侧是与任务无关的干扰物。每个试验之前都有一个提示,表明知觉负荷的水平(实验 1-3)以及可能获得的奖励(实验 2 和 3)。在所有三个实验中,前一个试验诱发的注意集和对知觉负荷的主动预期提示相互作用,决定了侧翼干扰,这对所有试验类型都是显著的,但提示为高负荷的试验除外,因为这些试验之前也有高负荷。这些效应不受奖励动机的调节,尽管在最后的实验中,奖励确实显著提高了总体成绩。因此,成功排除分心物本身并不取决于动机或负荷,但确实需要对高负荷的预期。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Attentional set and explicit expectations of perceptual load determine flanker interference.","authors":"Joshua O Eayrs, Nanne Kukkonen, Nicoleta Prutean, S Tabitha Steendam, C Nico Boehler, Jan R Wiersema, Ruth M Krebs, Wim Notebaert","doi":"10.1037/xhp0001217","DOIUrl":"10.1037/xhp0001217","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Task-irrelevant stimuli often capture our attention despite our best efforts to ignore them. It has been noted that tasks involving perceptually complex displays can lead to reduced interference from distractors. The mechanism behind this effect is debated, with some accounts emphasizing the \"perceptual load\" of the stimuli themselves and others emphasizing the role of proactive control. Here, in three experiments, we investigated the roles of perceptual load, proactive control, and reward motivation in determining distractor interference. Participants performed a visual search task of high, low, or intermediate load, with flanking task-irrelevant distractors. Each trial was preceded by a cue indicating the level of perceptual load (Experiments 1-3) as well as the potential reward that could be earned (Experiments 2 and 3). In all three experiments, the attentional set induced by the preceding trial and cued proactive expectation of perceptual load interacted to determine flanker interference, which was significant for all trial types except trials cued as high load which were also preceded by high load. These effects were not modulated by reward motivation, although in the final experiment reward did significantly improve performance overall. Thus, successful distractor exclusion does not depend upon motivation or load per se but does require an expectation of high load. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":50195,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Human Perception and Performance","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.1,"publicationDate":"2024-07-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140900090","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
State anticipation and task serialization attenuate embodied decision biases when deciding while moving. 在移动过程中做出决定时,状态预测和任务序列化可减轻体现的决策偏差。
IF 2.1 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-07-01 Epub Date: 2024-05-02 DOI: 10.1037/xhp0001208
Philipp Raßbach, Eric Grießbach, Rouwen Cañal-Bruland, Oliver Herbort

We examined whether and how embodied decision biases-related to motor costs (MC) as well as cognitive crosstalk (CC) due to the body state-are influenced by extended deliberation time. Participants performed a tracking task while concurrently making reward-based decisions, with rewards being presented with varying preview time. In Experiment 1 (N = 58), we observed a reduced CC bias with extended preview time. Partially, this was due to participants slightly adapting tracking to serialize it in relation to decision making. However, the influence of MC was only marginal and not subject to anticipatory state adjustments. In Experiment 2 (N = 67), we examined whether participants integrated the immediate state at reward presentation or anticipated state when a decision could be implemented when adapting their tracking and decision behavior. Results were most compatible with the anticipated state being integrated. We conclude that humans anticipate the body state when a decision must be implemented and consider the corresponding motor and cognitive demands when adapting their decision behavior. However, anticipatory state adaptations targeting the influence of MC with extended preview time were absent, suggesting that anticipatory adaptations are starkly limited in low-practice tasks compared to more overlearned behavior like walking. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

我们研究了与运动成本(MC)以及身体状态导致的认知串扰(CC)相关的体现决策偏差是否会受到延长的斟酌时间的影响,以及如何受到影响。参与者在进行追踪任务的同时做出基于奖励的决策,奖励的预览时间各不相同。在实验 1(N = 58)中,我们观察到随着预览时间的延长,CC 偏差减小。部分原因是参与者稍微调整了追踪,使其与决策相关的序列化。然而,MC 的影响微乎其微,而且不受预期状态调整的影响。在实验 2(N = 67)中,我们考察了参与者在调整追踪和决策行为时,是将奖励呈现时的即时状态与决策实施时的预期状态相结合。结果与预期状态的整合最为吻合。我们的结论是,人类会预测必须执行决策时的身体状态,并在调整决策行为时考虑相应的运动和认知需求。然而,在预览时间较长的情况下,针对MC影响的预期状态适应并不存在,这表明与行走等过度学习的行为相比,预期适应在低练习任务中受到了明显的限制。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"State anticipation and task serialization attenuate embodied decision biases when deciding while moving.","authors":"Philipp Raßbach, Eric Grießbach, Rouwen Cañal-Bruland, Oliver Herbort","doi":"10.1037/xhp0001208","DOIUrl":"10.1037/xhp0001208","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>We examined whether and how embodied decision biases-related to motor costs (MC) as well as cognitive crosstalk (CC) due to the body state-are influenced by extended deliberation time. Participants performed a tracking task while concurrently making reward-based decisions, with rewards being presented with varying preview time. In Experiment 1 (<i>N</i> = 58), we observed a reduced CC bias with extended preview time. Partially, this was due to participants slightly adapting tracking to serialize it in relation to decision making. However, the influence of MC was only marginal and not subject to anticipatory state adjustments. In Experiment 2 (<i>N</i> = 67), we examined whether participants integrated the immediate state at reward presentation or anticipated state when a decision could be implemented when adapting their tracking and decision behavior. Results were most compatible with the anticipated state being integrated. We conclude that humans anticipate the body state when a decision must be implemented and consider the corresponding motor and cognitive demands when adapting their decision behavior. However, anticipatory state adaptations targeting the influence of MC with extended preview time were absent, suggesting that anticipatory adaptations are starkly limited in low-practice tasks compared to more overlearned behavior like walking. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":50195,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Human Perception and Performance","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.1,"publicationDate":"2024-07-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140856632","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Visual-spatial abilities are NOT related to the speed of mental rotation. 视觉空间能力与心理旋转速度无关。
IF 2.1 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-07-01 Epub Date: 2024-05-09 DOI: 10.1037/xhp0001212
Mattan S Ben-Shachar, Andrea Berger

Individuals' reaction time (RT) slopes in tasks of mental rotation have been found to be related to other measures of visual-spatial abilities, and thus are often viewed as a psychometric measure of visual-spatial abilities. The common interpretation of individual RT slopes is as a measure of the speed at which the rotation is carried out. However, electroencephalography studies have found that the process of mental rotation continues after response selection has been carried out, casting doubt on the interpretation of RT slopes as measures of the speed of mental rotation. This study made use of electroencephalography techniques to directly capture individual differences in the speed of mental rotation and assess their association with visual-spatial abilities. We found that individual differences in mental rotation speed are not related to individual differences in RT slopes. Moreover, a computation model supports an alternative explanation by which RT slopes reflect individual differences in differential tolerances for stimulus identification within mental rotation tasks. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

研究发现,个体在心理旋转任务中的反应时间(RT)斜率与视觉空间能力的其他测量指标相关,因此通常被视为视觉空间能力的心理测量指标。对个体反应斜率的常见解释是旋转速度的测量。然而,脑电图研究发现,在进行反应选择之后,心理旋转过程仍在继续,这就对将 RT 斜率解释为心理旋转速度的测量方法产生了怀疑。本研究利用脑电图技术直接捕捉心理旋转速度的个体差异,并评估其与视觉空间能力的关联。我们发现,心理旋转速度的个体差异与RT斜率的个体差异无关。此外,计算模型支持另一种解释,即在心智旋转任务中,RT斜率反映了刺激识别容忍度的个体差异。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Visual-spatial abilities are NOT related to the speed of mental rotation.","authors":"Mattan S Ben-Shachar, Andrea Berger","doi":"10.1037/xhp0001212","DOIUrl":"10.1037/xhp0001212","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Individuals' reaction time (RT) slopes in tasks of mental rotation have been found to be related to other measures of visual-spatial abilities, and thus are often viewed as a psychometric measure of visual-spatial abilities. The common interpretation of individual RT slopes is as a measure of the speed at which the rotation is carried out. However, electroencephalography studies have found that the process of mental rotation continues after response selection has been carried out, casting doubt on the interpretation of RT slopes as measures of the speed of mental rotation. This study made use of electroencephalography techniques to directly capture individual differences in the speed of mental rotation and assess their association with visual-spatial abilities. We found that individual differences in mental rotation speed are not related to individual differences in RT slopes. Moreover, a computation model supports an alternative explanation by which RT slopes reflect individual differences in differential tolerances for stimulus identification within mental rotation tasks. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":50195,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Human Perception and Performance","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.1,"publicationDate":"2024-07-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140900174","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Correction to "Is musical expertise associated with self-reported foreign-language ability?" by Schellenberg, Correia, and Lima (2023). 更正 Schellenberg、Correia 和 Lima(2023 年)所作的 "音乐专长与自我报告的外语能力相关吗?
IF 2.1 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-07-01 DOI: 10.1037/xhp0001225

Reports an error in "Is musical expertise associated with self-reported foreign-language ability" by E. Glenn Schellenberg, Ana Isabel Correia and César F. Lima (Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 2023[Jul], Vol 49[7], 1083-1089). In the article, the following funding information was missing from the author note: "This work was funded by the Portuguese Foundation for Science and Technology through a PhD studentship awarded to Ana Isabel Correia (SFRH/BD/148360/2019), a Scientific Employment Stimulus grant awarded to E. Glenn Schellenberg (CEECIND/03266/2018), and a project grant awarded to César F. Lima (PTDC/PSI-GER/28274/2017) and was cofunded by the European Regional Development Fund through the Lisbon Regional Operational Program (LISBOA-01-0145-FEDER-028274) and the Operational Program for Competitiveness and Internationalization (POCI-01-0145-FEDER- 028274)." The online version of this article has been corrected. (The following abstract of the original article appeared in record 2023-76385-001.) Many claims have been made about links between musical expertise and language ability. Rhythm ability, in particular, has been shown to predict phonological, grammatical, and second-language (L2) abilities, whereas music training often predicts reading and speech-perception skills. Here, we asked whether musical expertise-musical ability and/or music training-relates to L2 (English) abilities of Portuguese native speakers. Participants (N = 154) rated their L2 ability on seven 7-point scales, one each for speaking, reading, writing, comprehension, vocabulary, fluency, and accent. They also completed a test of general cognitive ability, an objective test of musical ability with melody and rhythm subtests, and a questionnaire that measured music training and other aspects of musical behaviors. L2 ability correlated positively with education and cognitive ability but not with music training. It also had no association with musical ability or with self-reports of musical behaviors. Moreover, Bayesian analyses provided evidence for the null hypotheses (i.e., no link between L2 and rhythm ability, no link between L2 and years of music lessons). In short, our findings-based on participants' self-reports of L2 ability-raise doubts about proposed associations between musical and second-language abilities, which may be limited to specific populations or measures. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

报告 E. Glenn Schellenberg、Ana Isabel Correia 和 César F. Lima 所著 "Is musical expertise associated with self-reported foreign-language ability"(《实验心理学杂志:人类感知与表现》,2023 年 7 月,第 49 卷[7],1083-1089 页)中的一处错误。该文章的作者注释中缺少以下资助信息:"本研究由葡萄牙科技基金会通过授予安娜-伊莎贝尔-科雷亚(Ana Isabel Correia)的博士生奖学金(SFRH/BD/148360/2019)、授予E.Glenn Schellenberg (CEECIND/03266/2018)和César F. Lima (PTDC/PSI-GER/28274/2017)的项目资助,并由欧洲区域发展基金通过里斯本区域运营计划(LISBOA-01-0145-FEDER-028274)和竞争力与国际化运营计划(POCI-01-0145-FEDER- 028274)共同资助。本文在线版本已作更正。(原文摘要见 2023-76385-001 号记录)。关于音乐专长与语言能力之间的联系,有许多说法。特别是节奏能力,已被证明可以预测语音、语法和第二语言(L2)能力,而音乐训练通常可以预测阅读和语言感知能力。在此,我们探讨了音乐专长--音乐能力和/或音乐训练--是否与以葡萄牙语为母语的人的第二语言(英语)能力有关。受试者(154 人)用 7 分制对自己的第二语言能力进行了评分,其中口语、阅读、写作、理解、词汇、流利度和口音各占一分。他们还完成了一项一般认知能力测试、一项包含旋律和节奏子测试的客观音乐能力测试,以及一份测量音乐训练和音乐行为其他方面的问卷。第二语言能力与教育程度和认知能力呈正相关,但与音乐训练无关。它与音乐能力或音乐行为的自我报告也没有关联。此外,贝叶斯分析为零假设提供了证据(即第二语言能力与节奏能力之间没有联系,第二语言能力与音乐课年数之间没有联系)。简而言之,我们的研究结果是基于参与者对第二语言能力的自我报告,这使人们对音乐能力与第二语言能力之间的关联产生了怀疑,因为这种关联可能仅限于特定的人群或测量方法。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Correction to \"Is musical expertise associated with self-reported foreign-language ability?\" by Schellenberg, Correia, and Lima (2023).","authors":"","doi":"10.1037/xhp0001225","DOIUrl":"10.1037/xhp0001225","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Reports an error in \"Is musical expertise associated with self-reported foreign-language ability\" by E. Glenn Schellenberg, Ana Isabel Correia and César F. Lima (<i>Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance</i>, 2023[Jul], Vol 49[7], 1083-1089). In the article, the following funding information was missing from the author note: \"This work was funded by the Portuguese Foundation for Science and Technology through a PhD studentship awarded to Ana Isabel Correia (SFRH/BD/148360/2019), a Scientific Employment Stimulus grant awarded to E. Glenn Schellenberg (CEECIND/03266/2018), and a project grant awarded to César F. Lima (PTDC/PSI-GER/28274/2017) and was cofunded by the European Regional Development Fund through the Lisbon Regional Operational Program (LISBOA-01-0145-FEDER-028274) and the Operational Program for Competitiveness and Internationalization (POCI-01-0145-FEDER- 028274).\" The online version of this article has been corrected. (The following abstract of the original article appeared in record 2023-76385-001.) Many claims have been made about links between musical expertise and language ability. Rhythm ability, in particular, has been shown to predict phonological, grammatical, and second-language (L2) abilities, whereas music training often predicts reading and speech-perception skills. Here, we asked whether musical expertise-musical ability and/or music training-relates to L2 (English) abilities of Portuguese native speakers. Participants (<i>N</i> = 154) rated their L2 ability on seven 7-point scales, one each for speaking, reading, writing, comprehension, vocabulary, fluency, and accent. They also completed a test of general cognitive ability, an objective test of musical ability with melody and rhythm subtests, and a questionnaire that measured music training and other aspects of musical behaviors. L2 ability correlated positively with education and cognitive ability but not with music training. It also had no association with musical ability or with self-reports of musical behaviors. Moreover, Bayesian analyses provided evidence for the <i>null</i> hypotheses (i.e., no link between L2 and rhythm ability, no link between L2 and years of music lessons). In short, our findings-based on participants' self-reports of L2 ability-raise doubts about proposed associations between musical and second-language abilities, which may be limited to specific populations or measures. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":50195,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Human Perception and Performance","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.1,"publicationDate":"2024-07-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141312152","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Transfer of statistical learning between tasks. 任务间统计学习的迁移
IF 2.1 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-07-01 Epub Date: 2024-05-09 DOI: 10.1037/xhp0001216
Dirk van Moorselaar, Jan Theeuwes

Recent studies have shown that observers can learn to suppress locations in the visual field with a high distractor probability. Here, we investigated whether this learned suppression resulting from a spatial distractor imbalance transfers to a completely different search task that does not contain any distractors. Observers performed the additional singleton task and learned to suppress the location that was likely to contain a color singleton distractor. Within a block, the additional singleton task would randomly switch to a T-among-L task where observers searched in parallel (Experiment 1) or serially (Experiment 2) for a T among Ls. The upcoming search was either unpredictable (Experiment 1/2A) or cued (Experiment 1/2B). The results show that there was transfer of learning from one to the other task as the learned suppression stayed in place after the switch regardless of whether the T-among-L task was performed via parallel or serial search. Moreover, cueing that the task would switch had no effect on performance. The current findings indicate that implicit learned biases are rather inflexible and remain in place even when the task and the required search strategy are dramatically different and even when participants can anticipate that a change in the search required is imminent. This transfer of the suppression to a different task is consistent with the notion that suppression is proactively applied. Because the location is already suppressed proactively, that is, before display onset, regardless which display and task is presented, the suppressed location competes less for attention than all other locations. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

最近的研究表明,观察者可以学会抑制视野中分心概率较高的位置。在这里,我们研究了这种由空间分心物不平衡导致的学习抑制是否会转移到一个完全不同的、不包含任何分心物的搜索任务中。观察者在执行附加单子任务时,会学习抑制可能包含彩色单子分心物的位置。在一个区块内,附加单子任务会随机切换到T-among-L任务,观察者会平行(实验1)或连续(实验2)搜索L中的T。接下来的搜索要么是不可预测的(实验 1/2A),要么是有提示的(实验 1/2B)。结果表明,不管是通过平行搜索还是串行搜索来完成 "T-among-L "任务,在转换任务后,所学到的抑制都会保持不变,因此学习效果可以从一个任务转移到另一个任务。此外,任务切换的提示对成绩没有影响。目前的研究结果表明,内隐的习得性偏差是相当不灵活的,即使任务和所需的搜索策略有很大不同,即使参与者能预料到所需的搜索即将发生变化,内隐的习得性偏差也会保持不变。这种将抑制转移到不同任务中的做法与 "抑制是主动应用的 "这一概念是一致的。因为位置已经被主动抑制,也就是说,在显示开始之前,无论显示和任务是什么,被抑制的位置对注意力的竞争都小于所有其他位置。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Transfer of statistical learning between tasks.","authors":"Dirk van Moorselaar, Jan Theeuwes","doi":"10.1037/xhp0001216","DOIUrl":"10.1037/xhp0001216","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Recent studies have shown that observers can learn to suppress locations in the visual field with a high distractor probability. Here, we investigated whether this learned suppression resulting from a spatial distractor imbalance transfers to a completely different search task that does not contain any distractors. Observers performed the additional singleton task and learned to suppress the location that was likely to contain a color singleton distractor. Within a block, the additional singleton task would randomly switch to a T-among-L task where observers searched in parallel (Experiment 1) or serially (Experiment 2) for a T among Ls. The upcoming search was either unpredictable (Experiment 1/2A) or cued (Experiment 1/2B). The results show that there was transfer of learning from one to the other task as the learned suppression stayed in place after the switch regardless of whether the T-among-L task was performed via parallel or serial search. Moreover, cueing that the task would switch had no effect on performance. The current findings indicate that implicit learned biases are rather inflexible and remain in place even when the task and the required search strategy are dramatically different and even when participants can anticipate that a change in the search required is imminent. This transfer of the suppression to a different task is consistent with the notion that suppression is proactively applied. Because the location is already suppressed proactively, that is, before display onset, regardless which display and task is presented, the suppressed location competes less for attention than all other locations. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":50195,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Human Perception and Performance","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.1,"publicationDate":"2024-07-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140900173","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Is object-based warping solely object-based? 基于对象的扭曲是否只基于对象?
IF 2.1 3区 心理学 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2024-06-01 Epub Date: 2024-04-04 DOI: 10.1037/xhp0001205
Nicholas Baker, Alexa Vushaj, William Friebel, Juhea Yahng

Object-based warping is a visual illusion in which dots appear farther apart from each other when superimposed on an object. Previous research found that the illusion's strength varies with the perceived objecthood of the display. We tested whether objecthood alone determines the strength of the visual illusion or if low-level factors separable from objecthood also play a role. In Experiments 1-2, we varied low-level features to assess their impact on the warping illusion. We found that the warping illusion is equally strong for a variety of shapes but varies with the elements by which shape is defined. Shapes composed of continuous edges produced larger warping effects than shapes defined by disconnected elements. In Experiment 3, we varied a display's objecthood while holding low-level features constant. Displays with matched low-level features produced warping effects of the same size even when the perceived unity of the elements in the display varied. In Experiments 4-6, we tested whether displays with low-level features predicted to be important in spatial warping produced the visual illusion even when the display weakly configured into a single object. Results showed that the presence of low-level features like contour solidity and convexity determined warping effect sizes over and above what could be accounted for by the display's perceived objecthood. Our findings challenge the view that the spatial warping illusion is solely object-based. Other factors like the solidity of contours and contours' position relative to reference dots appear to play separate and important roles in determining warping effect sizes. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

基于物体的翘曲是一种视觉错觉,在这种错觉中,叠加在物体上的点看起来相互之间的距离更远。先前的研究发现,这种错觉的强度随显示对象的感知对象性而变化。我们测试了视觉错觉的强度是否仅由对象性决定,还是与对象性无关的低层次因素也起了作用。在实验 1-2 中,我们改变了低层次特征,以评估它们对翘曲错觉的影响。我们发现,翘曲错觉对于各种形状都同样强烈,但会随形状定义的元素而变化。由连续边缘组成的形状比由不相连元素定义的形状产生的翘曲效应更大。在实验 3 中,我们在保持低级特征不变的情况下,改变了显示屏的对象性。即使显示屏中元素的感知统一性不同,具有匹配低级特征的显示屏也会产生相同大小的翘曲效应。在实验 4-6 中,我们测试了具有低级特征的显示器是否会产生视错觉,这些低级特征被认为在空间扭曲中非常重要,即使显示器被弱化为一个单一的物体。结果表明,轮廓实体和凸度等低级特征的存在决定了翘曲效应的大小,超过了显示的感知对象性所能解释的范围。我们的研究结果对空间翘曲错觉仅仅基于物体的观点提出了质疑。轮廓的稳固性和轮廓相对于参考点的位置等其他因素似乎在决定翘曲效应大小方面发挥着单独而重要的作用。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Is object-based warping solely object-based?","authors":"Nicholas Baker, Alexa Vushaj, William Friebel, Juhea Yahng","doi":"10.1037/xhp0001205","DOIUrl":"10.1037/xhp0001205","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Object-based warping is a visual illusion in which dots appear farther apart from each other when superimposed on an object. Previous research found that the illusion's strength varies with the perceived objecthood of the display. We tested whether objecthood alone determines the strength of the visual illusion or if low-level factors separable from objecthood also play a role. In Experiments 1-2, we varied low-level features to assess their impact on the warping illusion. We found that the warping illusion is equally strong for a variety of shapes but varies with the elements by which shape is defined. Shapes composed of continuous edges produced larger warping effects than shapes defined by disconnected elements. In Experiment 3, we varied a display's objecthood while holding low-level features constant. Displays with matched low-level features produced warping effects of the same size even when the perceived unity of the elements in the display varied. In Experiments 4-6, we tested whether displays with low-level features predicted to be important in spatial warping produced the visual illusion even when the display weakly configured into a single object. Results showed that the presence of low-level features like contour solidity and convexity determined warping effect sizes over and above what could be accounted for by the display's perceived objecthood. Our findings challenge the view that the spatial warping illusion is solely object-based. Other factors like the solidity of contours and contours' position relative to reference dots appear to play separate and important roles in determining warping effect sizes. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":50195,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Human Perception and Performance","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.1,"publicationDate":"2024-06-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140867339","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Between-task transfer of item-specific control is replicable and extends to novel conditions. 特定项目控制在任务间的转移是可复制的,并可扩展到新的条件。
IF 2.1 3区 心理学 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2024-06-01 Epub Date: 2024-04-04 DOI: 10.1037/xhp0001200
Merve Ileri-Tayar, Jackson S Colvett, Julie M Bugg

Learning-guided control refers to adjustments of cognitive control settings based on learned associations between predictive cues and the likelihood of conflict. In three preregistered experiments, we examined transfer of item-specific control settings beyond conditions under which they were learned. In Experiment 1, an item-specific proportion congruence (ISPC) manipulation was applied in a training phase in which target color in a Flanker task was biased (mostly congruent or mostly incongruent). In a subsequent transfer phase, participants performed a color-word Stroop task in which the same target colors were unbiased (50% congruent). The same design was implemented in Experiment 2, but training and transfer tasks were intermixed within blocks. Between-task transfer was evidenced in both experiments, suggesting learned control settings associated with the predictive cues were retrieved when encountering unbiased transfer items. In Experiment 3, we investigated a farther version of between-task transfer by using training (color-word Stroop) and transfer (picture-word Stroop) tasks that did not share the relevant (to-be-named) dimension or response sets. Despite the stronger, between-task boundary, we observed an ISPC effect for the transfer items, but it did not emerge until the second half of the experiment. The results provided converging evidence for the flexibility and automaticity of item-specific control. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

学习引导控制指的是根据所学到的预测线索与冲突可能性之间的关联来调整认知控制设置。在三个预先注册的实验中,我们考察了特定项目控制设置在学习条件之外的转移。在实验 1 中,特定项目比例一致性(ISPC)操纵被应用于训练阶段,在该阶段,侧翼任务中的目标颜色是有偏差的(大部分一致或大部分不一致)。在随后的转移阶段,被试进行颜色词 Stroop 任务,在该任务中,相同的目标颜色是无偏差的(50% 相同)。实验 2 采用了相同的设计,但训练任务和转移任务在区块内混合进行。这两项实验都证明了任务间的转移,这表明在遇到无偏差的转移项目时,与预测线索相关的已学控制设置被检索出来。在实验 3 中,我们使用不共享相关(待命名)维度或反应集的训练任务(颜色词 Stroop)和迁移任务(图片词 Stroop),研究了任务间迁移的更远版本。尽管任务间的边界更强,我们还是观察到了转移项目的 ISPC 效应,但这种效应直到实验的后半段才出现。这些结果为特定项目控制的灵活性和自动性提供了一致的证据。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Between-task transfer of item-specific control is replicable and extends to novel conditions.","authors":"Merve Ileri-Tayar, Jackson S Colvett, Julie M Bugg","doi":"10.1037/xhp0001200","DOIUrl":"10.1037/xhp0001200","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Learning-guided control refers to adjustments of cognitive control settings based on learned associations between predictive cues and the likelihood of conflict. In three preregistered experiments, we examined transfer of item-specific control settings beyond conditions under which they were learned. In Experiment 1, an item-specific proportion congruence (ISPC) manipulation was applied in a training phase in which target color in a Flanker task was biased (mostly congruent or mostly incongruent). In a subsequent transfer phase, participants performed a color-word Stroop task in which the same target colors were unbiased (50% congruent). The same design was implemented in Experiment 2, but training and transfer tasks were intermixed within blocks. Between-task transfer was evidenced in both experiments, suggesting learned control settings associated with the predictive cues were retrieved when encountering unbiased transfer items. In Experiment 3, we investigated a farther version of between-task transfer by using training (color-word Stroop) and transfer (picture-word Stroop) tasks that did not share the relevant (to-be-named) dimension or response sets. Despite the stronger, between-task boundary, we observed an ISPC effect for the transfer items, but it did not emerge until the second half of the experiment. The results provided converging evidence for the flexibility and automaticity of item-specific control. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":50195,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Human Perception and Performance","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.1,"publicationDate":"2024-06-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140873168","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Speeded classification of visual events is sensitive to crossmodal intensity correspondence. 视觉事件的快速分类对跨模态强度对应很敏感。
IF 2.1 3区 心理学 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2024-06-01 Epub Date: 2024-03-28 DOI: 10.1037/xhp0001183
Robert Carl Gunnar Johansson, Paul Kelber, Rolf Ulrich

Crossmodal correspondences refer to systematic associations between stimulus attributes encountered in different sensory modalities. These correspondences can be probed in the speeded classification task where they tend to produce congruency effects. This study aimed to replicate and extend previous work conducted by Marks (1987, Experiment 3, Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, Vol. 13, No. 3, 384-394) which demonstrated a crossmodal correspondence between auditory and visual intensity attributes. Experiment 1 successfully replicates Marks' original finding that performance in a brightness classification task is affected by whether the loudness of a concurrently presented auditory distractor matches the brightness of the visual target. Furthermore, in line with the original study, we found that this effect was absent in a lightness classification task. In Experiment 2, we demonstrate that loudness-brightness correspondence is robust even when the exact stimulus input changes. This finding suggests that there is a context-dependent mapping between loudness and brightness levels, rather than an absolute mapping between any particular intensity levels. Finally, exploratory analysis using the diffusion model for conflict tasks indicated that evidence from the task-irrelevant modality generates a burst of weak, short-lived automatic activation that can bias decision-making in difficult tasks, but not in easy tasks. Our results provide further evidence for the existence of a flexible crossmodal correspondence between brightness and loudness, which might be helpful in determining one's distance to a stimulus source during the early stages of multisensory integration. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

跨模态对应指的是在不同感官模态中遇到的刺激属性之间的系统关联。这些对应关系可以在加速分类任务中进行探究,因为它们往往会产生一致性效应。本研究旨在复制和扩展马克斯(1987 年,实验 3,《实验心理学杂志:人类感知与表现》,第 13 卷,第 3 期,384-394 页)之前的研究成果,该成果证明了听觉和视觉强度属性之间的跨模态对应关系。实验 1 成功地复制了马克斯最初的发现,即亮度分类任务的表现会受到同时出现的听觉干扰物的响度是否与视觉目标亮度相匹配的影响。此外,与最初的研究结果一致,我们发现在亮度分类任务中也不存在这种影响。在实验 2 中,我们证明了响度与亮度的对应关系即使在确切的刺激输入发生变化时也是稳健的。这一发现表明,响度和亮度水平之间存在着一种依赖于情境的映射关系,而不是任何特定强度水平之间的绝对映射关系。最后,利用冲突任务的扩散模型进行的探索性分析表明,来自任务无关模式的证据会产生一阵微弱、短暂的自动激活,在困难任务中会使决策产生偏差,而在简单任务中则不会。我们的研究结果进一步证明了亮度和响度之间存在灵活的跨模态对应关系,这可能有助于在多感官整合的早期阶段确定一个人与刺激源的距离。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Speeded classification of visual events is sensitive to crossmodal intensity correspondence.","authors":"Robert Carl Gunnar Johansson, Paul Kelber, Rolf Ulrich","doi":"10.1037/xhp0001183","DOIUrl":"10.1037/xhp0001183","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Crossmodal correspondences refer to systematic associations between stimulus attributes encountered in different sensory modalities. These correspondences can be probed in the speeded classification task where they tend to produce congruency effects. This study aimed to replicate and extend previous work conducted by Marks (1987, Experiment 3, <i>Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance</i>, Vol. 13, No. 3, 384-394) which demonstrated a crossmodal correspondence between auditory and visual intensity attributes. Experiment 1 successfully replicates Marks' original finding that performance in a brightness classification task is affected by whether the loudness of a concurrently presented auditory distractor matches the brightness of the visual target. Furthermore, in line with the original study, we found that this effect was absent in a lightness classification task. In Experiment 2, we demonstrate that loudness-brightness correspondence is robust even when the exact stimulus input changes. This finding suggests that there is a context-dependent mapping between loudness and brightness levels, rather than an absolute mapping between any particular intensity levels. Finally, exploratory analysis using the diffusion model for conflict tasks indicated that evidence from the task-irrelevant modality generates a burst of weak, short-lived automatic activation that can bias decision-making in difficult tasks, but not in easy tasks. Our results provide further evidence for the existence of a flexible crossmodal correspondence between brightness and loudness, which might be helpful in determining one's distance to a stimulus source during the early stages of multisensory integration. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":50195,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Human Perception and Performance","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.1,"publicationDate":"2024-06-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140307676","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Journal of Experimental Psychology-Human Perception and Performance
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1