首页 > 最新文献

Cognitive Psychology最新文献

英文 中文
Evidence for abstract representations in children but not capuchin monkeys 儿童有抽象表征的证据,但卷尾猴没有
IF 2.6 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2023-02-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.cogpsych.2022.101530
Elisa Felsche , Patience Stevens , Christoph J. Völter , Daphna Buchsbaum , Amanda M. Seed

The use of abstract higher-level knowledge (also called overhypotheses) allows humans to learn quickly from sparse data and make predictions in new situations. Previous research has suggested that humans may be the only species capable of abstract knowledge formation, but this remains controversial. There is also mixed evidence for when this ability emerges over human development. Kemp et al. (2007) proposed a computational model of how overhypotheses could be learned from sparse examples. We provide the first direct test of this model: an ecologically valid paradigm for testing two species, capuchin monkeys (Sapajus spp.) and 4- to 5-year-old human children. We presented participants with sampled evidence from different containers which suggested that all containers held items of uniform type (type condition) or of uniform size (size condition). Subsequently, we presented two new test containers and an example item from each: a small, high-valued item and a large but low-valued item. Participants could then choose from which test container they would like to receive the next sample – the optimal choice was the container that yielded a large item in the size condition or a high-valued item in the type condition. We compared performance to a priori predictions made by models with and without the capacity to learn overhypotheses. Children's choices were consistent with the model predictions and thus suggest an ability for abstract knowledge formation in the preschool years, whereas monkeys performed at chance level.

使用抽象的高级知识(也称为过度假设)可以让人类从稀疏的数据中快速学习,并在新的情况下做出预测。先前的研究表明,人类可能是唯一能够形成抽象知识的物种,但这一点仍然存在争议。关于这种能力何时在人类发展中出现,也有各种各样的证据。Kemp等人(2007)提出了一个计算模型,说明如何从稀疏的例子中学习过度假设。我们提供了该模型的第一个直接测试:一个生态有效的范式,用于测试两个物种,卷尾猴(Sapajus spp.)和4至5岁的人类儿童。我们向参与者提供了来自不同容器的抽样证据,这些证据表明所有容器都装有统一类型(类型条件)或统一尺寸(尺寸条件)的物品。随后,我们展示了两个新的测试容器和每个容器中的一个示例项目:一个小的高价值项目和一个大的低价值项目。然后,参与者可以从哪个测试容器中选择他们想要接收下一个样本——最佳选择是在尺寸条件下产生大项目或在类型条件下产生高价值项目的容器。我们将性能与具有和不具有学习过度假设能力的模型所做的先验预测进行了比较。儿童的选择与模型预测一致,因此表明他们在学龄前有抽象知识形成的能力,而猴子的表现则处于偶然水平。
{"title":"Evidence for abstract representations in children but not capuchin monkeys","authors":"Elisa Felsche ,&nbsp;Patience Stevens ,&nbsp;Christoph J. Völter ,&nbsp;Daphna Buchsbaum ,&nbsp;Amanda M. Seed","doi":"10.1016/j.cogpsych.2022.101530","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.cogpsych.2022.101530","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>The use of abstract higher-level knowledge (also called overhypotheses) allows humans to learn quickly from sparse data and make predictions in new situations. Previous research has suggested that humans may be the only species capable of abstract knowledge formation, but this remains controversial. There is also mixed evidence for when this ability emerges over human development. <span>Kemp et al. (2007)</span> proposed a computational model of how overhypotheses could be learned from sparse examples. We provide the first direct test of this model: an ecologically valid paradigm for testing two species, capuchin monkeys (<em>Sapajus</em> spp.) and 4- to 5-year-old human children. We presented participants with sampled evidence from different containers which suggested that all containers held items of uniform type (type condition) or of uniform size (size condition). Subsequently, we presented two new test containers and an example item from each: a small, high-valued item and a large but low-valued item. Participants could then choose from which test container they would like to receive the next sample – the optimal choice was the container that yielded a large item in the size condition or a high-valued item in the type condition. We compared performance to a priori predictions made by models with and without the capacity to learn overhypotheses. Children's choices were consistent with the model predictions and thus suggest an ability for abstract knowledge formation in the preschool years, whereas monkeys performed at chance level.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":50669,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Psychology","volume":"140 ","pages":"Article 101530"},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2023-02-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9836153","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The perceived dilution of causal strength 感知到的因果强度的稀释
IF 2.6 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2023-02-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.cogpsych.2022.101540
Simon Stephan , Neele Engelmann , Michael R. Waldmann

Dependency theories of causal reasoning, such as causal Bayes net accounts, postulate that the strengths of individual causal links are independent of the causal structure in which they are embedded; they are inferred from dependency information, such as statistical regularities. We propose a psychological account that postulates that reasoners’ concept of causality is richer. It predicts a systematic influence of causal structure knowledge on causal strength intuitions. Our view incorporates the notion held by dispositional theories that causes produce effects in virtue of an underlying causal capacity. Going beyond existing normative dispositional theories, however, we argue that reasoners’ concept of causality involves the idea that continuous causes spread their capacity across their different causal pathways, analogous to fluids running through pipe systems. Such a representation leads to the prediction of a structure-dependent dilution of causal strength: the more links are served by a cause, the weaker individual links are expected to be. A series of experiments corroborate the theory. For continuous causes with continuous effects, but not in causal structures with genuinely binary variables that can only be present or absent, reasoners tend to think that link strength decreases with the number of links served by a cause. The effect reflects a default notion reasoners have about causality, but it is moderated by assumptions about the amount of causal capacity causes are assumed to possess, and by mechanism knowledge about how a cause generates its effect(s). We discuss the theoretical and empirical implications of our findings.

因果推理的依赖理论,如因果贝叶斯网络账户,假设单个因果联系的强度独立于它们所嵌入的因果结构;它们是从依赖信息(如统计规律)中推断出来的。我们提出了一种心理学解释,假设推理者的因果关系概念更丰富。它预测了因果结构知识对因果强度直觉的系统影响。我们的观点包含了倾向理论所持有的概念,即因果关系凭借潜在的因果能力产生效果。然而,超越现有的规范性倾向理论,我们认为推理者的因果关系概念涉及这样一种观点,即连续原因将其能力传播到不同的因果路径上,类似于流经管道系统的流体。这样的表述导致了因果强度的结构依赖性稀释的预测:一个原因服务的联系越多,预计单个联系就越弱。一系列实验证实了这一理论。对于具有连续影响的连续原因,而不是在具有只能存在或不存在的真正二元变量的因果结构中,推理者倾向于认为联系强度随着原因所服务的联系数量而降低。这种影响反映了推理者对因果关系的默认概念,但它受到对原因被假设拥有的因果能力数量的假设以及对原因如何产生其影响的机制知识的调节。我们讨论了研究结果的理论和实证意义。
{"title":"The perceived dilution of causal strength","authors":"Simon Stephan ,&nbsp;Neele Engelmann ,&nbsp;Michael R. Waldmann","doi":"10.1016/j.cogpsych.2022.101540","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.cogpsych.2022.101540","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Dependency theories of causal reasoning, such as causal Bayes net accounts, postulate that the strengths of individual causal links are independent of the causal structure in which they are embedded; they are inferred from dependency information, such as statistical regularities. We propose a psychological account that postulates that reasoners’ concept of causality is richer. It predicts a systematic influence of causal structure knowledge on causal strength intuitions. Our view incorporates the notion held by dispositional theories that causes produce effects in virtue of an underlying causal capacity. Going beyond existing normative dispositional theories, however, we argue that reasoners’ concept of causality involves the idea that continuous causes spread their capacity across their different causal pathways, analogous to fluids running through pipe systems. Such a representation leads to the prediction of a structure-dependent <em>dilution</em> of causal strength: the more links are served by a cause, the weaker individual links are expected to be. A series of experiments corroborate the theory. For continuous causes with continuous effects, but not in causal structures with genuinely binary variables that can only be present or absent, reasoners tend to think that link strength decreases with the number of links served by a cause. The effect reflects a default notion reasoners have about causality, but it is moderated by assumptions about the amount of causal capacity causes are assumed to possess, and by mechanism knowledge about how a cause generates its effect(s). We discuss the theoretical and empirical implications of our findings.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":50669,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Psychology","volume":"140 ","pages":"Article 101540"},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2023-02-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9474902","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
The role of task-relevant and task-irrelevant information in congruency sequence effects: Applying the diffusion model for conflict tasks 任务相关信息和任务无关信息在一致性序列效应中的作用:基于冲突任务扩散模型的研究
IF 2.6 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2023-02-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.cogpsych.2022.101528
Valentin Koob , Ian Mackenzie , Rolf Ulrich , Hartmut Leuthold , Markus Janczyk

In conflict tasks, such as the Simon, Eriksen flanker, or Stroop task, the congruency effect is often reduced after an incongruent compared to a congruent trial: the congruency sequence effect (CSE). It was suggested that the CSE may reflect increased processing of task-relevant information and/or suppression of task-irrelevant information after experiencing an incongruent relative to a congruent trial. In the present study, we contribute to this discussion by applying the Diffusion Model for Conflict tasks (DMC) framework in the context of CSEs to flanker and Simon tasks. We argue that DMC independently models the task-relevant and task-irrelevant information and thus is a first good candidate for disentangling their unique contributions. As a first approach, we fitted DMC conjointly or separately to previously congruent or incongruent trials, using four empirical flanker and two Simon data sets. For the flanker task, we fitted the classical DMC version. For the Simon task, we fitted a generalized DMC version which allows the task-irrelevant information to undershoot when swinging back to zero. After considering the model fits, we present a second approach, where we implemented a cognitive control mechanism to simulate the influence of increased processing of task-relevant information or increased suppression of task-irrelevant information. Both approaches demonstrate that the suppression of task-irrelevant information is essential to create the typical CSE pattern. Increased processing of task-relevant information, however, could rarely describe the CSE accurately.

在冲突任务中,如Simon、Eriksen flanker或Stroop任务,与一致性试验相比,不一致后的一致性效应通常会降低:一致性序列效应(CSE)。有人认为,CSE可能反映了在经历了与一致性试验相关的不一致后,对任务相关信息的处理增加和/或对任务无关信息的抑制。在本研究中,我们通过将CSEs背景下的冲突任务扩散模型(DMC)框架应用于侧卫和西蒙任务,为这一讨论做出了贡献。我们认为DMC独立地对与任务相关和与任务无关的信息进行建模,因此是解开其独特贡献的第一个好候选者。作为第一种方法,我们使用四个经验侧翼和两个Simon数据集,将DMC联合或单独拟合到先前一致或不一致的试验中。对于侧卫任务,我们采用了经典的DMC版本。对于Simon任务,我们拟合了一个广义DMC版本,该版本允许任务无关信息在摆动回零时下冲。在考虑了模型拟合后,我们提出了第二种方法,其中我们实现了一种认知控制机制,以模拟对任务相关信息的处理增加或对任务无关信息的抑制增加的影响。这两种方法都表明,抑制与任务无关的信息对于创建典型的CSE模式至关重要。然而,增加对任务相关信息的处理很少能够准确描述CSE。
{"title":"The role of task-relevant and task-irrelevant information in congruency sequence effects: Applying the diffusion model for conflict tasks","authors":"Valentin Koob ,&nbsp;Ian Mackenzie ,&nbsp;Rolf Ulrich ,&nbsp;Hartmut Leuthold ,&nbsp;Markus Janczyk","doi":"10.1016/j.cogpsych.2022.101528","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.cogpsych.2022.101528","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>In conflict tasks, such as the Simon, Eriksen flanker, or Stroop task, the congruency effect is often reduced after an incongruent compared to a congruent trial: the congruency sequence effect (CSE). It was suggested that the CSE may reflect increased processing of task-relevant information and/or suppression of task-irrelevant information after experiencing an incongruent relative to a congruent trial. In the present study, we contribute to this discussion by applying the Diffusion Model for Conflict tasks (DMC) framework in the context of CSEs to flanker and Simon tasks. We argue that DMC independently models the task-relevant and task-irrelevant information and thus is a first good candidate for disentangling their unique contributions. As a first approach, we fitted DMC conjointly or separately to previously congruent or incongruent trials, using four empirical flanker and two Simon data sets. For the flanker task, we fitted the classical DMC version. For the Simon task, we fitted a generalized DMC version which allows the task-irrelevant information to undershoot when swinging back to zero. After considering the model fits, we present a second approach, where we implemented a cognitive control mechanism to simulate the influence of increased processing of task-relevant information or increased suppression of task-irrelevant information. Both approaches demonstrate that the suppression of task-irrelevant information is essential to create the typical CSE pattern. Increased processing of task-relevant information, however, could rarely describe the CSE accurately.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":50669,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Psychology","volume":"140 ","pages":"Article 101528"},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2023-02-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9482238","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 4
Facial typicality and attractiveness reflect an ideal dimension of face structure 面部的典型性和吸引力反映了面部结构的理想维度
IF 2.6 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2023-02-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.cogpsych.2022.101541
Logan T. Trujillo , Erin M. Anderson

Face perception and recognition are important processes for social interaction and communication among humans, so understanding how faces are mentally represented and processed has major implications. At the same time, faces are just some of the many stimuli that we encounter in our everyday lives. Therefore, more general theories of how we represent objects might also apply to faces. Contemporary research on the mental representation of faces has centered on two competing theoretical frameworks that arose from more general categorization research: prototype-based face representation and exemplar-based face representation. Empirically distinguishing between these frameworks is difficult and neither one has been ruled out. In this paper, we advance this area of research in three ways. First, we introduce two additional frameworks for mental representation of categories, varying abstraction and ideal representation, which have not been applied to face perception and recognition before. Second, we fit formal computational models of all four of these theories to human perceptual judgments of the typicality and attractiveness (a strong correlate of typicality) of 100 young adult Caucasian female faces, with the models expressed within a face space derived from facial similarity judgments via multidimensional scaling. Third, we predict the perceived typicality and attractiveness of the faces using these models and compare the predictive performance of each to the empirical data. We found that of all four models, the ideal representation model provided the best account of perceived typicality and attractiveness for the present set of faces, although all models showed discrepancies from the empirical data. These findings demonstrate the relevance of mental categorization processes for representing faces.

人脸感知和识别是人类社会互动和交流的重要过程,因此了解人脸是如何在心理上表现和处理的具有重要意义。同时,面孔只是我们日常生活中遇到的许多刺激中的一部分。因此,关于我们如何表示物体的更一般的理论也可能适用于人脸。当代对人脸心理表征的研究集中在两个相互竞争的理论框架上,这两个框架是从更一般的分类研究中产生的:基于原型的人脸表征和基于样本的人脸表征。从经验上区分这些框架是很困难的,而且两者都没有被排除。在本文中,我们从三个方面推进了这一研究领域。首先,我们介绍了两个额外的类别心理表征框架,变化抽象和理想表征,这两个框架以前从未应用于人脸感知和识别。其次,我们将所有四种理论的形式计算模型与人类对100张年轻成年高加索女性面孔的典型性和吸引力(典型性的强相关性)的感知判断相匹配,这些模型在人脸空间内表达,这些人脸空间是通过多维标度从人脸相似性判断中得出的。第三,我们使用这些模型预测人脸的典型性和吸引力,并将每个模型的预测性能与经验数据进行比较。我们发现,在所有四个模型中,理想表示模型提供了对当前人脸组的典型性和吸引力的最佳描述,尽管所有模型都显示出与经验数据的差异。这些发现证明了心理分类过程对人脸表示的相关性。
{"title":"Facial typicality and attractiveness reflect an ideal dimension of face structure","authors":"Logan T. Trujillo ,&nbsp;Erin M. Anderson","doi":"10.1016/j.cogpsych.2022.101541","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.cogpsych.2022.101541","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Face perception and recognition are important processes for social interaction and communication among humans, so understanding how faces are mentally represented and processed has major implications. At the same time, faces are just some of the many stimuli that we encounter in our everyday lives. Therefore, more general theories of how we represent objects might also apply to faces. Contemporary research on the mental representation of faces has centered on two competing theoretical frameworks that arose from more general categorization research: prototype-based face representation and exemplar-based face representation. Empirically distinguishing between these frameworks is difficult and neither one has been ruled out. In this paper, we advance this area of research in three ways. First, we introduce two additional frameworks for mental representation of categories, <em>varying abstraction</em> and <em>ideal representation</em>, which have not been applied to face perception and recognition before. Second, we fit formal computational models of all four of these theories to human perceptual judgments of the typicality and attractiveness (a strong correlate of typicality) of 100 young adult Caucasian female faces, with the models expressed within a face space derived from facial similarity judgments via multidimensional scaling. Third, we predict the perceived typicality and attractiveness of the faces using these models and compare the predictive performance of each to the empirical data. We found that of all four models, the ideal representation model provided the best account of perceived typicality and attractiveness for the present set of faces, although all models showed discrepancies from the empirical data. These findings demonstrate the relevance of mental categorization processes for representing faces.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":50669,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Psychology","volume":"140 ","pages":"Article 101541"},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2023-02-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC9899519/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9836171","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Active causal structure learning in continuous time 连续时间的主动因果结构学习
IF 2.6 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2023-02-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.cogpsych.2022.101542
Tianwei Gong , Tobias Gerstenberg , Ralf Mayrhofer , Neil R. Bramley

Research on causal cognition has largely focused on learning and reasoning about contingency data aggregated across discrete observations or experiments. However, this setting represents only the tip of the causal cognition iceberg. A more general problem lurking beneath is that of learning the latent causal structure that connects events and actions as they unfold in continuous time. In this paper, we examine how people actively learn about causal structure in a continuous-time setting, focusing on when and where they intervene and how this shapes their learning. Across two experiments, we find that participants’ accuracy depends on both the informativeness and evidential complexity of the data they generate. Moreover, participants’ intervention choices strike a balance between maximizing expected information and minimizing inferential complexity. People time and target their interventions to create simple yet informative causal dynamics. We discuss how the continuous-time setting challenges existing computational accounts of active causal learning, and argue that metacognitive awareness of one’s inferential limitations plays a critical role for successful learning in the wild.

因果认知的研究主要集中在对离散观察或实验中汇总的偶然性数据的学习和推理上。然而,这种设置只是因果认知的冰山一角。隐藏在下面的一个更普遍的问题是,当事件和行动在连续时间内展开时,学习将其联系起来的潜在因果结构。在这篇论文中,我们研究了人们如何在连续的时间环境中积极学习因果结构,重点是他们何时何地进行干预,以及这如何影响他们的学习。在两个实验中,我们发现参与者的准确性取决于他们生成的数据的信息性和证据复杂性。此外,参与者的干预选择在最大化预期信息和最小化推理复杂性之间取得了平衡。人们安排时间并确定干预措施的目标,以创建简单但信息丰富的因果动态。我们讨论了连续时间设置如何挑战现有的主动因果学习的计算解释,并认为对推理局限性的元认知意识对野外成功学习起着关键作用。
{"title":"Active causal structure learning in continuous time","authors":"Tianwei Gong ,&nbsp;Tobias Gerstenberg ,&nbsp;Ralf Mayrhofer ,&nbsp;Neil R. Bramley","doi":"10.1016/j.cogpsych.2022.101542","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.cogpsych.2022.101542","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Research on causal cognition has largely focused on learning and reasoning about contingency data aggregated across discrete observations or experiments. However, this setting represents only the tip of the causal cognition iceberg. A more general problem lurking beneath is that of learning the latent causal structure that connects events and actions as they unfold in continuous time. In this paper, we examine how people actively learn about causal structure in a continuous-time setting, focusing on when and where they intervene and how this shapes their learning. Across two experiments, we find that participants’ accuracy depends on both the informativeness and evidential complexity of the data they generate. Moreover, participants’ intervention choices strike a balance between maximizing expected information and minimizing inferential complexity. People time and target their interventions to create simple yet informative causal dynamics. We discuss how the continuous-time setting challenges existing computational accounts of active causal learning, and argue that metacognitive awareness of one’s inferential limitations plays a critical role for successful learning in the wild.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":50669,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Psychology","volume":"140 ","pages":"Article 101542"},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2023-02-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9853376","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 7
Demonstrative systems: From linguistic typology to social cognition 示范系统:从语言类型学到社会认知
IF 2.6 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2022-12-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.cogpsych.2022.101519
Paula Rubio-Fernandez

This study explores the connection between language and social cognition by empirically testing different typological analyses of various demonstrative systems. Linguistic typology classifies demonstrative systems as distance-oriented or person-oriented, depending on whether they indicate the location of a referent relative only to the speaker, or to both the speaker and the listener. From the perspective of social cognition, speakers of languages with person-oriented systems must monitor their listener’s spatial location in order to accurately use their demonstratives, while speakers of languages with distance-oriented systems can use demonstratives from their own, egocentric perspective. Resolving an ongoing controversy around the nature of the Spanish demonstrative system, the results of Experiment 1 confirmed that this demonstrative system is person oriented, while the English system is distance oriented. Experiment 2 revealed that not all three-way demonstrative systems are person oriented, with Japanese speakers showing sensitivity to the listener’s spatial location, while Turkish speakers did not show such an effect in their demonstrative choice. In Experiment 3, Catalan-Spanish bilinguals showed sensitivity to listener position in their choice of the Spanish distal form, but not in their choice of the medial form. These results were interpreted as a transfer effect from Catalan, which revealed analogous results to English. Experiment 4 investigated the use of demonstratives to redirect a listener’s attention to the intended referent, which is a universal function of demonstratives that also hinges on social cognition. Japanese and Spanish speakers chose between their proximal and distal demonstratives flexibly, depending on whether the listener was looking closer or further from the referent, whereas Turkish speakers chose their medial form for attention correction. In conclusion, the results of this study support the view that investigating how speakers of different languages jointly use language and social cognition in communication has the potential to unravel the deep connection between these two fundamentally human capacities.

本研究通过实证检验不同的表征系统的类型分析,探讨语言与社会认知之间的联系。语言类型学将指示系统分为以距离为导向的或以人为导向的,这取决于它们是只指示指示物相对于说话者的位置,还是指示指示物相对于说话者和听者的位置。从社会认知的角度来看,以人为导向的语言的说话者必须监控听者的空间位置,才能准确地使用指示语,而以距离为导向的语言的说话者则可以从自己的、自我中心的角度来使用指示语。为了解决围绕西班牙语示范系统性质的持续争议,实验1的结果证实了西班牙语示范系统是以人为导向的,而英语示范系统是以距离为导向的。实验2显示,并非所有的三向指示系统都是以人为导向的,日语使用者对听者的空间位置表现出敏感性,而土耳其语使用者在指示选择上没有表现出这种影响。在实验3中,加泰罗尼亚-西班牙双语者在选择西班牙语远端形式时对听者位置表现出敏感性,而在选择中端形式时则没有。这些结果被解释为加泰罗尼亚语的迁移效应,这揭示了与英语相似的结果。实验4研究了指示语的使用将听者的注意力转移到预期所指上,这是指示语的普遍功能,也取决于社会认知。说日语和西班牙语的人可以根据听者看得离指称物更近还是更远,灵活地选择近端指示语和远端指示语,而说土耳其语的人则选择中间指示语进行注意力纠正。总之,本研究的结果支持这样一种观点,即调查不同语言的使用者如何在交流中共同使用语言和社会认知,有可能揭示这两种基本人类能力之间的深层联系。
{"title":"Demonstrative systems: From linguistic typology to social cognition","authors":"Paula Rubio-Fernandez","doi":"10.1016/j.cogpsych.2022.101519","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.cogpsych.2022.101519","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>This study explores the connection between language and social cognition by empirically testing different typological analyses of various demonstrative systems. Linguistic typology classifies demonstrative systems as <em>distance-oriented</em> or <em>person-oriented</em>, depending on whether they indicate the location of a referent relative only to the speaker, or to both the speaker and the listener. From the perspective of social cognition, speakers of languages with person-oriented systems must monitor their listener’s spatial location in order to accurately use their demonstratives, while speakers of languages with distance-oriented systems can use demonstratives from their own, egocentric perspective. Resolving an ongoing controversy around the nature of the Spanish demonstrative system, the results of Experiment 1 confirmed that this demonstrative system is person oriented, while the English system is distance oriented. Experiment 2 revealed that not all three-way demonstrative systems are person oriented, with Japanese speakers showing sensitivity to the listener’s spatial location, while Turkish speakers did not show such an effect in their demonstrative choice. In Experiment 3, Catalan-Spanish bilinguals showed sensitivity to listener position in their choice of the Spanish distal form, but not in their choice of the medial form. These results were interpreted as a transfer effect from Catalan, which revealed analogous results to English. Experiment 4 investigated the use of demonstratives to redirect a listener’s attention to the intended referent, which is a universal function of demonstratives that also hinges on social cognition. Japanese and Spanish speakers chose between their proximal and distal demonstratives flexibly, depending on whether the listener was looking closer or further from the referent, whereas Turkish speakers chose their medial form for attention correction. In conclusion, the results of this study support the view that investigating how speakers of different languages jointly use language and social cognition in communication has the potential to unravel the deep connection between these two fundamentally human capacities.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":50669,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Psychology","volume":"139 ","pages":"Article 101519"},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2022-12-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S001002852200055X/pdfft?md5=162ec6489bcf03327eb6f37984527fee&pid=1-s2.0-S001002852200055X-main.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10608056","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 2
A language of thought for the mental representation of geometric shapes 几何图形:对几何形状进行心理表征的思维语言
IF 2.6 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2022-12-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.cogpsych.2022.101527
Mathias Sablé-Meyer , Kevin Ellis , Josh Tenenbaum , Stanislas Dehaene

In various cultures and at all spatial scales, humans produce a rich complexity of geometric shapes such as lines, circles or spirals. Here, we propose that humans possess a language of thought for geometric shapes that can produce line drawings as recursive combinations of a minimal set of geometric primitives. We present a programming language, similar to Logo, that combines discrete numbers and continuous integration to form higher-level structures based on repetition, concatenation and embedding, and we show that the simplest programs in this language generate the fundamental geometric shapes observed in human cultures. On the perceptual side, we propose that shape perception in humans involves searching for the shortest program that correctly draws the image (program induction). A consequence of this framework is that the mental difficulty of remembering a shape should depend on its minimum description length (MDL) in the proposed language. In two experiments, we show that encoding and processing of geometric shapes is well predicted by MDL. Furthermore, our hypotheses predict additive laws for the psychological complexity of repeated, concatenated or embedded shapes, which we confirm experimentally.

在不同的文化和所有的空间尺度上,人类创造了丰富复杂的几何形状,如线、圆或螺旋。在这里,我们提出人类拥有一种几何形状的思维语言,可以将最小几何原语集的递归组合生成线条图。我们提出了一种类似于Logo的编程语言,它将离散数字和连续集成结合在一起,形成基于重复、连接和嵌入的高级结构,我们展示了这种语言中最简单的程序产生了在人类文化中观察到的基本几何形状。在感知方面,我们提出人类的形状感知包括寻找正确绘制图像的最短程序(程序归纳)。这个框架的结果是,记忆形状的心理难度应该取决于所提议语言中的最小描述长度(MDL)。在两个实验中,我们证明了MDL可以很好地预测几何形状的编码和处理。此外,我们的假设预测了重复、连接或嵌入形状的心理复杂性的加性规律,我们通过实验证实了这一点。
{"title":"A language of thought for the mental representation of geometric shapes","authors":"Mathias Sablé-Meyer ,&nbsp;Kevin Ellis ,&nbsp;Josh Tenenbaum ,&nbsp;Stanislas Dehaene","doi":"10.1016/j.cogpsych.2022.101527","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.cogpsych.2022.101527","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>In various cultures and at all spatial scales, humans produce a rich complexity of geometric shapes such as lines, circles or spirals. Here, we propose that humans possess a language of thought for geometric shapes that can produce line drawings as recursive combinations of a minimal set of geometric primitives. We present a programming language, similar to Logo, that combines discrete numbers and continuous integration to form higher-level structures based on repetition, concatenation and embedding, and we show that the simplest programs in this language generate the fundamental geometric shapes observed in human cultures. On the perceptual side, we propose that shape perception in humans involves searching for the shortest program that correctly draws the image (program induction). A consequence of this framework is that the mental difficulty of remembering a shape should depend on its minimum description length (MDL) in the proposed language. In two experiments, we show that encoding and processing of geometric shapes is well predicted by MDL. Furthermore, our hypotheses predict additive laws for the psychological complexity of repeated, concatenated or embedded shapes, which we confirm experimentally.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":50669,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Psychology","volume":"139 ","pages":"Article 101527"},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2022-12-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10615298","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 8
Developmental trajectories of delay discounting from childhood to young adulthood: longitudinal associations and test-retest reliability 儿童期到青年期延迟折扣的发展轨迹:纵向关联和重测信度
IF 2.6 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2022-12-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.cogpsych.2022.101518
Samuel D. Klein, Paul F. Collins, Monica Luciana

Delay discounting (DD) indexes an individual’s preference for smaller immediate rewards over larger delayed rewards, and is considered a form of cognitive impulsivity. Cross-sectional studies have demonstrated that DD peaks in adolescence; longitudinal studies are needed to validate this putative developmental trend, and to determine whether DD assesses a temporary state, or reflects a more stable behavioral trait. In this study, 140 individuals aged 9–23 completed a delay discounting (DD) task and cognitive battery at baseline and every-two years thereafter, yielding five assessments over approximately 10 years. Models fit with the inverse effect of age best approximated the longitudinal trajectory of two DD measures, hyperbolic discounting (log[k]) and area under the indifference-point curve (AUC). Discounting of future rewards increased rapidly from childhood to adolescence and appeared to plateau in late adolescence for both models of DD. Participants with greater verbal intelligence and working memory displayed reduced DD across the duration of the study, suggesting a functional interrelationship between these domains and DD from early adolescence to adulthood. Furthermore, AUC demonstrated good to excellent reliability across assessment points that was superior to log(k), with both measures demonstrating acceptable stability once participants reached late adolescence. The developmental trajectories of DD we observed from childhood through young adulthood suggest that DD may index cognitive control more than reward sensitivity, and that despite modest developmental changes with maturation, AUC may be conceptualized as a trait variable related to cognitive control vs impulsivity.

延迟折扣(DD)是指个体对较小的即时奖励比对较大的延迟奖励的偏好,被认为是认知冲动的一种形式。横断面研究表明,DD在青春期达到高峰;需要纵向研究来验证这种假定的发展趋势,并确定DD是评估一种暂时状态,还是反映一种更稳定的行为特征。在这项研究中,140名年龄在9-23岁之间的人在基线时和之后每两年完成一次延迟折扣(DD)任务和认知电池,在大约10年的时间里进行了5次评估。年龄逆效应拟合的模型最接近两个DD测量,双曲折现(log[k])和无差异点曲线下面积(AUC)的纵向轨迹。从童年到青春期,对未来奖励的折扣迅速增加,并在青春期后期趋于平稳。在研究期间,言语智力和工作记忆较高的参与者表现出较低的DD,这表明从青春期早期到成年期,这些领域与DD之间存在功能性相互关系。此外,AUC在评估点上表现出良好到优异的可靠性,优于log(k),一旦参与者进入青春期后期,两种测量方法都表现出可接受的稳定性。我们从童年到青年期观察到的DD的发展轨迹表明,DD可能更多地反映认知控制而不是奖励敏感性,尽管随着成熟的发展发生了适度的变化,但AUC可能被概念化为与认知控制和冲动相关的特征变量。
{"title":"Developmental trajectories of delay discounting from childhood to young adulthood: longitudinal associations and test-retest reliability","authors":"Samuel D. Klein,&nbsp;Paul F. Collins,&nbsp;Monica Luciana","doi":"10.1016/j.cogpsych.2022.101518","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.cogpsych.2022.101518","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Delay discounting (DD) indexes an individual’s preference for smaller immediate rewards over larger delayed rewards, and is considered a form of cognitive impulsivity. Cross-sectional studies have demonstrated that DD peaks in adolescence; longitudinal studies are needed to validate this putative developmental trend, and to determine whether DD assesses a temporary state, or reflects a more stable behavioral trait. In this study, 140 individuals aged 9–23 completed a delay discounting (DD) task and cognitive battery at baseline and every-two years thereafter, yielding five assessments over approximately 10 years. Models fit with the inverse effect of age best approximated the longitudinal trajectory of two DD measures, hyperbolic discounting (log[<em>k</em>]) and area under the indifference-point curve (AUC). Discounting of future rewards increased rapidly from childhood to adolescence and appeared to plateau in late adolescence for both models of DD. Participants with greater verbal intelligence and working memory displayed reduced DD across the duration of the study, suggesting a functional interrelationship between these domains and DD from early adolescence to adulthood. Furthermore, AUC demonstrated good to excellent reliability across assessment points that was superior to log(<em>k</em>), with both measures demonstrating acceptable stability once participants reached late adolescence. The developmental trajectories of DD we observed from childhood through young adulthood suggest that DD may index cognitive control more than reward sensitivity, and that despite modest developmental changes with maturation, AUC may be conceptualized as a trait variable related to cognitive control vs impulsivity.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":50669,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Psychology","volume":"139 ","pages":"Article 101518"},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2022-12-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10744581","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 2
How “is” shapes “ought” for folk-biological concepts 形状在民间生物学概念中的“应该”是怎样的
IF 2.6 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2022-12-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.cogpsych.2022.101507
Emily Foster-Hanson, Tania Lombrozo

Knowing which features are frequent among a biological kind (e.g., that most zebras have stripes) shapes people’s representations of what category members are like (e.g., that typical zebras have stripes) and normative judgments about what they ought to be like (e.g., that zebras should have stripes). In the current work, we ask if people’s inclination to explain why features are frequent is a key mechanism through which what “is” shapes beliefs about what “ought” to be. Across four studies (N = 591), we find that frequent features are often explained by appeal to feature function (e.g., that stripes are for camouflage), that functional explanations in turn shape judgments of typicality, and that functional explanations and typicality both predict normative judgments that category members ought to have functional features. We also identify the causal assumptions that license inferences from feature frequency and function, as well as the nature of the normative inferences that are drawn: by specifying an instrumental goal (e.g., camouflage), functional explanations establish a basis for normative evaluation. These findings shed light on how and why our representations of how the natural world is shape our judgments of how it ought to be.

知道哪些特征在生物种类中是常见的(例如,大多数斑马都有条纹),可以塑造人们对类别成员是什么样的表征(例如,典型的斑马有条纹),以及对他们应该是什么样的规范性判断(例如,斑马应该有条纹)。在目前的工作中,我们提出了一个问题,即人们倾向于解释为什么特征频繁出现,这是否是一个关键机制,通过这个机制,“现状”决定了人们对“应该”是什么的信念。在四项研究中(N = 591),我们发现频繁的特征通常是通过对特征功能的吸引力来解释的(例如,条纹是用来伪装的),功能解释反过来塑造了典型性的判断,功能解释和典型性都预测了规范性判断,即类别成员应该具有功能特征。我们还确定了从特征频率和功能推断的因果假设,以及所绘制的规范性推断的性质:通过指定工具目标(例如,伪装),功能解释为规范性评估奠定了基础。这些发现揭示了我们对自然世界的描述如何以及为什么会影响我们对自然世界应该如何的判断。
{"title":"How “is” shapes “ought” for folk-biological concepts","authors":"Emily Foster-Hanson,&nbsp;Tania Lombrozo","doi":"10.1016/j.cogpsych.2022.101507","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.cogpsych.2022.101507","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Knowing which features are frequent among a biological kind (e.g., that most zebras have stripes) shapes people’s representations of what category members are like (e.g., that typical zebras have stripes) and normative judgments about what they ought to be like (e.g., that zebras should have stripes). In the current work, we ask if people’s inclination to explain <em>why</em> features are frequent is a key mechanism through which what “is” shapes beliefs about what “ought” to be. Across four studies (<em>N</em> = 591), we find that frequent features are often explained by appeal to feature function (e.g., that stripes are for camouflage), that functional explanations in turn shape judgments of typicality, and that functional explanations and typicality both predict normative judgments that category members ought to have functional features. We also identify the causal assumptions that license inferences from feature frequency and function, as well as the nature of the normative inferences that are drawn: by specifying an instrumental goal (e.g., camouflage), functional explanations establish a basis for normative evaluation. These findings shed light on how and why our representations of how the natural world <em>is</em> shape our judgments of how it <em>ought</em> to be.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":50669,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Psychology","volume":"139 ","pages":"Article 101507"},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2022-12-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10608023","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Integrated diffusion models for distance effects in number memory 数字记忆中距离效应的综合扩散模型
IF 2.6 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2022-11-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.cogpsych.2022.101516
Roger Ratcliff

I evaluated three models for the representation of numbers in memory. These were integrated with the diffusion decision model to explain accuracy and response time (RT) data from a recognition memory experiment in which the stimuli were two-digit numbers. The integrated models accounted for distance/confusability effects: when a test number was numerically close to a studied number, accuracy was lower and RTs were longer than when a test number was numerically far from a studied number. For two of the models, the representations of numbers are distributed over number (with Gaussian or exponential distributions) and the overlap between the distributions of a studied number and a test number provides the evidence (drift rate) on which a decision is made. For the third, the exponential gradient model, drift rate is an exponential function of the numerical distance between studied and test numbers. The exponential gradient model fit the data slightly better than the two overlap models. Monte Carlo simulations showed that the variability in the important parameter estimates from fitting data collected over 30–40 min is smaller than the variability among individuals, allowing differences among individuals to be studied. A second experiment compared number memory and number discrimination tasks and results showed different distance effects. Number memory had an exponential-like distance-effect and number discrimination had a linear function which shows radically different representations drive the two tasks.

我评估了数字在记忆中的三种表现模式。这些结果与扩散决策模型相结合,用于解释两位数刺激的识别记忆实验的准确性和反应时间(RT)数据。综合模型考虑了距离/混淆效应:当测试数在数值上接近研究数时,与测试数在数值上远离研究数时相比,准确性更低,RTs更长。对于其中两个模型,数字的表示分布在数字上(使用高斯分布或指数分布),研究数字和测试数字分布之间的重叠提供了做出决策的证据(漂移率)。对于指数梯度模型,漂移率是研究值与试验值之间数值距离的指数函数。指数梯度模型的拟合效果略好于两种重叠模型。蒙特卡罗模拟表明,30-40分钟内收集的拟合数据的重要参数估计值的变异性小于个体之间的变异性,从而可以研究个体之间的差异。第二个实验比较了数字记忆和数字辨别任务,结果显示了不同的距离效应。数字记忆具有指数型距离效应,而数字识别具有线性函数,这表明驱动这两个任务的表征完全不同。
{"title":"Integrated diffusion models for distance effects in number memory","authors":"Roger Ratcliff","doi":"10.1016/j.cogpsych.2022.101516","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.cogpsych.2022.101516","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>I evaluated three models for the representation of numbers in memory. These were integrated with the diffusion decision model to explain accuracy and response time (RT) data from a recognition memory experiment in which the stimuli were two-digit numbers. The integrated models accounted for distance/confusability effects: when a test number was numerically close to a studied number, accuracy was lower and RTs were longer than when a test number was numerically far from a studied number. For two of the models, the representations of numbers are distributed over number (with Gaussian or exponential distributions) and the overlap between the distributions of a studied number and a test number provides the evidence (drift rate) on which a decision is made. For the third, the exponential gradient model, drift rate is an exponential function of the numerical distance between studied and test numbers. The exponential gradient model fit the data slightly better than the two overlap models. Monte Carlo simulations showed that the variability in the important parameter estimates from fitting data collected over 30–40 min is smaller than the variability among individuals, allowing differences among individuals to be studied. A second experiment compared number memory and number discrimination tasks and results showed different distance effects. Number memory had an exponential-like distance-effect and number discrimination had a linear function which shows radically different representations drive the two tasks.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":50669,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Psychology","volume":"138 ","pages":"Article 101516"},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2022-11-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10374944","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Cognitive Psychology
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1