首页 > 最新文献

Cognitive Psychology最新文献

英文 中文
An instance-based model account of the benefits of varied practice in visuomotor skill 一个基于实例的模型,说明各种视觉运动技能练习的好处
IF 2.6 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2022-09-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.cogpsych.2022.101491
Thomas E. Gorman, Robert L. Goldstone

Exposing learners to variability during training has been demonstrated to improve performance in subsequent transfer testing. Such variability benefits are often accounted for by assuming that learners are developing some general task schema or structure. However much of this research has neglected to account for differences in similarity between varied and constant training conditions. In a between-groups manipulation, we trained participants on a simple projectile launching task, with either varied or constant conditions. We replicate previous findings showing a transfer advantage of varied over constant training. Furthermore, we show that a standard similarity model is insufficient to account for the benefits of variation, but, if the model is adjusted to assume that varied learners are tuned towards a broader generalization gradient, then a similarity-based model is sufficient to explain the observed benefits of variation. Our results therefore suggest that some variability benefits can be accommodated within instance-based models without positing the learning of some schemata or structure.

在训练过程中让学习者暴露在变化中已被证明可以提高随后的迁移测试中的表现。这种可变性的好处通常是通过假设学习者正在开发一些通用的任务模式或结构来解释的。然而,许多研究忽略了考虑不同和恒定训练条件之间的相似性差异。在组间操作中,我们训练参与者在不同或恒定条件下进行简单的抛射任务。我们重复了先前的研究结果,显示了不同的迁移优势,而不是持续的训练。此外,我们表明,标准的相似性模型不足以解释变化的好处,但是,如果模型被调整到假设不同的学习者被调整到更广泛的泛化梯度,那么基于相似性的模型就足以解释观察到的变化的好处。因此,我们的结果表明,一些可变性的好处可以容纳在基于实例的模型中,而无需假定学习一些模式或结构。
{"title":"An instance-based model account of the benefits of varied practice in visuomotor skill","authors":"Thomas E. Gorman,&nbsp;Robert L. Goldstone","doi":"10.1016/j.cogpsych.2022.101491","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.cogpsych.2022.101491","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Exposing learners to variability during training has been demonstrated to improve performance in subsequent transfer testing. Such variability benefits are often accounted for by assuming that learners are developing some general task schema or structure. However much of this research has neglected to account for differences in similarity between varied and constant training conditions. In a between-groups manipulation, we trained participants on a simple projectile launching task, with either varied or constant conditions. We replicate previous findings showing a transfer advantage of varied over constant training. Furthermore, we show that a standard similarity model is insufficient to account for the benefits of variation, but, if the model is adjusted to assume that varied learners are tuned towards a broader generalization gradient, then a similarity-based model is sufficient to explain the observed benefits of variation. Our results therefore suggest that some variability benefits can be accommodated within instance-based models without positing the learning of some schemata or structure.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":50669,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Psychology","volume":"137 ","pages":"Article 101491"},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2022-09-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"40556173","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Visual foundations of Euclidean geometry 欧几里得几何的视觉基础
IF 2.6 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2022-08-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.cogpsych.2022.101494
Véronique Izard , Pierre Pica , Elizabeth S. Spelke

Geometry defines entities that can be physically realized in space, and our knowledge of abstract geometry may therefore stem from our representations of the physical world. Here, we focus on Euclidean geometry, the geometry historically regarded as “natural”. We examine whether humans possess representations describing visual forms in the same way as Euclidean geometry – i.e., in terms of their shape and size. One hundred and twelve participants from the U.S. (age 3–34 years), and 25 participants from the Amazon (age 5–67 years) were asked to locate geometric deviants in panels of 6 forms of variable orientation. Participants of all ages and from both cultures detected deviant forms defined in terms of shape or size, while only U.S. adults drew distinctions between mirror images (i.e. forms differing in “sense”). Moreover, irrelevant variations of sense did not disrupt the detection of a shape or size deviant, while irrelevant variations of shape or size did. At all ages and in both cultures, participants thus retained the same properties as Euclidean geometry in their analysis of visual forms, even in the absence of formal instruction in geometry. These findings show that representations of planar visual forms provide core intuitions on which humans’ knowledge in Euclidean geometry could possibly be grounded.

几何定义了可以在空间中物理实现的实体,因此我们对抽象几何的知识可能源于我们对物理世界的表征。在这里,我们关注欧几里得几何,历史上被认为是“自然”的几何。我们研究人类是否拥有与欧几里得几何相同的描述视觉形式的表征,即它们的形状和大小。来自美国(3-34岁)的112名参与者和来自亚马逊(5-67岁)的25名参与者被要求在6种形式的可变方向面板中定位几何偏差。来自两种文化的所有年龄的参与者都发现了根据形状或大小定义的异常形式,而只有美国成年人区分了镜像(即“感觉”上的不同形式)。此外,不相关的感觉变化不会干扰对形状或大小偏差的检测,而不相关的形状或大小变化会。因此,在所有的年龄和文化中,参与者在分析视觉形式时,即使没有正式的几何指导,也保留了与欧几里得几何相同的性质。这些发现表明,平面视觉形式的表征提供了人类欧几里得几何知识可能建立的核心直觉。
{"title":"Visual foundations of Euclidean geometry","authors":"Véronique Izard ,&nbsp;Pierre Pica ,&nbsp;Elizabeth S. Spelke","doi":"10.1016/j.cogpsych.2022.101494","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.cogpsych.2022.101494","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Geometry defines entities that can be physically realized in space, and our knowledge of abstract geometry may therefore stem from our representations of the physical world. Here, we focus on Euclidean geometry, the geometry historically regarded as “natural”. We examine whether humans possess representations describing visual forms in the same way as Euclidean geometry – i.e., in terms of their shape and size. One hundred and twelve participants from the U.S. (age 3–34 years), and 25 participants from the Amazon (age 5–67 years) were asked to locate geometric deviants in panels of 6 forms of variable orientation. Participants of all ages and from both cultures detected deviant forms defined in terms of shape or size, while only U.S. adults drew distinctions between mirror images (i.e. forms differing in “sense”). Moreover, irrelevant variations of sense did not disrupt the detection of a shape or size deviant, while irrelevant variations of shape or size did. At all ages and in both cultures, participants thus retained the same properties as Euclidean geometry in their analysis of visual forms, even in the absence of formal instruction in geometry. These findings show that representations of planar visual forms provide core intuitions on which humans’ knowledge in Euclidean geometry could possibly be grounded.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":50669,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Psychology","volume":"136 ","pages":"Article 101494"},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2022-08-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0010028522000317/pdfft?md5=e64e420dd1700ec86782f915e035e507&pid=1-s2.0-S0010028522000317-main.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"40398938","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 3
What is theory of mind? A psychometric study of theory of mind and intelligence 什么是心智理论?心理和智力理论的心理测量学研究
IF 2.6 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2022-08-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.cogpsych.2022.101495
Ester Navarro

Theory of mind (ToM) is an essential ability for social competence and communication, and it is necessary for understanding behaviors that differ from our own (Premack & Woodruff, 1978). Recent research suggests that tasks designed to measure ToM do not adequately capture a single ToM ability (Warnell and Redcay, 2019, Quesque and Rossetti, 2020) and, instead, might be related to tasks of general cognitive ability (Coyle, Elpers, Gonzalez, Freeman, & Baggio, 2018). This hinders the interpretation of experimental findings and puts into question the validity of the ToM construct. The current study is the first psychometric assessment of the structure of ToM to date. Comparing ToM to crystallized intelligence (Gc) and fluid intelligence (Gf), the study aims to (a) understand whether ToM should be considered a monolithic ability and (b) explore whether tasks of ToM adequately assess ToM, above and beyond general cognitive ability. For this, confirmatory factor analyses (CFAs), exploratory factor analysis (EFA), and exploratory network analysis (NMA) were conducted. The results of the models largely point to the same conclusion: while ToM tasks are not merely assessing cognitive ability, they are not purely assessing a single ToM construct either. Importantly, these findings align with recent theoretical accounts proposing that ToM should not be considered a monolithic construct (Quesque and Rossetti, 2020, Schaafsma et al., 2015, Devaine et al., 2014), and should instead be explored and measured as multiple domains.

心理理论(ToM)是社会能力和沟通的基本能力,对于理解与我们自己不同的行为是必要的(Premack &伍德乐夫,1978)。最近的研究表明,旨在衡量ToM的任务并不能充分捕捉到单一的ToM能力(Warnell and redkay, 2019; Quesque and Rossetti, 2020),相反,它可能与一般认知能力的任务有关(Coyle, Elpers, Gonzalez, Freeman, &巴乔,2018)。这阻碍了对实验结果的解释,并对ToM结构的有效性提出了质疑。目前的研究是迄今为止首次对ToM结构进行心理测量评估。将ToM与结晶智力(Gc)和流体智力(Gf)进行比较,本研究旨在(a)了解ToM是否应该被视为一种单一的能力,(b)探索ToM的任务是否能够在一般认知能力之外充分评估ToM。为此,我们进行了验证性因子分析(CFAs)、探索性因子分析(EFA)和探索性网络分析(NMA)。这些模型的结果在很大程度上指向了相同的结论:虽然ToM任务不仅仅是评估认知能力,但它们也不是纯粹评估单一的ToM结构。重要的是,这些发现与最近提出的ToM不应被视为单一结构的理论相符(Quesque和Rossetti, 2020, Schaafsma等人,2015,Devaine等人,2014),而应作为多个领域进行探索和测量。
{"title":"What is theory of mind? A psychometric study of theory of mind and intelligence","authors":"Ester Navarro","doi":"10.1016/j.cogpsych.2022.101495","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.cogpsych.2022.101495","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Theory of mind (ToM) is an essential ability for social competence and communication, and it is necessary for understanding behaviors that differ from our own (<span>Premack &amp; Woodruff, 1978</span>). Recent research suggests that tasks designed to measure ToM do not adequately capture a single ToM ability (<span>Warnell and Redcay, 2019</span>, <span>Quesque and Rossetti, 2020</span>) and, instead, might be related to tasks of general cognitive ability (<span>Coyle, Elpers, Gonzalez, Freeman, &amp; Baggio, 2018</span>). This hinders the interpretation of experimental findings and puts into question the validity of the ToM construct. The current study is the first psychometric assessment of the structure of ToM to date. Comparing ToM to crystallized intelligence (Gc) and fluid intelligence (Gf), the study aims to (a) understand whether ToM should be considered a monolithic ability and (b) explore whether tasks of ToM adequately assess ToM, above and beyond general cognitive ability. For this, confirmatory factor analyses (CFAs), exploratory factor analysis (EFA), and exploratory network analysis (NMA) were conducted. The results of the models largely point to the same conclusion: while ToM tasks are not merely assessing cognitive ability, they are not purely assessing a single ToM construct either. Importantly, these findings align with recent theoretical accounts proposing that ToM should not be considered a monolithic construct (<span>Quesque and Rossetti, 2020</span>, <span>Schaafsma et al., 2015</span>, <span>Devaine et al., 2014</span>), and should instead be explored and measured as multiple domains.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":50669,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Psychology","volume":"136 ","pages":"Article 101495"},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2022-08-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0010028522000329/pdfft?md5=836f0d5531c179da5f05b0ebf8d59522&pid=1-s2.0-S0010028522000329-main.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"40398103","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 7
Strategy selection in decisions from givens: Deciding at a glance? 给定决策中的策略选择:一眼决定?
IF 2.6 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2022-08-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.cogpsych.2022.101483
Thorsten Pachur

People deciding between alternatives have at their disposal a toolbox containing both compensatory strategies, which take into account all available attributes of those alternatives, and noncompensatory strategies, which consider only some of the attributes. It is commonly assumed that noncompensatory strategies play only a minor role in decisions from givens, where attribute information is openly presented, because all attributes can be processed automatically “at a glance.” Based on a literature review, however, I establish that previous studies on strategy selection in decisions from givens have yielded highly heterogeneous findings, including evidence of widespread use of noncompensatory strategies. Drawing on insights from visual attention research on subitizing, I argue that this heterogeneity might be due to differences across studies in the number of attributes and in whether the same or different symbols are used to represent high/low attribute values across attributes. I tested the impact of these factors in two experiments with decisions from givens in which both the number of attributes shown for each alternative and the coding of attribute values was manipulated. An analysis of participants’ strategy use with a Bayesian multimethod approach (taking into account both decisions and response-time patterns) showed that a noncompensatory strategy was more frequently selected in conditions with a higher number of attributes; the type of attribute coding scheme did not affect strategy selection. Using a compensatory strategy in the conditions with eight (vs. four) attributes was associated with rather long response times and a high rate of strategy execution errors. The results suggest that decisions from givens can incur cognitive costs that prohibit reliance on automatic compensatory decision making and that can favor the adaptive selection of a noncompensatory strategy.

在选择方案时,人们有一个工具箱,里面既有补偿策略,它考虑了这些方案的所有可用属性,也有非补偿策略,它只考虑了其中的一些属性。通常认为,非补偿策略在给定的决策中只起很小的作用,在给定的决策中,属性信息是公开呈现的,因为所有属性都可以“一眼”自动处理。然而,基于文献回顾,我确定先前关于给定决策中的策略选择的研究已经产生了高度异质性的发现,包括广泛使用非补偿策略的证据。根据视觉注意力细分研究的见解,我认为这种异质性可能是由于不同研究在属性数量上的差异,以及在不同属性中是否使用相同或不同的符号来表示高/低属性值。我在两个实验中测试了这些因素的影响,在两个实验中,每个选项显示的属性数量和属性值的编码都是被操纵的。用贝叶斯多方法分析参与者的策略使用(考虑决策和反应时间模式)表明,在属性数量较多的条件下,非补偿策略被更频繁地选择;属性编码方案的类型对策略选择没有影响。在具有8个(相对于4个)属性的条件下使用补偿策略与相当长的响应时间和较高的策略执行错误率相关。结果表明,给定的决策可能会产生认知成本,从而禁止依赖自动补偿性决策,从而有利于非补偿性策略的适应性选择。
{"title":"Strategy selection in decisions from givens: Deciding at a glance?","authors":"Thorsten Pachur","doi":"10.1016/j.cogpsych.2022.101483","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.cogpsych.2022.101483","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p><span>People deciding between alternatives have at their disposal a toolbox containing both compensatory strategies, which take into account all available attributes of those alternatives, and noncompensatory strategies, which consider only some of the attributes. It is commonly assumed that noncompensatory strategies play only a minor role in </span><em>decisions from givens</em><span>, where attribute information is openly presented, because all attributes can be processed automatically “at a glance.” Based on a literature review, however, I establish that previous studies on strategy selection in decisions from givens have yielded highly heterogeneous findings, including evidence of widespread use of noncompensatory strategies. Drawing on insights from visual attention research on subitizing, I argue that this heterogeneity might be due to differences across studies in the number of attributes and in whether the same or different symbols are used to represent high/low attribute values across attributes. I tested the impact of these factors in two experiments with decisions from givens in which both the number of attributes shown for each alternative and the coding of attribute values was manipulated. An analysis of participants’ strategy use with a Bayesian multimethod approach (taking into account both decisions and response-time patterns) showed that a noncompensatory strategy was more frequently selected in conditions with a higher number of attributes; the type of attribute coding scheme did not affect strategy selection. Using a compensatory strategy in the conditions with eight (vs. four) attributes was associated with rather long response times and a high rate of strategy execution errors. The results suggest that decisions from givens can incur cognitive costs that prohibit reliance on automatic compensatory decision making and that can favor the adaptive selection of a noncompensatory strategy.</span></p></div>","PeriodicalId":50669,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Psychology","volume":"136 ","pages":"Article 101483"},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2022-08-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"44580859","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 3
Motivation improves working memory by two processes: Prioritisation and retrieval thresholds 动机通过两个过程改善工作记忆:优先排序和检索阈值
IF 2.6 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2022-06-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.cogpsych.2022.101472
John P. Grogan , Govind Randhawa , Minho Kim , Sanjay G. Manohar

Motivation can improve performance when the potential rewards outweigh the cost of effort expended. In working memory (WM), people can prioritise rewarded items at the expense of unrewarded items, suggesting a fixed memory capacity. But can capacity itself change with motivation? Across four experiments (N = 30–34) we demonstrate motivational improvements in WM even when all items were rewarded. However, this was not due to better memory precision, but rather better selection of the probed item within memory. Motivational improvements operated independently of encoding, maintenance, or attention shifts between items in memory. Moreover, motivation slowed responses. This contrasted with the benefits of rewarding items unequally, which allowed prioritisation of one item over another. We conclude that motivation can improve memory recall, not via precision or capacity, but via speed-accuracy trade-offs when selecting the item to retrieve.

当潜在的回报超过付出的努力成本时,激励可以提高绩效。在工作记忆(WM)中,人们可以以牺牲非奖励项目为代价优先考虑奖励项目,这表明记忆容量是固定的。但是能力本身能随着动机而改变吗?在四个实验中(N = 30-34),我们证明了即使所有物品都得到奖励,WM的动机也会得到改善。然而,这不是由于更好的内存精度,而是由于更好地选择了内存中的探测项。动机改善独立于编码、维持或记忆项目之间的注意力转移。此外,动机会减缓反应。这与不平等奖励道具的好处形成了对比,后者允许将一个道具优先于另一个。我们得出结论,动机可以提高记忆,不是通过准确性或容量,而是通过选择要检索的项目时的速度-准确性权衡。
{"title":"Motivation improves working memory by two processes: Prioritisation and retrieval thresholds","authors":"John P. Grogan ,&nbsp;Govind Randhawa ,&nbsp;Minho Kim ,&nbsp;Sanjay G. Manohar","doi":"10.1016/j.cogpsych.2022.101472","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.cogpsych.2022.101472","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Motivation can improve performance when the potential rewards outweigh the cost of effort expended. In working memory (WM), people can prioritise rewarded items at the expense of unrewarded items, suggesting a fixed memory capacity. But can capacity itself change with motivation? Across four experiments (N = 30–34) we demonstrate motivational improvements in WM even when <em>all</em> items were rewarded. However, this was not due to better memory precision, but rather better selection of the probed item within memory. Motivational improvements operated independently of encoding, maintenance, or attention shifts between items in memory. Moreover, motivation slowed responses. This contrasted with the benefits of rewarding items unequally, which allowed prioritisation of one item over another. We conclude that motivation can improve memory recall, not via precision or capacity, but via speed-accuracy trade-offs when selecting the item to retrieve.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":50669,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Psychology","volume":"135 ","pages":"Article 101472"},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2022-06-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S001002852200010X/pdfft?md5=c7bff394b0f9a3286f01fdad650d1cb4&pid=1-s2.0-S001002852200010X-main.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"41607918","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 4
The development of reasoning by exclusion in infancy 排斥:在婴儿期通过排斥来发展推理能力
IF 2.6 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2022-06-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.cogpsych.2022.101473
Roman Feiman , Shilpa Mody , Susan Carey

How do humans develop the capacity to reason? In five studies, we examined infants’ emerging ability to make exclusion inferences using negation, as in the disjunctive syllogism (P or Q; not P; therefore Q). Inspired by studies of non-human animals and older children, Experiments 1–3 used an exclusion task adapted from Call’s (2004) 2-cup paradigm and Experiments 4–5 used an exclusion task adapted from the blicket detector paradigm (Sobel & Kirkham, 2006). In both tasks, we found failure to make exclusion inferences at 15 months, fragile success at 17 months, and robust success by 20 months of age. These data converge with some prior evidence that fails to find a capacity to represent negation in infants younger than 15 months of age and conflict with other evidence from different paradigms that suggests infants do have this capacity. We discuss three different resolutions of these conflicting data, and suggest lines of further work that might adjudicate among them.

人类是如何发展推理能力的?在五项研究中,我们检查了婴儿使用否定进行排除推理的新能力,如在析取三段论中(P或Q;不是P;受非人类动物和年龄较大的儿童研究的启发,实验1-3使用了一个改编自Call(2004)的2杯范式的排除任务,实验4-5使用了一个改编自小块探测器范式的排除任务(Sobel &Kirkham, 2006)。在这两项任务中,我们都发现15个月大的孩子无法做出排除推断,17个月大的孩子无法做出脆弱的推断,20个月大的孩子能够做出强大的推断。这些数据与之前的一些证据一致,这些证据没有发现15个月以下的婴儿有表达否定的能力,并且与来自不同范式的其他证据相冲突,这些证据表明婴儿确实有这种能力。我们讨论了这些相互矛盾的数据的三种不同的解决方案,并提出了进一步的工作路线,可能在其中裁决。
{"title":"The development of reasoning by exclusion in infancy","authors":"Roman Feiman ,&nbsp;Shilpa Mody ,&nbsp;Susan Carey","doi":"10.1016/j.cogpsych.2022.101473","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.cogpsych.2022.101473","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>How do humans develop the capacity to reason? In five studies, we examined infants’ emerging ability to make exclusion inferences using negation<em>,</em><span> as in the disjunctive syllogism (</span><em>P or Q; not P; therefore Q</em>). Inspired by studies of non-human animals and older children, Experiments 1–3 used an exclusion task adapted from Call’s (2004) 2-cup paradigm and Experiments 4–5 used an exclusion task adapted from the blicket detector paradigm (Sobel &amp; Kirkham, 2006). In both tasks, we found failure to make exclusion inferences at 15 months, fragile success at 17 months, and robust success by 20 months of age. These data converge with some prior evidence that fails to find a capacity to represent negation in infants younger than 15 months of age and conflict with other evidence from different paradigms that suggests infants do have this capacity. We discuss three different resolutions of these conflicting data, and suggest lines of further work that might adjudicate among them.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":50669,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Psychology","volume":"135 ","pages":"Article 101473"},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2022-06-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9926979","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 11
Distinct but correlated latent factors support the regulation of learned conflict-control and task-switching 不同但相关的潜在因素支持习得性冲突控制和任务转换的调节
IF 2.6 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2022-06-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.cogpsych.2022.101474
Christina Bejjani , Rick H. Hoyle , Tobias Egner

Cognitive control is guided by learning, as people adjust control to meet changing task demands. The two best-studied instances of “control-learning” are the enhancement of attentional task focus in response to increased frequencies of incongruent distracter stimuli, reflected in the list-wide proportion congruent (LWPC) effect, and the enhancement of switch-readiness in response to increased frequencies of task switches, reflected in the list-wide proportion switch (LWPS) effect. However, the latent architecture underpinning these adaptations in cognitive stability and flexibility – specifically, whether there is a single, domain-general, or multiple, domain-specific learners – is currently not known. To reveal the underlying structure of control-learning, we had a large sample of participants (N = 950) perform LWPC and LWPS paradigms, and afterwards assessed their explicit awareness of the task manipulations, as well as general cognitive ability and motivation. Structural equation modeling was used to evaluate several preregistered models representing different plausible hypotheses concerning the latent structure of control-learning. Task performance replicated standard LWPC and LWPS effects. Crucially, the model that best fit the data had correlated domain- and context-specific latent factors. Thus, people’s ability to adapt their on-task focus and between-task switch-readiness to changing levels of demand was mediated by distinct (though correlated) underlying factors. Model fit remained good when accounting for speed-accuracy trade-offs, variance in individual cognitive ability and self-reported motivation, as well as self-reported explicit awareness of manipulations and the order in which different levels of demand were experienced. Implications of these results for the cognitive architecture of dynamic cognitive control are discussed.

认知控制是由学习引导的,因为人们调整控制以满足不断变化的任务需求。“控制学习”的两个最好的研究实例是:对不一致干扰物刺激频率增加的反应,注意任务焦点的增强,反映在全列表比例一致(LWPC)效应中;对任务切换频率增加的反应,切换准备的增强,反映在全列表比例切换(LWPS)效应中。然而,在认知稳定性和灵活性方面支持这些适应的潜在架构——特别是,是否存在单个、一般领域或多个、特定领域的学习者——目前尚不清楚。为了揭示控制学习的内在结构,我们选取了大量的被试(N = 950)执行LWPC和LWPS范式,然后评估了他们对任务操作的外显意识,以及一般认知能力和动机。利用结构方程模型对几种预注册模型进行评估,这些模型代表了控制学习潜在结构的不同似是而非的假设。任务性能复制了标准的LWPC和LWPS效果。至关重要的是,最适合数据的模型具有相关的领域和上下文特定的潜在因素。因此,人们调整任务内焦点和任务间切换准备以适应不断变化的需求水平的能力是由不同的(尽管相关的)潜在因素介导的。当考虑到速度-准确性的权衡,个体认知能力和自我报告动机的差异,以及自我报告的操纵外显意识和不同层次需求的经历顺序时,模型拟合仍然很好。这些结果对动态认知控制的认知结构的意义进行了讨论。
{"title":"Distinct but correlated latent factors support the regulation of learned conflict-control and task-switching","authors":"Christina Bejjani ,&nbsp;Rick H. Hoyle ,&nbsp;Tobias Egner","doi":"10.1016/j.cogpsych.2022.101474","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.cogpsych.2022.101474","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p><em>Cognitive control</em> is guided by learning, as people adjust control to meet changing task demands. The two best-studied instances of “control-learning” are the enhancement of attentional task focus in response to increased frequencies of incongruent distracter stimuli, reflected in the list-wide proportion congruent (LWPC) effect, and the enhancement of switch-readiness in response to increased frequencies of task switches, reflected in the list-wide proportion switch (LWPS) effect. However, the latent architecture underpinning these adaptations in cognitive stability and flexibility – specifically, whether there is a single, domain-general, or multiple, domain-specific learners – is currently not known. To reveal the underlying structure of control-learning, we had a large sample of participants (<em>N</em><span> = 950) perform LWPC and LWPS paradigms, and afterwards assessed their explicit awareness of the task manipulations, as well as general cognitive ability and motivation. Structural equation modeling<span> was used to evaluate several preregistered models representing different plausible hypotheses concerning the latent structure of control-learning. Task performance replicated standard LWPC and LWPS effects. Crucially, the model that best fit the data had correlated domain- and context-specific latent factors. Thus, people’s ability to adapt their on-task focus and between-task switch-readiness to changing levels of demand was mediated by distinct (though correlated) underlying factors. Model fit remained good when accounting for speed-accuracy trade-offs, variance in individual cognitive ability and self-reported motivation, as well as self-reported explicit awareness of manipulations and the order in which different levels of demand were experienced. Implications of these results for the cognitive architecture of dynamic cognitive control are discussed.</span></span></p></div>","PeriodicalId":50669,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Psychology","volume":"135 ","pages":"Article 101474"},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2022-06-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9554451","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Patterns in CAOSS: Distributed representations predict variation in relational interpretations for familiar and novel compound words CAOSS中的模式:分布式表示预测了熟悉和新复合词关系解释的变化
IF 2.6 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2022-05-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.cogpsych.2022.101471
Fritz Günther , Marco Marelli

While distributional semantic models that represent word meanings as high-dimensional vectors induced from large text corpora have been shown to successfully predict human behavior across a wide range of tasks, they have also received criticism from different directions. These include concerns over their interpretability (how can numbers specifying abstract, latent dimensions represent meaning?) and their ability to capture variation in meaning (how can a single vector representation capture multiple different interpretations for the same expression?). Here, we demonstrate that semantic vectors can indeed rise up to these challenges, by training a mapping system (a simple linear regression) that predicts inter-individual variation in relational interpretations for compounds such as wood brush (for example brush FOR wood, or brush MADE OF wood) from (compositional) semantic vectors representing the meanings of these compounds. These predictions consistently beat different random baselines, both for familiar compounds (moon light, Experiment 1) as well as novel compounds (wood brush, Experiment 2), demonstrating that distributional semantic vectors encode variations in qualitative interpretations that can be decoded using techniques as simple as linear regression.

虽然从大型文本语料库中提取的以高维向量表示词义的分布式语义模型已经被证明可以成功地预测人类在各种任务中的行为,但它们也受到了来自不同方向的批评。这些问题包括它们的可解释性(数字如何指定抽象的、潜在的维度来表示意义?)和它们捕获意义变化的能力(单个向量表示如何捕获同一表达的多个不同解释?)在这里,我们证明了语义向量确实可以应对这些挑战,通过训练一个映射系统(一个简单的线性回归),该映射系统可以从代表这些化合物含义的(组成)语义向量中预测化合物(例如木刷)(例如刷子for wood,或刷子MADE OF wood)的关系解释中的个体间变化。这些预测始终优于不同的随机基线,无论是熟悉的化合物(月光,实验1)还是新化合物(木刷,实验2),这表明分布语义向量在定性解释中编码变化,可以使用线性回归等简单技术解码。
{"title":"Patterns in CAOSS: Distributed representations predict variation in relational interpretations for familiar and novel compound words","authors":"Fritz Günther ,&nbsp;Marco Marelli","doi":"10.1016/j.cogpsych.2022.101471","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.cogpsych.2022.101471","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p><span>While distributional semantic models that represent word meanings as high-dimensional vectors induced from large text corpora have been shown to successfully predict human behavior across a wide range of tasks, they have also received criticism from different directions. These include concerns over their interpretability (how can numbers specifying abstract, latent dimensions represent meaning?) and their ability to capture variation in meaning (how can a single vector representation capture multiple different interpretations for the same expression?). Here, we demonstrate that semantic vectors can indeed rise up to these challenges, by training a mapping system (a simple linear regression) that predicts inter-individual variation in relational interpretations for compounds such as </span><em>wood brush</em> (for example <em>brush FOR wood</em>, or <em>brush MADE OF wood</em>) from (compositional) semantic vectors representing the meanings of these compounds. These predictions consistently beat different random baselines, both for familiar compounds (<em>moon light</em>, Experiment 1) as well as novel compounds (<em>wood brush</em>, Experiment 2), demonstrating that distributional semantic vectors encode variations in qualitative interpretations that can be decoded using techniques as simple as linear regression.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":50669,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Psychology","volume":"134 ","pages":"Article 101471"},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2022-05-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"44699475","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 3
Ambivalence in decision making: An eye tracking study 决策中的矛盾心理:一项眼动追踪研究
IF 2.6 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2022-05-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.cogpsych.2022.101464
Agnes Rosner , Irina Basieva , Albert Barque-Duran , Andreas Glöckner , Bettina von Helversen , Andrei Khrennikov , Emmanuel M. Pothos

An intuition of ambivalence in cognition is particularly strong for complex decisions, for which the merits and demerits of different options are roughly equal but hard to compare. We examined information search in an experimental paradigm which tasked participants with an ambivalent question, while monitoring attentional dynamics concerning the information relevant to each option in different Areas of Interest (AOIs). We developed two dynamical models for describing eye tracking curves, for each response separately. The models incorporated a drift mechanism towards the various options, as in standard drift diffusion theory. In addition, they included a mechanism for intrinsic oscillation, which competed with the drift process and undermined eventual stabilization of the dynamics. The two models varied in the range of drift processes postulated. Higher support was observed for the simpler model, which only included drifts from an uncertainty state to either of two certainty states. In addition, model parameters could be weakly related to the eventual decision, complementing our knowledge of the way eye tracking structure relates to decision (notably the gaze cascade effect).

对于复杂的决策,认知中的矛盾心理直觉尤其强烈,因为不同选择的优缺点大致相等,但很难进行比较。我们在一个实验范式中研究了信息搜索,该范式向参与者分配了一个矛盾的问题,同时监测了与不同兴趣领域(AOIs)中每个选项相关的信息的注意力动态。我们为每一种反应分别开发了两种动态模型来描述眼动追踪曲线。这些模型将漂移机制与不同的选择结合起来,就像在标准漂移扩散理论中一样。此外,它们包括一个内在振荡的机制,它与漂移过程竞争,破坏了动力学的最终稳定。这两种模型所假定的漂移过程范围不同。更简单的模型得到了更高的支持,该模型只包括从不确定状态到两个确定状态中的任何一个的漂移。此外,模型参数可能与最终决策的相关性较弱,补充了我们对眼动追踪结构与决策相关方式的了解(特别是凝视级联效应)。
{"title":"Ambivalence in decision making: An eye tracking study","authors":"Agnes Rosner ,&nbsp;Irina Basieva ,&nbsp;Albert Barque-Duran ,&nbsp;Andreas Glöckner ,&nbsp;Bettina von Helversen ,&nbsp;Andrei Khrennikov ,&nbsp;Emmanuel M. Pothos","doi":"10.1016/j.cogpsych.2022.101464","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.cogpsych.2022.101464","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>An intuition of ambivalence in cognition is particularly strong for complex decisions, for which the merits and demerits of different options are roughly equal but hard to compare. We examined information search in an experimental paradigm which tasked participants with an ambivalent question, while monitoring attentional dynamics concerning the information relevant to each option in different Areas of Interest (AOIs). We developed two dynamical models for describing eye tracking curves, for each response separately. The models incorporated a drift mechanism towards the various options, as in standard drift diffusion theory. In addition, they included a mechanism for intrinsic oscillation, which competed with the drift process and undermined eventual stabilization of the dynamics. The two models varied in the range of drift processes postulated. Higher support was observed for the simpler model, which only included drifts from an uncertainty state to either of two certainty states. In addition, model parameters could be weakly related to the eventual decision, complementing our knowledge of the way eye tracking structure relates to decision (notably the gaze cascade effect).</p></div>","PeriodicalId":50669,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Psychology","volume":"134 ","pages":"Article 101464"},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2022-05-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0010028522000020/pdfft?md5=c074a8b48e1bc203cf228ca94cef1ee5&pid=1-s2.0-S0010028522000020-main.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"48762379","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 5
An interference model for visual working memory: Applications to the change detection task 视觉工作记忆的干扰模型:在变化检测任务中的应用
IF 2.6 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2022-03-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.cogpsych.2022.101463
Hsuan-Yu Lin , Klaus Oberauer

Most studies of visual-working memory employ one of two experimental paradigms: change-detection or continuous-stimulus reproduction. In this study, we extended the Interference Model (IM; Oberauer & Lin, 2017), which was designed for continuous reproduction, to the single-probe change-detection task. In continuous reproduction, participants occasionally report the non-target items instead of the target. The presence of non-target response is predicted by the Interference Model, which relies in part on the interference of non-target items to explain the set-size effect. By presenting a probe matching a non-target item, we can investigate the amount of interference from non-target items in change detection. As predicted by the Interference Model, we observed poorer performance in rejecting a probe matching a non-target item compared to a new probe (i.e., a cost due to intrusions from non-targets). We fitted the IM along with the Variable Precision, the Slot-Averaging, and the Neural-Population model to the data from two change-detection experiments. The models were equipped with a Bayesian decision rule based on the one used in Keshvari, van den Berg, and Ma (2013). The Interference Model and the Neural-Population model successfully predicted the set-size effect and the non-target intrusion cost, whereas the Variable Precision (VP) and Slot-Averaging (SA) models failed to predict the intrusion cost at all. Even with additional assumptions enabling VP and SA to produce intrusion costs, the IM still performed better than the competing models quantitatively.

大多数视觉工作记忆的研究采用两种实验范式之一:变化检测或连续刺激再现。在本研究中,我们扩展了干涉模型(IM;Oberauer,Lin, 2017),它是为连续复制而设计的,到单探针变化检测任务。在连续复制过程中,参与者偶尔会报告非目标项目而不是目标项目。干扰模型预测了非目标反应的存在,该模型部分依赖于非目标项目的干扰来解释集合大小效应。通过提出一个与非目标项匹配的探针,我们可以研究非目标项在变化检测中的干扰量。正如干扰模型所预测的那样,我们观察到在拒绝与非目标项目匹配的探针时,与新探针相比,性能较差(即,由于来自非目标的入侵而产生的成本)。我们将IM与可变精度、槽平均和神经种群模型一起拟合到两个变化检测实验的数据中。这些模型配备了一个基于Keshvari, van den Berg, and Ma(2013)中使用的贝叶斯决策规则。干扰模型和神经种群模型成功预测了集大小效应和非目标入侵成本,而变精度(VP)和间隔平均(SA)模型完全无法预测入侵成本。即使有额外的假设使VP和SA产生入侵成本,IM仍然比竞争模型在数量上表现得更好。
{"title":"An interference model for visual working memory: Applications to the change detection task","authors":"Hsuan-Yu Lin ,&nbsp;Klaus Oberauer","doi":"10.1016/j.cogpsych.2022.101463","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.cogpsych.2022.101463","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Most studies of visual-working memory employ one of two experimental paradigms: change-detection or continuous-stimulus reproduction. In this study, we extended the Interference Model (IM; Oberauer &amp; Lin, 2017), which was designed for continuous reproduction, to the single-probe change-detection task. In continuous reproduction, participants occasionally report the non-target items instead of the target. The presence of non-target response is predicted by the Interference Model, which relies in part on the interference of non-target items to explain the set-size effect. By presenting a probe matching a non-target item, we can investigate the amount of interference from non-target items in change detection. As predicted by the Interference Model, we observed poorer performance in rejecting a probe matching a non-target item compared to a new probe (i.e., a cost due to intrusions from non-targets). We fitted the IM along with the Variable Precision, the Slot-Averaging, and the Neural-Population model to the data from two change-detection experiments. The models were equipped with a Bayesian decision rule based on the one used in Keshvari, van den Berg, and Ma (2013). The Interference Model and the Neural-Population model successfully predicted the set-size effect and the non-target intrusion cost, whereas the Variable Precision (VP) and Slot-Averaging (SA) models failed to predict the intrusion cost at all. Even with additional assumptions enabling VP and SA to produce intrusion costs, the IM still performed better than the competing models quantitatively.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":50669,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Psychology","volume":"133 ","pages":"Article 101463"},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2022-03-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"39788805","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 4
期刊
Cognitive Psychology
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1