首页 > 最新文献

Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience最新文献

英文 中文
A Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging Study of Phrase Structure and Subject Island Violations. 词组结构和主题岛违规的功能磁共振成像研究
IF 3.1 3区 医学 Q2 NEUROSCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-11-01 DOI: 10.1162/jocn_a_02266
William Matchin, Diogo Almeida, Gregory Hickok, Jon Sprouse

In principle, functional neuroimaging provides uniquely informative data in addressing linguistic questions, because it can indicate distinct processes that are not apparent from behavioral data alone. This could involve adjudicating the source of unacceptability via the different patterns of elicited brain responses to different ungrammatical sentence types. However, it is difficult to interpret brain activations to syntactic violations. Such responses could reflect processes that have nothing intrinsically related to linguistic representations, such as domain-general executive function abilities. To facilitate the potential use of functional neuroimaging methods to identify the source of different syntactic violations, we conducted a functional magnetic resonance imaging experiment to identify the brain activation maps associated with two distinct syntactic violation types: phrase structure (created by inverting the order of two adjacent words within a sentence) and subject islands (created by extracting a wh-phrase out of an embedded subject). The comparison of these violations to control sentences surprisingly showed no indication of a generalized violation response, with almost completely divergent activation patterns. Phrase structure violations seemingly activated regions previously implicated in verbal working memory and structural complexity in sentence processing, whereas the subject islands appeared to activate regions previously implicated in conceptual-semantic processing, broadly defined. We review our findings in the context of previous research on syntactic and semantic violations using ERPs. Although our results suggest potentially distinct underlying mechanisms underlying phrase structure and subject island violations, our results are tentative and suggest important methodological considerations for future research in this area.

原则上,功能神经影像学在解决语言学问题时能提供独一无二的信息数据,因为它能显示仅从行为数据中无法看出的独特过程。这可能涉及到通过大脑对不同非语法句子类型的不同反应模式来判断不可接受性的来源。然而,很难解释大脑对句法违规行为的激活。这些反应可能反映了与语言表征没有任何内在联系的过程,如一般领域的执行功能能力。为了便于使用功能神经影像学方法识别不同句法违规的来源,我们进行了一项功能磁共振成像实验,以识别与两种不同句法违规类型相关的大脑激活图谱:短语结构(通过颠倒句子中两个相邻单词的顺序而产生)和主语岛(通过从嵌入的主语中提取一个wh短语而产生)。令人惊讶的是,将这些违规句子与对照句子进行比较后发现,没有迹象表明存在普遍的违规反应,激活模式几乎完全不同。短语结构违规似乎激活了以前与句子处理中的言语工作记忆和结构复杂性有关的区域,而主语岛似乎激活了以前与广义概念-语义处理有关的区域。我们结合之前使用 ERPs 对句法和语义违规的研究,回顾了我们的发现。尽管我们的研究结果表明短语结构和主题岛违规行为的潜在机制可能是不同的,但我们的研究结果只是初步的,并为这一领域未来的研究提出了重要的方法论考虑。
{"title":"A Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging Study of Phrase Structure and Subject Island Violations.","authors":"William Matchin, Diogo Almeida, Gregory Hickok, Jon Sprouse","doi":"10.1162/jocn_a_02266","DOIUrl":"10.1162/jocn_a_02266","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>In principle, functional neuroimaging provides uniquely informative data in addressing linguistic questions, because it can indicate distinct processes that are not apparent from behavioral data alone. This could involve adjudicating the source of unacceptability via the different patterns of elicited brain responses to different ungrammatical sentence types. However, it is difficult to interpret brain activations to syntactic violations. Such responses could reflect processes that have nothing intrinsically related to linguistic representations, such as domain-general executive function abilities. To facilitate the potential use of functional neuroimaging methods to identify the source of different syntactic violations, we conducted a functional magnetic resonance imaging experiment to identify the brain activation maps associated with two distinct syntactic violation types: phrase structure (created by inverting the order of two adjacent words within a sentence) and subject islands (created by extracting a wh-phrase out of an embedded subject). The comparison of these violations to control sentences surprisingly showed no indication of a generalized violation response, with almost completely divergent activation patterns. Phrase structure violations seemingly activated regions previously implicated in verbal working memory and structural complexity in sentence processing, whereas the subject islands appeared to activate regions previously implicated in conceptual-semantic processing, broadly defined. We review our findings in the context of previous research on syntactic and semantic violations using ERPs. Although our results suggest potentially distinct underlying mechanisms underlying phrase structure and subject island violations, our results are tentative and suggest important methodological considerations for future research in this area.</p>","PeriodicalId":51081,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience","volume":" ","pages":"1-29"},"PeriodicalIF":3.1,"publicationDate":"2024-11-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142607240","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Movement Strategy Moderates the Effect of Spatially Congruent Cues on the Stability of Rhythmic Bimanual Finger Movements. 运动策略可调节空间一致性线索对有节奏双指运动稳定性的影响
IF 3.1 3区 医学 Q2 NEUROSCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-11-01 DOI: 10.1162/jocn_a_02268
Ronan Denyer, Lara A Boyd

Spatially congruent cues increase the speed of bimanual reach decisions compared with abstract symbolic cues, particularly for asymmetric reaches. Asymmetric rhythmic bimanual movements are less stable than symmetric rhythmic movements, but it is not well understood if spatially congruent cues similarly increase the stability of asymmetric rhythmic bimanual movements. To address this question, in Experiment 1, participants performed symmetric and asymmetric bimanual rhythmic finger tapping movements at different movement frequencies in time with flickering spatially congruent and abstract symbolic stimuli. As expected, symmetric movements were more stable. Spatially congruent cues similarly increased the stability of symmetric and asymmetric movements compared with abstract symbolic cues. The benefits of spatial congruence and movement symmetry were restricted to high movement frequencies (>2 Hz). To better understand if the emergence of these effects at high movement frequencies was driven by a change in movement strategy, in Experiment 2, video of the hands was concurrently recorded during task performance. Markerless motion tracking software revealed that participants switched from discontinuous to continuous movement strategies with increasing movement frequency. Because discontinuous and continuous movements are thought to be controlled by distinct neurocognitive systems, this might explain why the beneficial effects of spatial congruence and response symmetry emerged only at high movement frequencies. Overall, results from the current study indicate that the perceptual quality of the stimulus use to cue movement frequency can have powerful effects on the stability of rhythmic bimanual movements, but that these effects may depend on whether discontinuous or continuous movement strategies are selected.

与抽象的符号线索相比,空间一致的线索能提高双臂伸展决策的速度,尤其是在非对称伸展的情况下。非对称节奏双臂动作的稳定性低于对称节奏动作,但空间一致性线索是否同样能提高非对称节奏双臂动作的稳定性,目前还不十分清楚。为了解决这个问题,在实验 1 中,受试者在闪烁的空间一致和抽象符号刺激下,以不同的运动频率进行对称和不对称的双指节奏性敲击运动。不出所料,对称动作更加稳定。与抽象符号线索相比,空间一致性线索同样提高了对称和不对称动作的稳定性。空间一致性和运动对称性的益处仅限于高运动频率(>2 Hz)。为了更好地了解这些效应在高运动频率下的出现是否是由运动策略的改变所驱动的,在实验 2 中,我们同时记录了任务执行过程中双手的视频。无标记运动跟踪软件显示,随着运动频率的增加,参与者从非连续运动策略转换为连续运动策略。由于不连续运动和连续运动被认为是由不同的神经认知系统控制的,这也许可以解释为什么空间一致性和反应对称性的有益效果只有在高运动频率时才会出现。总之,本研究的结果表明,用于提示运动频率的刺激物的知觉质量会对有节奏的双足运动的稳定性产生强大的影响,但这些影响可能取决于选择的是非连续运动策略还是连续运动策略。
{"title":"Movement Strategy Moderates the Effect of Spatially Congruent Cues on the Stability of Rhythmic Bimanual Finger Movements.","authors":"Ronan Denyer, Lara A Boyd","doi":"10.1162/jocn_a_02268","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn_a_02268","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Spatially congruent cues increase the speed of bimanual reach decisions compared with abstract symbolic cues, particularly for asymmetric reaches. Asymmetric rhythmic bimanual movements are less stable than symmetric rhythmic movements, but it is not well understood if spatially congruent cues similarly increase the stability of asymmetric rhythmic bimanual movements. To address this question, in Experiment 1, participants performed symmetric and asymmetric bimanual rhythmic finger tapping movements at different movement frequencies in time with flickering spatially congruent and abstract symbolic stimuli. As expected, symmetric movements were more stable. Spatially congruent cues similarly increased the stability of symmetric and asymmetric movements compared with abstract symbolic cues. The benefits of spatial congruence and movement symmetry were restricted to high movement frequencies (>2 Hz). To better understand if the emergence of these effects at high movement frequencies was driven by a change in movement strategy, in Experiment 2, video of the hands was concurrently recorded during task performance. Markerless motion tracking software revealed that participants switched from discontinuous to continuous movement strategies with increasing movement frequency. Because discontinuous and continuous movements are thought to be controlled by distinct neurocognitive systems, this might explain why the beneficial effects of spatial congruence and response symmetry emerged only at high movement frequencies. Overall, results from the current study indicate that the perceptual quality of the stimulus use to cue movement frequency can have powerful effects on the stability of rhythmic bimanual movements, but that these effects may depend on whether discontinuous or continuous movement strategies are selected.</p>","PeriodicalId":51081,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience","volume":" ","pages":"1-20"},"PeriodicalIF":3.1,"publicationDate":"2024-11-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142584924","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The Impact of Selective Attention and Musical Training on the Cortical Speech Tracking in the Delta and Theta Frequency Bands. 选择性注意和音乐训练对德尔塔和塞尔塔频段皮层语音跟踪的影响
IF 3.1 3区 医学 Q2 NEUROSCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-11-01 DOI: 10.1162/jocn_a_02275
Alina Schüller, Annika Mücke, Jasmin Riegel, Tobias Reichenbach

Oral communication regularly takes place amidst background noise, requiring the ability to selectively attend to a target speech stream. Musical training has been shown to be beneficial for this task. Regarding the underlying neural mechanisms, recent studies showed that the speech envelope is tracked by neural activity in auditory cortex, which plays a role in the neural processing of speech, including speech in noise. The neural tracking occurs predominantly in two frequency bands, the delta and the theta bands. However, much regarding the specifics of these neural responses, as well as their modulation through musical training, still remain unclear. Here, we investigated the delta- and theta-band cortical tracking of the speech envelope of attended and ignored speech using magnetoencephalography (MEG) recordings. We thereby assessed both musicians and nonmusicians to explore potential differences between these groups. The cortical speech tracking was quantified through source-reconstructing the MEG data and subsequently relating the speech envelope in a certain frequency band to the MEG data using linear models. We thereby found the theta-band tracking to be dominated by early responses with comparable magnitudes for attended and ignored speech, whereas the delta band tracking exhibited both earlier and later responses that were modulated by selective attention. Almost no significant differences emerged in the neural responses between musicians and nonmusicians. Our findings show that only the speech tracking in the delta but not in the theta band contributes to selective attention, but that this mechanism is essentially unaffected by musical training.

口语交流经常是在背景噪音中进行的,这就要求人们能够选择性地注意目标语流。音乐训练已被证明对这项任务有益。关于潜在的神经机制,最近的研究表明,听觉皮层的神经活动会跟踪语音包络,而听觉皮层在语音(包括噪音中的语音)的神经处理过程中扮演着重要角色。神经跟踪主要发生在两个频段,即 delta 和 theta 频段。然而,这些神经反应的具体细节以及通过音乐训练对其进行调节的情况仍不清楚。在此,我们利用脑磁图(MEG)记录研究了大脑皮层对被关注和被忽略的语音包络的δ和θ波段跟踪。因此,我们同时对音乐家和非音乐家进行了评估,以探索这些群体之间的潜在差异。通过对 MEG 数据进行源重构,然后使用线性模型将某一频段的语音包络与 MEG 数据联系起来,对大脑皮层的语音跟踪进行量化。因此,我们发现θ波段跟踪以早期反应为主,被注意和被忽略的语音反应幅度相当,而δ波段跟踪则表现出受选择性注意调节的早期和晚期反应。音乐家和非音乐家之间的神经反应几乎没有明显差异。我们的研究结果表明,只有δ波段而不是θ波段的语音跟踪有助于选择性注意,但这一机制基本上不受音乐训练的影响。
{"title":"The Impact of Selective Attention and Musical Training on the Cortical Speech Tracking in the Delta and Theta Frequency Bands.","authors":"Alina Schüller, Annika Mücke, Jasmin Riegel, Tobias Reichenbach","doi":"10.1162/jocn_a_02275","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn_a_02275","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Oral communication regularly takes place amidst background noise, requiring the ability to selectively attend to a target speech stream. Musical training has been shown to be beneficial for this task. Regarding the underlying neural mechanisms, recent studies showed that the speech envelope is tracked by neural activity in auditory cortex, which plays a role in the neural processing of speech, including speech in noise. The neural tracking occurs predominantly in two frequency bands, the delta and the theta bands. However, much regarding the specifics of these neural responses, as well as their modulation through musical training, still remain unclear. Here, we investigated the delta- and theta-band cortical tracking of the speech envelope of attended and ignored speech using magnetoencephalography (MEG) recordings. We thereby assessed both musicians and nonmusicians to explore potential differences between these groups. The cortical speech tracking was quantified through source-reconstructing the MEG data and subsequently relating the speech envelope in a certain frequency band to the MEG data using linear models. We thereby found the theta-band tracking to be dominated by early responses with comparable magnitudes for attended and ignored speech, whereas the delta band tracking exhibited both earlier and later responses that were modulated by selective attention. Almost no significant differences emerged in the neural responses between musicians and nonmusicians. Our findings show that only the speech tracking in the delta but not in the theta band contributes to selective attention, but that this mechanism is essentially unaffected by musical training.</p>","PeriodicalId":51081,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience","volume":" ","pages":"1-18"},"PeriodicalIF":3.1,"publicationDate":"2024-11-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142607310","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Rapid Learning of Temporal Dependencies at Multiple Timescales 在多个时间尺度上快速学习时间相关性。
IF 3.1 3区 医学 Q2 NEUROSCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-10-29 DOI: 10.1162/jocn_a_02232
Cybelle M. Smith;Sharon L. Thompson-Schill;Anna C. Schapiro
Our environment contains temporal information unfolding simultaneously at multiple timescales. How do we learn and represent these dynamic and overlapping information streams? We investigated these processes in a statistical learning paradigm with simultaneous short and long timescale contingencies. Human participants (n = 96) played a game where they learned to quickly click on a target image when it appeared in one of nine locations, in eight different contexts. Across contexts, we manipulated the order of target locations: at a short timescale, the order of pairs of sequential locations in which the target appeared; at a longer timescale, the set of locations that appeared in the first versus the second half of the game. Participants periodically predicted the upcoming target location, and later performed similarity judgments comparing the games based on their order properties. Participants showed context-dependent sensitivity to order information at both short and long timescales, with evidence of stronger learning for short timescales. We modeled the learning paradigm using a gated recurrent network trained to make immediate predictions, which demonstrated multilevel learning timecourses and patterns of sensitivity to the similarity structure of the games that mirrored human participants. The model grouped games with matching rule structure and dissociated games based on low-level order information more so than high-level order information. The work shows how humans and models can rapidly and concurrently acquire order information at different timescales.
我们的环境包含在多个时间尺度上同时展开的时间信息。我们如何学习和表征这些动态和重叠的信息流?我们在一个统计学习范例中研究了这些过程。人类参与者(n = 96)玩了一个游戏,在游戏中,他们学会了在八个不同情境下,当目标图像出现在九个位置之一时,快速点击该图像。在不同的情境中,我们操纵了目标位置的顺序:在短时间内,操纵了目标出现的成对连续位置的顺序;在较长时间内,操纵了游戏前半部分和后半部分出现的位置集合。受试者会定期预测即将出现的目标位置,然后根据游戏的顺序属性对游戏进行相似性判断。在短时标和长时标中,参与者都表现出了对顺序信息的情境敏感性,而且短时标的学习效果更强。我们使用一个经过训练的门控递归网络对学习范式进行建模,以进行即时预测,该网络显示了多层次的学习时间历程以及对游戏相似性结构的敏感性模式,这与人类参与者的情况如出一辙。该模型将具有匹配规则结构的游戏分组,并根据低级顺序信息而不是高级顺序信息对游戏进行分离。这项工作显示了人类和模型是如何在不同的时间尺度上同时快速获取秩序信息的。
{"title":"Rapid Learning of Temporal Dependencies at Multiple Timescales","authors":"Cybelle M. Smith;Sharon L. Thompson-Schill;Anna C. Schapiro","doi":"10.1162/jocn_a_02232","DOIUrl":"10.1162/jocn_a_02232","url":null,"abstract":"Our environment contains temporal information unfolding simultaneously at multiple timescales. How do we learn and represent these dynamic and overlapping information streams? We investigated these processes in a statistical learning paradigm with simultaneous short and long timescale contingencies. Human participants (n = 96) played a game where they learned to quickly click on a target image when it appeared in one of nine locations, in eight different contexts. Across contexts, we manipulated the order of target locations: at a short timescale, the order of pairs of sequential locations in which the target appeared; at a longer timescale, the set of locations that appeared in the first versus the second half of the game. Participants periodically predicted the upcoming target location, and later performed similarity judgments comparing the games based on their order properties. Participants showed context-dependent sensitivity to order information at both short and long timescales, with evidence of stronger learning for short timescales. We modeled the learning paradigm using a gated recurrent network trained to make immediate predictions, which demonstrated multilevel learning timecourses and patterns of sensitivity to the similarity structure of the games that mirrored human participants. The model grouped games with matching rule structure and dissociated games based on low-level order information more so than high-level order information. The work shows how humans and models can rapidly and concurrently acquire order information at different timescales.","PeriodicalId":51081,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience","volume":"36 11","pages":"2343-2356"},"PeriodicalIF":3.1,"publicationDate":"2024-10-29","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141898869","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Human Visual Pathways for Action Recognition versus Deep Convolutional Neural Networks: Representation Correspondence in Late but Not Early Layers 用于动作识别的人类视觉通路与深度卷积神经网络:后期层而非早期层的表征对应性。
IF 3.1 3区 医学 Q2 NEUROSCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-10-29 DOI: 10.1162/jocn_a_02233
Yujia Peng;Xizi Gong;Hongjing Lu;Fang Fang
Deep convolutional neural networks (DCNNs) have attained human-level performance for object categorization and exhibited representation alignment between network layers and brain regions. Does such representation alignment naturally extend to other visual tasks beyond recognizing objects in static images? In this study, we expanded the exploration to the recognition of human actions from videos and assessed the representation capabilities and alignment of two-stream DCNNs in comparison with brain regions situated along ventral and dorsal pathways. Using decoding analysis and representational similarity analysis, we show that DCNN models do not show hierarchical representation alignment to human brain across visual regions when processing action videos. Instead, later layers of DCNN models demonstrate greater representation similarities to the human visual cortex. These findings were revealed for two display formats: photorealistic avatars with full-body information and simplified stimuli in the point-light display. The discrepancies in representation alignment suggest fundamental differences in how DCNNs and the human brain represent dynamic visual information related to actions.
深度卷积神经网络(DCNN)在物体分类方面的表现已达到人类水平,并显示出网络层与大脑区域之间的表征一致性。除了识别静态图像中的物体,这种表征一致性是否还能自然扩展到其他视觉任务?在这项研究中,我们将探索范围扩大到从视频中识别人类动作,并评估了双流 DCNN 与位于腹侧和背侧通路的脑区的表征能力和一致性。通过解码分析和表征相似性分析,我们发现 DCNN 模型在处理动作视频时并没有显示出与人脑各视觉区域的分层表征一致性。相反,DCNN 模型的后几层与人类视觉皮层表现出更大的表征相似性。这些发现针对两种显示格式:具有全身信息的逼真化身和点光源显示屏中的简化刺激。表征一致性的差异表明,DCNN 和人类大脑在如何表征与动作相关的动态视觉信息方面存在根本差异。
{"title":"Human Visual Pathways for Action Recognition versus Deep Convolutional Neural Networks: Representation Correspondence in Late but Not Early Layers","authors":"Yujia Peng;Xizi Gong;Hongjing Lu;Fang Fang","doi":"10.1162/jocn_a_02233","DOIUrl":"10.1162/jocn_a_02233","url":null,"abstract":"Deep convolutional neural networks (DCNNs) have attained human-level performance for object categorization and exhibited representation alignment between network layers and brain regions. Does such representation alignment naturally extend to other visual tasks beyond recognizing objects in static images? In this study, we expanded the exploration to the recognition of human actions from videos and assessed the representation capabilities and alignment of two-stream DCNNs in comparison with brain regions situated along ventral and dorsal pathways. Using decoding analysis and representational similarity analysis, we show that DCNN models do not show hierarchical representation alignment to human brain across visual regions when processing action videos. Instead, later layers of DCNN models demonstrate greater representation similarities to the human visual cortex. These findings were revealed for two display formats: photorealistic avatars with full-body information and simplified stimuli in the point-light display. The discrepancies in representation alignment suggest fundamental differences in how DCNNs and the human brain represent dynamic visual information related to actions.","PeriodicalId":51081,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience","volume":"36 11","pages":"2458-2480"},"PeriodicalIF":3.1,"publicationDate":"2024-10-29","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://ieeexplore.ieee.org/stamp/stamp.jsp?tp=&arnumber=10738325","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141898868","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Goal Shifts Structure Memories and Prioritize Event-defining Information in Memory 目标转移构建了记忆,并在记忆中优先考虑事件定义信息。
IF 3.1 3区 医学 Q2 NEUROSCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-10-29 DOI: 10.1162/jocn_a_02220
Emily T. Cowan;Avi J. Chanales;Lila Davachi;David Clewett
Every day, we encounter far more information than we could possibly remember. Thus, our memory systems must organize and prioritize the details from an experience that can adaptively guide the storage and retrieval of specific episodic events. Prior work has shown that shifts in internal goal states can function as event boundaries, chunking experiences into distinct and memorable episodes. In addition, at short delays, memory for contextual information at boundaries has been shown to be enhanced compared with items within each event. However, it remains unclear if these memory enhancements are limited to features that signal a meaningful transition between events. To determine how changes in dynamic goal states influence the organization and content of long-term memory, we designed a 2-day experiment in which participants viewed a series of black-and-white objects surrounded by a color border on a two-by-two grid. The location of the object on the grid determined which of two tasks participants performed on a given trial. To examine if distinct types of goal shifts modulate the effects of event segmentation, we changed the border color, the task, or both after every four items in a sequence. We found that goal shifts influenced temporal memory in a manner consistent with the formation of distinct events. However, for subjective memory representations in particular, these effects differed by the type of event boundary. Furthermore, to examine if goal shifts lead to the prioritization of goal-relevant features in longer lasting memories, we tested source memory for each object's color and grid location both immediately and after a 24-hr delay. On the immediate test, boundaries enhanced the memory for all concurrent source features compared with nonboundary items, but only if those boundaries involved a goal shift. In contrast, after a delay, the source memory was selectively enhanced for the feature relevant to the goal shift. These findings suggest that goals can adaptively structure memories by prioritizing contextual features that define a unique episode in memory.
每天,我们遇到的信息远远多于我们可能记住的信息。因此,我们的记忆系统必须对经历中的细节进行组织和优先排序,以适应性地指导特定情节事件的存储和检索。先前的研究表明,内部目标状态的变化可以作为事件的边界,将经验分成不同的、令人难忘的事件。此外,与每个事件中的项目相比,在短时间的延迟中,人们对边界处上下文信息的记忆会得到增强。然而,目前还不清楚这些记忆增强是否仅限于事件之间有意义过渡的特征。为了确定动态目标状态的变化如何影响长时记忆的组织和内容,我们设计了一个为期两天的实验。物体在网格上的位置决定了参与者在特定试验中执行的两项任务中的哪一项。为了研究不同类型的目标转移是否会调节事件分割的效果,我们在一个序列中每四个项目之后改变了边框的颜色、任务或两者。我们发现,目标转移对时间记忆的影响与不同事件的形成方式一致。然而,特别是对主观记忆表征而言,这些影响因事件边界的类型而异。此外,为了研究目标转移是否会导致目标相关特征在更持久记忆中的优先级,我们在即时和延迟 24 小时后测试了对每个物体的颜色和网格位置的源记忆。在即时测试中,与无边界项目相比,边界增强了对所有并发源特征的记忆,但前提是这些边界涉及目标转移。相反,在延迟之后,源记忆会选择性地增强与目标转移相关的特征。这些研究结果表明,目标可以通过优先选择定义记忆中独特情节的上下文特征来适应性地构建记忆。
{"title":"Goal Shifts Structure Memories and Prioritize Event-defining Information in Memory","authors":"Emily T. Cowan;Avi J. Chanales;Lila Davachi;David Clewett","doi":"10.1162/jocn_a_02220","DOIUrl":"10.1162/jocn_a_02220","url":null,"abstract":"Every day, we encounter far more information than we could possibly remember. Thus, our memory systems must organize and prioritize the details from an experience that can adaptively guide the storage and retrieval of specific episodic events. Prior work has shown that shifts in internal goal states can function as event boundaries, chunking experiences into distinct and memorable episodes. In addition, at short delays, memory for contextual information at boundaries has been shown to be enhanced compared with items within each event. However, it remains unclear if these memory enhancements are limited to features that signal a meaningful transition between events. To determine how changes in dynamic goal states influence the organization and content of long-term memory, we designed a 2-day experiment in which participants viewed a series of black-and-white objects surrounded by a color border on a two-by-two grid. The location of the object on the grid determined which of two tasks participants performed on a given trial. To examine if distinct types of goal shifts modulate the effects of event segmentation, we changed the border color, the task, or both after every four items in a sequence. We found that goal shifts influenced temporal memory in a manner consistent with the formation of distinct events. However, for subjective memory representations in particular, these effects differed by the type of event boundary. Furthermore, to examine if goal shifts lead to the prioritization of goal-relevant features in longer lasting memories, we tested source memory for each object's color and grid location both immediately and after a 24-hr delay. On the immediate test, boundaries enhanced the memory for all concurrent source features compared with nonboundary items, but only if those boundaries involved a goal shift. In contrast, after a delay, the source memory was selectively enhanced for the feature relevant to the goal shift. These findings suggest that goals can adaptively structure memories by prioritizing contextual features that define a unique episode in memory.","PeriodicalId":51081,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience","volume":"36 11","pages":"2415-2431"},"PeriodicalIF":3.1,"publicationDate":"2024-10-29","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141591990","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The Relationship between Event Boundary Strength and Pattern Shifts across the Cortical Hierarchy during Naturalistic Movie-viewing 自然电影观看过程中事件边界强度与大脑皮层模式转变之间的关系
IF 3.1 3区 医学 Q2 NEUROSCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-10-29 DOI: 10.1162/jocn_a_02213
Yoonjung Lee;Janice Chen
Our continuous experience is spontaneously segmented by the brain into discrete events. However, the beginning of a new event (an event boundary) is not always sharply identifiable: Phenomenologically, event boundaries vary in salience. How are the response profiles of cortical areas at event boundaries modulated by boundary strength during complex, naturalistic movie-viewing? Do cortical responses scale in a graded manner with boundary strength, or do they merely detect boundaries in a binary fashion? We measured “cortical boundary shifts” as transient changes in multivoxel patterns at event boundaries with different strengths (weak, moderate, and strong), determined by across-participant agreement. Cortical regions with different processing timescales were examined. In auditory areas, which have short timescales, cortical boundary shifts exhibited a clearly graded profile in both group-level and individual-level analyses. In cortical areas with long timescales, including the default mode network, boundary strength modulated pattern shift magnitude at the individual participant level. We also observed a positive relationship between boundary strength and the extent of temporal alignment of boundary shifts across different levels of the cortical hierarchy. In addition, hippocampal activity was highest at event boundaries for which cortical boundary shifts were most aligned across hierarchical levels. Overall, we found that event boundary strength modulated cortical pattern shifts strongly in sensory areas and more weakly in higher-level areas and that stronger boundaries were associated with greater alignment of these shifts across the cortical hierarchy.
我们的连续经验会被大脑自发地分割成离散的事件。然而,一个新事件的开始(事件边界)并不总是清晰可辨的:从现象上看,事件边界的显著性各不相同。在观看复杂、自然的电影时,大脑皮层区域在事件边界处的反应曲线是如何受边界强度调节的?大脑皮层的反应是随着边界强度的变化而分级,还是仅仅以二元方式检测边界?我们测量的 "皮层边界偏移 "是指不同强度(弱、中、强)的事件边界上多体素模式的瞬时变化,由参与者之间的一致性决定。我们对具有不同处理时间尺度的皮层区域进行了研究。在时间尺度较短的听觉区域,皮层边界移动在群体和个体分析中都表现出明显的分级特征。在时间尺度较长的皮层区域,包括默认模式网络,边界强度在个体参与者水平上调节模式偏移的幅度。我们还观察到边界强度与大脑皮层不同层次的边界移动的时间一致性之间存在正相关。此外,在大脑皮层边界移动在不同层次间最一致的事件边界处,海马活动最高。总之,我们发现事件边界强度对感觉区域皮层模式移动的调节作用很强,而对高层区域皮层模式移动的调节作用较弱。
{"title":"The Relationship between Event Boundary Strength and Pattern Shifts across the Cortical Hierarchy during Naturalistic Movie-viewing","authors":"Yoonjung Lee;Janice Chen","doi":"10.1162/jocn_a_02213","DOIUrl":"10.1162/jocn_a_02213","url":null,"abstract":"Our continuous experience is spontaneously segmented by the brain into discrete events. However, the beginning of a new event (an event boundary) is not always sharply identifiable: Phenomenologically, event boundaries vary in salience. How are the response profiles of cortical areas at event boundaries modulated by boundary strength during complex, naturalistic movie-viewing? Do cortical responses scale in a graded manner with boundary strength, or do they merely detect boundaries in a binary fashion? We measured “cortical boundary shifts” as transient changes in multivoxel patterns at event boundaries with different strengths (weak, moderate, and strong), determined by across-participant agreement. Cortical regions with different processing timescales were examined. In auditory areas, which have short timescales, cortical boundary shifts exhibited a clearly graded profile in both group-level and individual-level analyses. In cortical areas with long timescales, including the default mode network, boundary strength modulated pattern shift magnitude at the individual participant level. We also observed a positive relationship between boundary strength and the extent of temporal alignment of boundary shifts across different levels of the cortical hierarchy. In addition, hippocampal activity was highest at event boundaries for which cortical boundary shifts were most aligned across hierarchical levels. Overall, we found that event boundary strength modulated cortical pattern shifts strongly in sensory areas and more weakly in higher-level areas and that stronger boundaries were associated with greater alignment of these shifts across the cortical hierarchy.","PeriodicalId":51081,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience","volume":"36 11","pages":"2317-2342"},"PeriodicalIF":3.1,"publicationDate":"2024-10-29","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141591994","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
“All the Stars Will Be Wells with a Rusty Pulley”: Neural Processing of the Social and Pragmatic Content in a Narrative "所有的星星都将用生锈的滑轮打井":叙事中社会和实用内容的神经处理。
IF 3.1 3区 医学 Q2 NEUROSCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-10-29 DOI: 10.1162/jocn_a_02228
Melissa Thye;Paul Hoffman;Daniel Mirman
Making sense of natural language and narratives requires building and manipulating a situation model by adding incoming information to the model and using the context stored in the model to comprehend subsequent details and events. Situation model maintenance is supported by the default mode network (DMN), but comprehension of the individual moments in the narrative relies on access to the conceptual store within the semantic system. The present study examined how these systems are engaged by different narrative content to investigate whether highly informative, or semantic, content is a particularly strong driver of semantic system activation compared with contextually driven content that requires using the situation model, which might instead engage DMN regions. The study further investigated which subregions of the graded semantic hub in the left anterior temporal lobe (ATL) were engaged by the type of narrative content. To do this, we quantified the semantic, pragmatic, social, ambiguous, and emotional content for each sentence in a complete narrative, the English translation of The Little Prince. Increased activation in the transmodal hub in the ventral ATL was only observed for high semantic (i.e., informative) relative to low semantic sentences. Activation in the dorsolateral and ventrolateral ATL subregions was observed for both high relative to low semantic and social content sentences, but the ventrolateral ATL effects were more extensive in the social condition. There was high correspondence between the social and pragmatic content results, particularly in the ventrolateral ATL. We argue that the ventrolateral ATL may be particularly engaged by internal, or endogenous, processing demands, aided by functional connections between the anterior middle temporal gyrus and the DMN. Pragmatic and social content may have driven endogenous processing given the pervasive and plot-progressing nature of this content in the narrative. We put forward a revised account of how the semantic system is engaged in naturalistic contexts, a critical step toward better understanding real-world semantic and social processing.
要理解自然语言和叙事,就需要建立和操作一个情境模型,将输入的信息添加到模型中,并利用模型中存储的上下文来理解随后的细节和事件。情境模型的维持需要默认模式网络(DMN)的支持,但对叙述中各个时刻的理解则依赖于对语义系统中概念存储的访问。本研究考察了不同的叙述内容是如何调动这些系统的,以研究与需要使用情境模型的情境驱动型内容相比,信息量大或语义性强的内容是否是激活语义系统的特别强大的驱动力,而后者可能会调动 DMN 区域。该研究进一步调查了叙事内容的类型对左前颞叶(ATL)分级语义枢纽的哪些子区域产生了影响。为此,我们对《小王子》英译本这一完整叙事中每个句子的语义、语用、社会、歧义和情感内容进行了量化。与低语义句子相比,只有在高语义句子(即信息量大的句子)中才能观察到腹侧 ATL 跨模态中枢的激活增加。在高语义句子和低语义句子以及社会内容句子中,都可以观察到 ATL 背外侧和腹外侧子区域的激活,但在社会句子中,ATL 腹外侧的影响更为广泛。社交和语用内容结果之间的对应性很高,尤其是在腹外侧 ATL。我们认为,在颞叶前中回和 DMN 之间的功能连接的帮助下,腹外侧 ATL 可能特别受内部或内源性加工需求的影响。鉴于叙事中的实用性和社会性内容的普遍性和情节发展性,这些内容可能驱动了内源性加工。我们对语义系统在自然语境中的参与方式提出了新的解释,这是更好地理解现实世界语义和社会加工的关键一步。
{"title":"“All the Stars Will Be Wells with a Rusty Pulley”: Neural Processing of the Social and Pragmatic Content in a Narrative","authors":"Melissa Thye;Paul Hoffman;Daniel Mirman","doi":"10.1162/jocn_a_02228","DOIUrl":"10.1162/jocn_a_02228","url":null,"abstract":"Making sense of natural language and narratives requires building and manipulating a situation model by adding incoming information to the model and using the context stored in the model to comprehend subsequent details and events. Situation model maintenance is supported by the default mode network (DMN), but comprehension of the individual moments in the narrative relies on access to the conceptual store within the semantic system. The present study examined how these systems are engaged by different narrative content to investigate whether highly informative, or semantic, content is a particularly strong driver of semantic system activation compared with contextually driven content that requires using the situation model, which might instead engage DMN regions. The study further investigated which subregions of the graded semantic hub in the left anterior temporal lobe (ATL) were engaged by the type of narrative content. To do this, we quantified the semantic, pragmatic, social, ambiguous, and emotional content for each sentence in a complete narrative, the English translation of The Little Prince. Increased activation in the transmodal hub in the ventral ATL was only observed for high semantic (i.e., informative) relative to low semantic sentences. Activation in the dorsolateral and ventrolateral ATL subregions was observed for both high relative to low semantic and social content sentences, but the ventrolateral ATL effects were more extensive in the social condition. There was high correspondence between the social and pragmatic content results, particularly in the ventrolateral ATL. We argue that the ventrolateral ATL may be particularly engaged by internal, or endogenous, processing demands, aided by functional connections between the anterior middle temporal gyrus and the DMN. Pragmatic and social content may have driven endogenous processing given the pervasive and plot-progressing nature of this content in the narrative. We put forward a revised account of how the semantic system is engaged in naturalistic contexts, a critical step toward better understanding real-world semantic and social processing.","PeriodicalId":51081,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience","volume":"36 11","pages":"2495-2517"},"PeriodicalIF":3.1,"publicationDate":"2024-10-29","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141898866","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The Mindful Brain: A Systematic Review of the Neural Correlates of Trait Mindfulness 正念大脑:正念特质的神经相关性系统回顾》(The Mindful Brain: A Systematic Review of the Neural Correlates of Trait Mindfulness.
IF 3.1 3区 医学 Q2 NEUROSCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-10-29 DOI: 10.1162/jocn_a_02230
Isaac N. Treves;Kannammai Pichappan;Jude Hammoud;Clemens C. C. Bauer;Sebastian Ehmann;Matthew D. Sacchet;John D. E. Gabrieli
Trait self-report mindfulness scales measure one's disposition to pay nonjudgmental attention to the present moment. Concerns have been raised about the validity of trait mindfulness scales. Despite this, there is extensive literature correlating mindfulness scales with objective brain measures, with the goal of providing insight into mechanisms of mindfulness, and insight into associated positive mental health outcomes. Here, we systematically examined the neural correlates of trait mindfulness. We assessed 68 correlational studies across structural magnetic resonance imaging, task-based fMRI, resting-state fMRI, and EEG. Several consistent findings were identified, associating greater trait mindfulness with decreased amygdala reactivity to emotional stimuli, increased cortical thickness in frontal regions and insular cortex regions, and decreased connectivity within the default-mode network. These findings converged with results from intervention studies and those that included mindfulness experts. On the other hand, the connections between trait mindfulness and EEG metrics remain inconclusive, as do the associations between trait mindfulness and between-network resting-state fMRI metrics. ERP measures from EEG used to measure attentional or emotional processing may not show reliable individual variation. Research on body awareness and self-relevant processing is scarce. For a more robust correlational neuroscience of trait mindfulness, we recommend larger sample sizes, data-driven, multivariate approaches to self-report and brain measures, and careful consideration of test–retest reliability. In addition, we should leave behind simplistic explanations of mindfulness, as there are many ways to be mindful, and leave behind simplistic explanations of the brain, as distributed networks of brain areas support mindfulness.
特质自我报告正念量表测量的是一个人对当下不加评判地关注的倾向。有人对正念特质量表的有效性表示担忧。尽管如此,仍有大量文献将正念量表与客观的大脑测量相关联,目的是让人们了解正念的机制,以及相关的积极心理健康结果。在此,我们系统地研究了正念特质的神经相关性。我们评估了结构性磁共振成像、基于任务的 fMRI、静息态 fMRI 和脑电图的 68 项相关研究。我们发现了几项一致的研究结果,即更高的正念特质与杏仁核对情绪刺激的反应性降低、额叶区域和岛叶皮质区域的皮质厚度增加以及默认模式网络内的连接性降低有关。这些发现与干预研究和包括正念专家的研究结果一致。另一方面,正念特质与脑电图指标之间的联系仍无定论,正念特质与网络间静息态fMRI指标之间的联系也是如此。用于测量注意力或情绪处理的 EEG ERP 测量可能不会显示可靠的个体差异。有关身体意识和自我相关处理的研究还很少。为了对正念特质进行更稳健的相关神经科学研究,我们建议采用更大的样本量、数据驱动的多变量方法来进行自我报告和大脑测量,并仔细考虑测试-再测试的可靠性。此外,我们应该抛弃对正念的简单化解释,因为正念的方式有很多种;我们也应该抛弃对大脑的简单化解释,因为支持正念的是分布式脑区网络。
{"title":"The Mindful Brain: A Systematic Review of the Neural Correlates of Trait Mindfulness","authors":"Isaac N. Treves;Kannammai Pichappan;Jude Hammoud;Clemens C. C. Bauer;Sebastian Ehmann;Matthew D. Sacchet;John D. E. Gabrieli","doi":"10.1162/jocn_a_02230","DOIUrl":"10.1162/jocn_a_02230","url":null,"abstract":"Trait self-report mindfulness scales measure one's disposition to pay nonjudgmental attention to the present moment. Concerns have been raised about the validity of trait mindfulness scales. Despite this, there is extensive literature correlating mindfulness scales with objective brain measures, with the goal of providing insight into mechanisms of mindfulness, and insight into associated positive mental health outcomes. Here, we systematically examined the neural correlates of trait mindfulness. We assessed 68 correlational studies across structural magnetic resonance imaging, task-based fMRI, resting-state fMRI, and EEG. Several consistent findings were identified, associating greater trait mindfulness with decreased amygdala reactivity to emotional stimuli, increased cortical thickness in frontal regions and insular cortex regions, and decreased connectivity within the default-mode network. These findings converged with results from intervention studies and those that included mindfulness experts. On the other hand, the connections between trait mindfulness and EEG metrics remain inconclusive, as do the associations between trait mindfulness and between-network resting-state fMRI metrics. ERP measures from EEG used to measure attentional or emotional processing may not show reliable individual variation. Research on body awareness and self-relevant processing is scarce. For a more robust correlational neuroscience of trait mindfulness, we recommend larger sample sizes, data-driven, multivariate approaches to self-report and brain measures, and careful consideration of test–retest reliability. In addition, we should leave behind simplistic explanations of mindfulness, as there are many ways to be mindful, and leave behind simplistic explanations of the brain, as distributed networks of brain areas support mindfulness.","PeriodicalId":51081,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience","volume":"36 11","pages":"2518-2555"},"PeriodicalIF":3.1,"publicationDate":"2024-10-29","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://ieeexplore.ieee.org/stamp/stamp.jsp?tp=&arnumber=10738334","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141908301","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Remembering Sarah DuBrow across All Contexts 在各种背景下缅怀莎拉-杜布朗
IF 3.1 3区 医学 Q2 NEUROSCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-10-29 DOI: 10.1162/jocn_a_02189
Alexa Tompary;Megan T. deBettencourt;Nina Rouhani
{"title":"Remembering Sarah DuBrow across All Contexts","authors":"Alexa Tompary;Megan T. deBettencourt;Nina Rouhani","doi":"10.1162/jocn_a_02189","DOIUrl":"10.1162/jocn_a_02189","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":51081,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience","volume":"36 11","pages":"2455-2457"},"PeriodicalIF":3.1,"publicationDate":"2024-10-29","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140917370","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1