首页 > 最新文献

Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience最新文献

英文 中文
Is That You I Hear? Speaker Familiarity Modulates Neural Signatures of Lexical-semantic Activation in 18-month-old Infants 我听到的是你吗?扬声器熟悉度调节 18 个月大婴儿词义激活的神经特征
IF 3.1 3区 医学 Q2 NEUROSCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-08-16 DOI: 10.1162/jocn_a_02218
Clarissa Montgomery;Bahia Guellaï;Pia Rämä
Developmental language studies have shown that lexical-semantic organization develops between 18 and 24 months of age in monolingual infants. In the present study, we aimed to examine whether voice familiarity facilitates lexical-semantic activation in the infant brain. We recorded the brain activity of 18-month-old, French-learning infants using EEG while they listened to taxonomically related and unrelated spoken word pairs by one voice with which they were familiarized with before the experiment, and one voice with which they were not familiarized. The ERPs were measured in response to related and unrelated target words. Our results showed an N400 effect (greater amplitudes for unrelated as opposed to related target words) over the left hemisphere, only for the familiar voice, suggesting that the voice familiarity facilitated lexical-semantic activation. For unfamiliar voices, we observed an earlier congruence effect (greater amplitudes for related than for unrelated target words). This suggests that although 18-month-olds process lexical-semantic information from unfamiliar speakers, their neural signatures of lexical-semantic processing are less mature. Our results show that even in the absence of personal relation with a speaker, familiarity with a voice augments infant lexical-semantic processing. This supports the idea that extralinguistic information plays a role in infant lexical-semantic activation.
语言发育研究表明,单语婴儿的词汇-语义组织是在 18 到 24 个月大时形成的。在本研究中,我们旨在探讨语音熟悉程度是否会促进婴儿大脑中的词义激活。我们使用脑电图记录了 18 个月大的学习法语的婴儿在听分类学相关和不相关的口语词对时的大脑活动,其中一个声音是他们在实验前熟悉的,另一个是他们不熟悉的。对相关和不相关目标词的反应进行了 ERP 测量。我们的结果表明,只有熟悉的声音才会在左半球出现 N400 效应(与相关目标词相比,不相关目标词的振幅更大),这表明声音的熟悉程度促进了词汇-语义的激活。对于不熟悉的声音,我们观察到更早的一致性效应(相关目标词的振幅大于不相关目标词的振幅)。这表明,虽然 18 个月大的幼儿会处理来自陌生说话者的词义信息,但他们的词义处理神经特征还不太成熟。我们的研究结果表明,即使与说话者没有个人关系,对声音的熟悉程度也会增强婴儿的词义处理能力。这支持了语外信息在婴儿词义激活中发挥作用的观点。
{"title":"Is That You I Hear? Speaker Familiarity Modulates Neural Signatures of Lexical-semantic Activation in 18-month-old Infants","authors":"Clarissa Montgomery;Bahia Guellaï;Pia Rämä","doi":"10.1162/jocn_a_02218","DOIUrl":"10.1162/jocn_a_02218","url":null,"abstract":"Developmental language studies have shown that lexical-semantic organization develops between 18 and 24 months of age in monolingual infants. In the present study, we aimed to examine whether voice familiarity facilitates lexical-semantic activation in the infant brain. We recorded the brain activity of 18-month-old, French-learning infants using EEG while they listened to taxonomically related and unrelated spoken word pairs by one voice with which they were familiarized with before the experiment, and one voice with which they were not familiarized. The ERPs were measured in response to related and unrelated target words. Our results showed an N400 effect (greater amplitudes for unrelated as opposed to related target words) over the left hemisphere, only for the familiar voice, suggesting that the voice familiarity facilitated lexical-semantic activation. For unfamiliar voices, we observed an earlier congruence effect (greater amplitudes for related than for unrelated target words). This suggests that although 18-month-olds process lexical-semantic information from unfamiliar speakers, their neural signatures of lexical-semantic processing are less mature. Our results show that even in the absence of personal relation with a speaker, familiarity with a voice augments infant lexical-semantic processing. This supports the idea that extralinguistic information plays a role in infant lexical-semantic activation.","PeriodicalId":51081,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.1,"publicationDate":"2024-08-16","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141591991","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Model-based Mind Wandering in Older Adults: Age Differences in the Behavioral and Electrophysiological Correlates of Subjective and Objective Measures of Mind Wandering 老年人基于模型的思维徘徊:主观和客观思维徘徊测量的行为和电生理相关性的年龄差异。
IF 3.1 3区 医学 Q2 NEUROSCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-08-16 DOI: 10.1162/jocn_a_02195
Sarah E. Henderson;A. Dawn Ryan;Luke W. Atack;Karen Campbell
Mind wandering is typically characterized as a failure of attentional control, yet despite age-related executive function deficits, older adults typically report less mind wandering than younger adults during cognitive tasks and in daily life. Self-reported mind wandering episodes usually result in similar behavioral detriments in younger and older adults (e.g., greater RT variability, more task errors). However, the relatively few studies investigating the neural correlates of mind wandering and aging have revealed mixed findings, possibly because they typically rely on infrequent thought probes and, therefore, few trials for neural analyses. In the current study, we propose a method to recover more task data by categorizing trials from a commonly used sustained attention to response task according to RT variability. Behavioral data (n = 49 younger; n = 40 older) revealed that compared with younger adults, older adults reported fewer mind wandering episodes, but showed similar behavioral impacts thereof. Furthermore, in both age groups, subjective reports of mind wandering predicted the more objective sorting of trials into “on-” and “off-task” according to RT variability. Using these objectively sorted trials, we investigated two commonly reported EEG measures of mind wandering (diminished P1 and P3 amplitude) in 26 younger and 24 older adults. Although the P1 did not differ between on- and off-task trials for either group, the P3 was diminished for off-task trials in both age groups (albeit significantly less in older adults) suggesting preserved perceptual but reduced higher-order processing during off-task periods in both groups.
思维游离的典型特征是注意力控制失效,然而,尽管存在与年龄相关的执行功能缺陷,老年人在认知任务和日常生活中的思维游离程度通常低于年轻人。在年轻人和老年人中,自我报告的思维游离发作通常会导致类似的行为损害(如更大的RT变异性、更多的任务错误)。然而,调查思维游走与衰老的神经相关性的研究相对较少,其结果也不尽相同,这可能是因为这些研究通常依赖于不频繁的思维探测,因此用于神经分析的试验较少。在当前的研究中,我们提出了一种方法,通过将常用的持续注意反应任务中的试验按照RT变异性进行分类,从而恢复更多的任务数据。行为数据(n = 49 名年轻人;n = 40 名老年人)显示,与年轻人相比,老年人报告的思维游离发作次数较少,但表现出的行为影响相似。此外,在这两个年龄组中,对思维游移的主观报告都能预测根据实时变异性将试验分为 "任务中 "和 "任务外 "的更客观分类。利用这些客观分类的试验,我们对 26 名年轻人和 24 名老年人中两种常见的精神游离脑电图测量方法(P1 和 P3 振幅减弱)进行了研究。虽然两个年龄组的 P1 在任务中和任务外试验之间没有差异,但两个年龄组的 P3 在任务外试验中都有所减弱(尽管在老年人中明显减弱),这表明两个年龄组在任务外期间都保留了知觉处理,但减少了高阶处理。
{"title":"Model-based Mind Wandering in Older Adults: Age Differences in the Behavioral and Electrophysiological Correlates of Subjective and Objective Measures of Mind Wandering","authors":"Sarah E. Henderson;A. Dawn Ryan;Luke W. Atack;Karen Campbell","doi":"10.1162/jocn_a_02195","DOIUrl":"10.1162/jocn_a_02195","url":null,"abstract":"Mind wandering is typically characterized as a failure of attentional control, yet despite age-related executive function deficits, older adults typically report less mind wandering than younger adults during cognitive tasks and in daily life. Self-reported mind wandering episodes usually result in similar behavioral detriments in younger and older adults (e.g., greater RT variability, more task errors). However, the relatively few studies investigating the neural correlates of mind wandering and aging have revealed mixed findings, possibly because they typically rely on infrequent thought probes and, therefore, few trials for neural analyses. In the current study, we propose a method to recover more task data by categorizing trials from a commonly used sustained attention to response task according to RT variability. Behavioral data (n = 49 younger; n = 40 older) revealed that compared with younger adults, older adults reported fewer mind wandering episodes, but showed similar behavioral impacts thereof. Furthermore, in both age groups, subjective reports of mind wandering predicted the more objective sorting of trials into “on-” and “off-task” according to RT variability. Using these objectively sorted trials, we investigated two commonly reported EEG measures of mind wandering (diminished P1 and P3 amplitude) in 26 younger and 24 older adults. Although the P1 did not differ between on- and off-task trials for either group, the P3 was diminished for off-task trials in both age groups (albeit significantly less in older adults) suggesting preserved perceptual but reduced higher-order processing during off-task periods in both groups.","PeriodicalId":51081,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.1,"publicationDate":"2024-08-16","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141184695","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Comparing Neural Correlates of Memory Encoding and Maintenance for Foveal and Peripheral Stimuli 比较眼窝和周边刺激记忆编码和保持的神经相关性
IF 3.1 3区 医学 Q2 NEUROSCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-08-16 DOI: 10.1162/jocn_a_02203
Güven Kandemir;Chris Olivers
Visual working memory is believed to rely on top–down attentional mechanisms that sustain active sensory representations in early visual cortex, a mechanism referred to as sensory recruitment. However, both bottom–up sensory input and top–down attentional modulations thereof appear to prioritize the fovea over the periphery, such that initially peripheral percepts may even be assimilated by foveal processes. This raises the question whether and how visual working memory differs for central and peripheral input. To address this, we conducted a delayed orientation recall task in which an orientation was presented either at the center of the screen or at 15° eccentricity to the left or right. Response accuracy, EEG activity, and gaze position were recorded from 30 participants. Accuracy was slightly but significantly higher for foveal versus peripheral memories. Decoding of EEG recordings revealed a clear dissociation between early sensory and later maintenance signals. Although sensory signals were clearly decodable for foveal stimuli, they were not for peripheral input. In contrast, maintenance signals were equally decodable for both foveal and peripheral memories, suggesting comparable top–down components regardless of eccentricity. Moreover, although memory representations were initially spatially specific and reflected in voltage fluctuations, later during the maintenance period, they generalized across locations, as emerged in alpha oscillations, thus revealing a dynamic transformation within memory from separate sensory traces to what we propose are common output-related codes. Furthermore, the combined absence of reliable decoding of sensory signals and robust presence of maintenance decoding indicates that storage activity patterns as measured by EEG reflect signals beyond primary visual cortex. We discuss the implications for the sensory recruitment hypothesis.
人们认为,视觉工作记忆依赖于自上而下的注意机制,这种机制维持了早期视觉皮层中活跃的感觉表征,这种机制被称为感觉招募。然而,自下而上的感觉输入及其自上而下的注意调节似乎都优先于眼窝而非外围,因此最初的外围知觉甚至可能被眼窝过程同化。这就提出了一个问题:视觉工作记忆对于中心输入和外围输入是否不同,以及如何不同。为了解决这个问题,我们进行了一项延迟方位回忆任务,在这项任务中,方位出现在屏幕中央或偏左或偏右 15° 处。我们记录了 30 名参与者的反应准确性、脑电图活动和注视位置。眼窝记忆的准确率略高于外围记忆,但明显高于外围记忆。对脑电图记录的解码显示,早期感觉信号和后期维持信号之间存在明显的分离。虽然对眼窝刺激的感觉信号可以清楚地解码,但对外围输入的信号却不能。与此相反,维持信号对眼窝和外围记忆同样可解码,这表明无论偏心率如何,自上而下的成分都是相似的。此外,虽然记忆表征最初具有空间特异性,并反映在电压波动中,但在维持期的后期,记忆表征会跨位置泛化,表现为阿尔法振荡,从而揭示了记忆内部从独立的感觉痕迹到我们提出的共同输出相关代码的动态转变。此外,感觉信号没有可靠的解码,而维持性解码却强劲存在,这表明脑电图测量的存储活动模式反映了初级视觉皮层以外的信号。我们将讨论感觉招募假说的意义。
{"title":"Comparing Neural Correlates of Memory Encoding and Maintenance for Foveal and Peripheral Stimuli","authors":"Güven Kandemir;Chris Olivers","doi":"10.1162/jocn_a_02203","DOIUrl":"10.1162/jocn_a_02203","url":null,"abstract":"Visual working memory is believed to rely on top–down attentional mechanisms that sustain active sensory representations in early visual cortex, a mechanism referred to as sensory recruitment. However, both bottom–up sensory input and top–down attentional modulations thereof appear to prioritize the fovea over the periphery, such that initially peripheral percepts may even be assimilated by foveal processes. This raises the question whether and how visual working memory differs for central and peripheral input. To address this, we conducted a delayed orientation recall task in which an orientation was presented either at the center of the screen or at 15° eccentricity to the left or right. Response accuracy, EEG activity, and gaze position were recorded from 30 participants. Accuracy was slightly but significantly higher for foveal versus peripheral memories. Decoding of EEG recordings revealed a clear dissociation between early sensory and later maintenance signals. Although sensory signals were clearly decodable for foveal stimuli, they were not for peripheral input. In contrast, maintenance signals were equally decodable for both foveal and peripheral memories, suggesting comparable top–down components regardless of eccentricity. Moreover, although memory representations were initially spatially specific and reflected in voltage fluctuations, later during the maintenance period, they generalized across locations, as emerged in alpha oscillations, thus revealing a dynamic transformation within memory from separate sensory traces to what we propose are common output-related codes. Furthermore, the combined absence of reliable decoding of sensory signals and robust presence of maintenance decoding indicates that storage activity patterns as measured by EEG reflect signals beyond primary visual cortex. We discuss the implications for the sensory recruitment hypothesis.","PeriodicalId":51081,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.1,"publicationDate":"2024-08-16","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141472349","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Changes in Brain Functional Connectivity Underlying the Space-Number Association. 大脑功能连接性的变化是空间与数字关联的基础
IF 3.1 3区 医学 Q2 NEUROSCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-08-15 DOI: 10.1162/jocn_a_02240
Stefano Lasaponara, Mario Pinto, Silvana Lozito, Gabriele Scozia, Michele Pellegrino, Sara Lo Presti, Steve Gazzitano, Federico Giove, Fabrizio Doricchi

Whether small number magnitudes are inherently represented as lying to the left of larger ones, the space-number association (SNA), is an important issue in mathematical cognition. In this fMRI study, we used a go/no-go implicit association task to investigate the brain activity and functional connectivity underlying the SNA. Arabic digits lower or higher than 5 and left- or right-pointing arrows were alternated as central targets. In a single-code task condition, participants responded to a specific number magnitude and to all arrows or to a specific arrow direction and to all number magnitudes. In a joint-code (JC) condition, responses were provided after congruent, for example, "go when a number is lower than 5 or an arrow points left," or incongruent, for example, "go when a number is lower than 5 or an arrow points right," SNAs. The SNA was only found in the JC condition, where responses were faster with congruent instructions. Analyses of fMRI functional connectivity showed that the SNA was matched with enhanced excitatory inputs from ACC, the left TPJ, and the left inferior frontal gyrus to the left and right intraparietal sulcus (IPS). Incongruent JC trials were associated with enhanced excitatory modulation from ACC to the left and right IPS. These results show that the SNA is associated with enhanced activation of top-down brain control and changes in the functional interaction between the left and right IPS. We conclude that the SNA does not depend on an inherent and bottom-up spatial coding of number magnitudes.

空间数联想(SNA)是数学认知中的一个重要问题。在这项 fMRI 研究中,我们使用了 "走/不走 "内隐联想任务来研究空间数联想的大脑活动和功能连接。小于或大于 5 的阿拉伯数字和向左或向右的箭头交替作为中心目标。在单一代码任务条件下,参与者对特定数字大小和所有箭头做出反应,或对特定箭头方向和所有数字大小做出反应。在联合代码(JC)条件下,参与者会在一致(例如 "当数字小于 5 或箭头指向左边时走")或不一致(例如 "当数字小于 5 或箭头指向右边时走")的 SNA 后做出反应。只有在 JC 条件下才会出现 SNA,而在同向指令条件下反应更快。对 fMRI 功能连接的分析表明,SNA 与从 ACC、左 TPJ 和左额叶下回到左右顶内沟(IPS)的兴奋性输入增强相匹配。不一致的 JC 试验与来自 ACC 至左侧和右侧 IPS 的兴奋调节增强有关。这些结果表明,SNA 与自上而下的大脑控制激活增强以及左右 IPS 之间的功能互动变化有关。我们的结论是,SNA 并不依赖于固有的、自下而上的数字幅度空间编码。
{"title":"Changes in Brain Functional Connectivity Underlying the Space-Number Association.","authors":"Stefano Lasaponara, Mario Pinto, Silvana Lozito, Gabriele Scozia, Michele Pellegrino, Sara Lo Presti, Steve Gazzitano, Federico Giove, Fabrizio Doricchi","doi":"10.1162/jocn_a_02240","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn_a_02240","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Whether small number magnitudes are inherently represented as lying to the left of larger ones, the space-number association (SNA), is an important issue in mathematical cognition. In this fMRI study, we used a go/no-go implicit association task to investigate the brain activity and functional connectivity underlying the SNA. Arabic digits lower or higher than 5 and left- or right-pointing arrows were alternated as central targets. In a single-code task condition, participants responded to a specific number magnitude and to all arrows or to a specific arrow direction and to all number magnitudes. In a joint-code (JC) condition, responses were provided after congruent, for example, \"go when a number is lower than 5 or an arrow points left,\" or incongruent, for example, \"go when a number is lower than 5 or an arrow points right,\" SNAs. The SNA was only found in the JC condition, where responses were faster with congruent instructions. Analyses of fMRI functional connectivity showed that the SNA was matched with enhanced excitatory inputs from ACC, the left TPJ, and the left inferior frontal gyrus to the left and right intraparietal sulcus (IPS). Incongruent JC trials were associated with enhanced excitatory modulation from ACC to the left and right IPS. These results show that the SNA is associated with enhanced activation of top-down brain control and changes in the functional interaction between the left and right IPS. We conclude that the SNA does not depend on an inherent and bottom-up spatial coding of number magnitudes.</p>","PeriodicalId":51081,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.1,"publicationDate":"2024-08-15","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141983857","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The Visual Scene Network and Hippocampus Represent Spatial Boundary Structures for Temporal Episodic Memory Organization. 视觉场景网络和海马代表了时空记忆组织的空间边界结构
IF 3.1 3区 医学 Q2 NEUROSCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-08-05 DOI: 10.1162/jocn_a_02229
Yu Jin Rah, Jung Han Shin, Sang Ah Lee

Crossing a spatial boundary such as a doorway plays an important role in the temporal organization of episodic memory. However, despite the wealth of evidence showing that visual boundary structures in scenes affect our representation of space, no studies have tested the possibility that they also function as event boundaries even without active navigation. In this study, we used a nonnavigational scene-based memory task that required participants to remember a sequence of objects moving to various baskets in a scene. In the boundary condition, there was a freestanding boundary in the middle of the room, low enough to see the rest of the room beyond it. We found that the additional boundary within the scene was sufficient to trigger a larger response in the cortical visual scene network, the hippocampus, and their coordinated activity. These results suggest that active navigation across a spatial boundary such as a doorway into another room is not necessary to form an event boundary and that a visual representation of boundaries is sufficient to influence the organization of a hippocampal episodic memory.

跨越门廊等空间边界在记忆的时间组织中起着重要作用。然而,尽管有大量证据表明场景中的视觉边界结构会影响我们对空间的表征,但还没有研究测试过它们在没有主动导航的情况下也能作为事件边界发挥作用的可能性。在这项研究中,我们使用了一个非导航场景记忆任务,要求参与者记住一个场景中物体移动到不同篮子的序列。在边界条件下,房间中央有一个独立的边界,边界很低,可以看到边界外房间的其他部分。我们发现,场景中的额外边界足以引发大脑皮层视觉场景网络、海马体及其协调活动的更大反应。这些结果表明,主动导航穿过空间边界(如进入另一个房间的门口)并非形成事件边界的必要条件,而边界的视觉表征足以影响海马表观记忆的组织。
{"title":"The Visual Scene Network and Hippocampus Represent Spatial Boundary Structures for Temporal Episodic Memory Organization.","authors":"Yu Jin Rah, Jung Han Shin, Sang Ah Lee","doi":"10.1162/jocn_a_02229","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn_a_02229","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Crossing a spatial boundary such as a doorway plays an important role in the temporal organization of episodic memory. However, despite the wealth of evidence showing that visual boundary structures in scenes affect our representation of space, no studies have tested the possibility that they also function as event boundaries even without active navigation. In this study, we used a nonnavigational scene-based memory task that required participants to remember a sequence of objects moving to various baskets in a scene. In the boundary condition, there was a freestanding boundary in the middle of the room, low enough to see the rest of the room beyond it. We found that the additional boundary within the scene was sufficient to trigger a larger response in the cortical visual scene network, the hippocampus, and their coordinated activity. These results suggest that active navigation across a spatial boundary such as a doorway into another room is not necessary to form an event boundary and that a visual representation of boundaries is sufficient to influence the organization of a hippocampal episodic memory.</p>","PeriodicalId":51081,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.1,"publicationDate":"2024-08-05","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141898870","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Two "What" Networks in the Human Brain. 人脑中的两个 "什么 "网络
IF 3.1 3区 医学 Q2 NEUROSCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-08-05 DOI: 10.1162/jocn_a_02234
Maryam Vaziri-Pashkam

Ungerleider and Mishkin, in their influential work that relied on detailed anatomical and ablation studies, suggested that visual information is processed along two distinct pathways: the dorsal "where" pathway, primarily responsible for spatial vision, and the ventral "what" pathway, dedicated to object vision. This strict division of labor has faced challenges in light of compelling evidence revealing robust shape and object selectivity within the putative "where" pathway. This article reviews evidence that supports the presence of shape selectivity in the dorsal pathway. A comparative examination of dorsal and ventral object representations in terms of invariance, task dependency, and representational content reveals similarities and differences between the two pathways. Both exhibit some level of tolerance to image transformations and are influenced by tasks, but responses in the dorsal pathway show weaker tolerance and stronger task modulations than those in the ventral pathway. Furthermore, an examination of their representational content highlights a divergence between the responses in the two pathways, suggesting that they are sensitive to distinct features of objects. Collectively, these findings suggest that two networks exist in the human brain for processing object shapes, one in the dorsal and another in the ventral visual cortex. These studies lay the foundation for future research aimed at revealing the precise roles the two "what" networks play in our ability to understand and interact with objects.

昂格尔莱德和米什金在他们颇具影响力的工作中,依靠详细的解剖和消融研究,提出视觉信息是沿着两条不同的通路进行处理的:背侧的 "哪里 "通路主要负责空间视觉,而腹侧的 "什么 "通路则专门负责物体视觉。这种严格的分工面临着挑战,因为有令人信服的证据显示,在假定的 "哪里 "通路中存在着强大的形状和物体选择性。本文回顾了支持背侧通路存在形状选择性的证据。从不变性、任务依赖性和表征内容等方面对背侧和腹侧的物体表征进行比较研究,揭示了这两条通路的异同。两者都对图像变换表现出一定程度的耐受性,并受到任务的影响,但与腹侧通路相比,背侧通路的反应表现出更弱的耐受性和更强的任务调节性。此外,对其表征内容的研究也凸显了两条通路反应的差异,这表明它们对物体的不同特征非常敏感。总之,这些研究结果表明,人脑中存在两个处理物体形状的网络,一个在背侧,另一个在腹侧视觉皮层。这些研究为今后的研究奠定了基础,研究旨在揭示这两个 "什么 "网络在我们理解物体并与之互动的能力中所发挥的确切作用。
{"title":"Two \"What\" Networks in the Human Brain.","authors":"Maryam Vaziri-Pashkam","doi":"10.1162/jocn_a_02234","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn_a_02234","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Ungerleider and Mishkin, in their influential work that relied on detailed anatomical and ablation studies, suggested that visual information is processed along two distinct pathways: the dorsal \"where\" pathway, primarily responsible for spatial vision, and the ventral \"what\" pathway, dedicated to object vision. This strict division of labor has faced challenges in light of compelling evidence revealing robust shape and object selectivity within the putative \"where\" pathway. This article reviews evidence that supports the presence of shape selectivity in the dorsal pathway. A comparative examination of dorsal and ventral object representations in terms of invariance, task dependency, and representational content reveals similarities and differences between the two pathways. Both exhibit some level of tolerance to image transformations and are influenced by tasks, but responses in the dorsal pathway show weaker tolerance and stronger task modulations than those in the ventral pathway. Furthermore, an examination of their representational content highlights a divergence between the responses in the two pathways, suggesting that they are sensitive to distinct features of objects. Collectively, these findings suggest that two networks exist in the human brain for processing object shapes, one in the dorsal and another in the ventral visual cortex. These studies lay the foundation for future research aimed at revealing the precise roles the two \"what\" networks play in our ability to understand and interact with objects.</p>","PeriodicalId":51081,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.1,"publicationDate":"2024-08-05","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141898916","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Children's Memory for Events: The Challenge of Free Recall. 儿童对事件的记忆:自由回忆的挑战
IF 3.1 3区 医学 Q2 NEUROSCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-07-17 DOI: 10.1162/jocn_a_02221
Susan L Benear, Obinnaya J Onwukanjo, Ingrid R Olson, Nora S Newcombe

Early childhood is a critical period for episodic memory development, with sharp behavioral improvements between ages 4 to 7 years. Prior work has demonstrated this extensively with prompted memory tasks, but we explored performance on unprompted, free recall of a naturalistic experience in children, and how their performance relates to other cognitive measures. We asked children and adults to view a television episode, a naturalistic task for which there exists a ground truth, and assessed their free recall memory for the episode. Children's free recall performance improved dramatically with age, with many young children producing no verbal free recall whatsoever, although prompted recognition memory measures showed retention of material. However, the detail in free recall was related to both recognition and temporal order forced-choice memory performance in our full sample, showing agreement among memory measures. Free recall was strongly predicted by verbal skills, suggesting that children's sparse recall reflects verbal skill development rather than a pure mnemonic deficit. We propose that free recall has a more protracted developmental trajectory because it requires more substantial verbal skills as well as metacognitive skills that direct memory search, as compared with forced-choice memory tasks.

幼儿期是外显记忆发展的关键时期,在 4 到 7 岁期间,行为能力会有显著提高。之前的研究已通过提示性记忆任务广泛证明了这一点,但我们探讨了儿童在无提示、自由回忆自然体验时的表现,以及他们的表现与其他认知测量的关系。我们要求儿童和成人观看一集电视节目,这是一项存在基本事实的自然任务,然后评估他们对这一集节目的自由回忆记忆。儿童的自由回忆能力随着年龄的增长而显著提高,许多幼儿根本无法进行口头自由回忆,但提示性辨认记忆测量却显示他们保留了材料。不过,在我们的全部样本中,自由回忆的细节与识别和时序强迫选择记忆的表现都有关系,这显示了记忆测量之间的一致性。言语技能对自由回忆有很强的预测作用,这表明儿童的稀疏回忆反映了言语技能的发展,而不是单纯的记忆力缺陷。我们认为,与强制选择记忆任务相比,自由回忆需要更多的语言技能以及指导记忆搜索的元认知技能,因此其发展轨迹更为漫长。
{"title":"Children's Memory for Events: The Challenge of Free Recall.","authors":"Susan L Benear, Obinnaya J Onwukanjo, Ingrid R Olson, Nora S Newcombe","doi":"10.1162/jocn_a_02221","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn_a_02221","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Early childhood is a critical period for episodic memory development, with sharp behavioral improvements between ages 4 to 7 years. Prior work has demonstrated this extensively with prompted memory tasks, but we explored performance on unprompted, free recall of a naturalistic experience in children, and how their performance relates to other cognitive measures. We asked children and adults to view a television episode, a naturalistic task for which there exists a ground truth, and assessed their free recall memory for the episode. Children's free recall performance improved dramatically with age, with many young children producing no verbal free recall whatsoever, although prompted recognition memory measures showed retention of material. However, the detail in free recall was related to both recognition and temporal order forced-choice memory performance in our full sample, showing agreement among memory measures. Free recall was strongly predicted by verbal skills, suggesting that children's sparse recall reflects verbal skill development rather than a pure mnemonic deficit. We propose that free recall has a more protracted developmental trajectory because it requires more substantial verbal skills as well as metacognitive skills that direct memory search, as compared with forced-choice memory tasks.</p>","PeriodicalId":51081,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.1,"publicationDate":"2024-07-17","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141635631","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Entities, Uncertainties, and Behavioral Indicators of Consciousness 实体、不确定性和意识的行为指标。
IF 3.1 3区 医学 Q2 NEUROSCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-07-01 DOI: 10.1162/jocn_a_02130
L. Syd M Johnson
Two problems related to the identification of consciousness are the distribution problem—or how and among which entities consciousness is distributed in the world—and the moral status problem—or which species, entities, and individuals have moral status. The use of inferences from neurobiological and behavioral evidence, and their confounds, for identifying consciousness in nontypically functioning humans, nonhuman animals, and artificial intelligence is considered in light of significant scientific uncertainty and ethical biases, with implications for both problems. Methodological, epistemic, and ethical consensus are needed for responsible consciousness science under epistemic and ethical uncertainty. Consideration of inductive risk is proposed as a potential tool for managing both epistemic and ethical risks in consciousness science.
与意识识别相关的两个问题是分布问题--即意识在世界上如何分布以及在哪些实体中分布;以及道德地位问题--即哪些物种、实体和个体具有道德地位。本文从重大的科学不确定性和伦理偏见出发,探讨了如何利用神经生物学和行为学证据的推论及其混淆物来识别非典型功能的人类、非人类动物和人工智能中的意识,以及对这两个问题的影响。在认识论和伦理学的不确定性下,负责任的意识科学需要方法论、认识论和伦理学方面的共识。建议考虑归纳风险,将其作为管理意识科学中认识论和伦理风险的潜在工具。
{"title":"Entities, Uncertainties, and Behavioral Indicators of Consciousness","authors":"L. Syd M Johnson","doi":"10.1162/jocn_a_02130","DOIUrl":"10.1162/jocn_a_02130","url":null,"abstract":"Two problems related to the identification of consciousness are the distribution problem—or how and among which entities consciousness is distributed in the world—and the moral status problem—or which species, entities, and individuals have moral status. The use of inferences from neurobiological and behavioral evidence, and their confounds, for identifying consciousness in nontypically functioning humans, nonhuman animals, and artificial intelligence is considered in light of significant scientific uncertainty and ethical biases, with implications for both problems. Methodological, epistemic, and ethical consensus are needed for responsible consciousness science under epistemic and ethical uncertainty. Consideration of inductive risk is proposed as a potential tool for managing both epistemic and ethical risks in consciousness science.","PeriodicalId":51081,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.1,"publicationDate":"2024-07-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139736669","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Metacognitive Awareness and the Subjective Experience of Remembering in Aphantasia 元认知意识与幻觉症患者记忆的主观体验
IF 3.1 3区 医学 Q2 NEUROSCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-07-01 DOI: 10.1162/jocn_a_02120
Michael J. Siena;Jon S. Simons
Individuals with aphantasia, a nonclinical condition typically characterized by mental imagery deficits, often report reduced episodic memory. However, findings have hitherto rested largely on subjective self-reports, with few studies experimentally investigating both objective and subjective aspects of episodic memory in aphantasia. In this study, we tested both aspects of remembering in aphantasic individuals using a custom 3-D object and spatial memory task that manipulated visuospatial perspective, which is considered to be a key factor determining the subjective experience of remembering. Objective and subjective measures of memory performance were taken for both object and spatial memory features under different perspective conditions. Surprisingly, aphantasic participants were found to be unimpaired on all objective memory measures, including those for object memory features, despite reporting weaker overall mental imagery experience and lower subjective vividness ratings on the memory task. These results add to newly emerging evidence that aphantasia is a heterogenous condition, where some aphantasic individuals may lack metacognitive awareness of mental imagery rather than mental imagery itself. In addition, we found that both participant groups remembered object memory features with greater precision when encoded and retrieved in the first person versus third person, suggesting a first-person perspective might facilitate subjective memory reliving by enhancing the representational quality of scene contents.
幻觉症是一种非临床病症,典型特征是心理意象障碍,幻觉症患者经常报告其外显记忆减少。然而,迄今为止,研究结果主要依赖于主观的自我报告,很少有研究通过实验对象皮症患者外显记忆的客观和主观方面进行调查。在这项研究中,我们使用了一项定制的三维物体和空间记忆任务来测试象失患者记忆的两个方面,该任务操纵了视觉空间视角,而视觉空间视角被认为是决定记忆主观体验的关键因素。在不同的视角条件下,对物体和空间记忆特征的记忆表现进行了客观和主观测量。令人惊讶的是,尽管象觉受试者在记忆任务中的整体心理意象体验较弱,主观生动性评分较低,但他们在所有客观记忆测量(包括对象记忆特征的测量)中都没有受到影响。这些结果补充了新出现的证据,即象觉障碍是一种异质性疾病,一些象觉障碍患者可能缺乏对心理意象的元认知意识,而不是心理意象本身。此外,我们还发现,在以第一人称编码和检索时,两组受试者对物体记忆特征的记忆精度都高于以第三人称编码和检索时的记忆精度,这表明第一人称视角可能会通过提高场景内容的表征质量来促进主观记忆重现。
{"title":"Metacognitive Awareness and the Subjective Experience of Remembering in Aphantasia","authors":"Michael J. Siena;Jon S. Simons","doi":"10.1162/jocn_a_02120","DOIUrl":"10.1162/jocn_a_02120","url":null,"abstract":"Individuals with aphantasia, a nonclinical condition typically characterized by mental imagery deficits, often report reduced episodic memory. However, findings have hitherto rested largely on subjective self-reports, with few studies experimentally investigating both objective and subjective aspects of episodic memory in aphantasia. In this study, we tested both aspects of remembering in aphantasic individuals using a custom 3-D object and spatial memory task that manipulated visuospatial perspective, which is considered to be a key factor determining the subjective experience of remembering. Objective and subjective measures of memory performance were taken for both object and spatial memory features under different perspective conditions. Surprisingly, aphantasic participants were found to be unimpaired on all objective memory measures, including those for object memory features, despite reporting weaker overall mental imagery experience and lower subjective vividness ratings on the memory task. These results add to newly emerging evidence that aphantasia is a heterogenous condition, where some aphantasic individuals may lack metacognitive awareness of mental imagery rather than mental imagery itself. In addition, we found that both participant groups remembered object memory features with greater precision when encoded and retrieved in the first person versus third person, suggesting a first-person perspective might facilitate subjective memory reliving by enhancing the representational quality of scene contents.","PeriodicalId":51081,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.1,"publicationDate":"2024-07-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139698880","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Developmental Roots of Human Self-consciousness 人类自我意识的发展根源。
IF 3.1 3区 医学 Q2 NEUROSCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-07-01 DOI: 10.1162/jocn_a_02117
Philippe Rochat
Human consciousness is considered in the perspective of early development. Infants and young children remind us that at its core, the problem of consciousness is primarily a problem of identity, in particular a problem of self-identity with others in mind. It is about how we feel and construe ourselves as an entity among other entities. It is about becoming co-conscious: Aware of oneself through the evaluative eyes of others. This development unfolds in the first 18 months of life, following major steps that are described, and arguably considered as a human trademark.
从早期发展的角度来审视人类的意识。婴幼儿提醒我们,意识问题的核心主要是身份认同问题,尤其是心中有他人的自我认同问题。它关系到我们如何感受和理解自己是其他实体中的一个实体。这是一个关于共同意识的问题:通过他人的评价眼光来认识自己。这种发展在人出生后的头 18 个月中展开,遵循着描述的主要步骤,可以说是人类的标志。
{"title":"Developmental Roots of Human Self-consciousness","authors":"Philippe Rochat","doi":"10.1162/jocn_a_02117","DOIUrl":"10.1162/jocn_a_02117","url":null,"abstract":"Human consciousness is considered in the perspective of early development. Infants and young children remind us that at its core, the problem of consciousness is primarily a problem of identity, in particular a problem of self-identity with others in mind. It is about how we feel and construe ourselves as an entity among other entities. It is about becoming co-conscious: Aware of oneself through the evaluative eyes of others. This development unfolds in the first 18 months of life, following major steps that are described, and arguably considered as a human trademark.","PeriodicalId":51081,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.1,"publicationDate":"2024-07-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139693522","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1