首页 > 最新文献

Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience最新文献

英文 中文
The Mechanisms and Neural Signature of Time-averaged Numerosity Perception. 时间平均数值感知的机制和神经特征。
IF 3.1 3区 医学 Q2 NEUROSCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-10-18 DOI: 10.1162/jocn_a_02263
Irene Togoli, Olivier Collignon, Domenica Bueti, Michele Fornaciai

The animal brain is endowed with an innate sense of number allowing to intuitively perceive the approximate quantity of items in a scene, or "numerosity." This ability is not limited to items distributed in space, but also to events unfolding in time and to the average numerosity of dynamic scenes. How the brain computes and represents the average numerosity over time, however, remains unclear. Here, we investigate the mechanisms and EEG signature of the perception of average numerosity over time. To do so, we used stimuli composed of a variable number (3-12) of briefly presented dot arrays (50 msec each) and asked participants to judge the average numerosity of the sequence. We first show that the weight of different portions of the stimuli in determining the judgment depends on how many arrays are included in the sequence itself: the longer the sequence, the lower the weight of the latest arrays. Second, we show systematic adaptation effects across stimuli in consecutive trials. Importantly, the EEG results highlight two processing stages whereby the amplitude of occipital ERPs reflects the adaptation effect (∼300 msec after stimulus onset) and the accuracy and precision of average numerosity judgments (∼450-700 msec). These two stages are consistent with processes involved with the representation of perceived average numerosity and with perceptual decision-making, respectively. Overall, our findings provide new evidence showing how the visual system computes the average numerosity of dynamic visual stimuli, and support the existence of a dedicated, relatively low-level perceptual mechanism mediating this process.

动物大脑具有与生俱来的数字感,能够直观地感知场景中物品的大致数量,即 "数量感"。这种能力不仅限于空间分布的物品,还包括在时间中展开的事件和动态场景的平均数量。然而,大脑是如何计算和表示随时间变化的平均数值的,目前仍不清楚。在这里,我们研究了感知随时间变化的平均数值的机制和脑电图特征。为此,我们使用了由不同数量(3-12 个)短暂呈现的点阵(每个点阵 50 毫秒)组成的刺激物,并要求参与者判断序列的平均数字度。我们首先证明,刺激物不同部分在判断中的权重取决于序列本身包含多少个点阵:序列越长,最新点阵的权重越低。其次,我们显示了连续试验中不同刺激的系统适应效应。重要的是,脑电图结果突出了两个处理阶段,枕叶 ERP 的振幅反映了适应效应(刺激开始后 300 毫秒)和平均数字判断的准确性和精确性(450-700 毫秒)。这两个阶段分别与感知平均数值的表征过程和感知决策过程相一致。总之,我们的研究结果提供了新的证据,说明视觉系统是如何计算动态视觉刺激的平均数值的,并支持存在一种专门的、相对低级的知觉机制来介导这一过程。
{"title":"The Mechanisms and Neural Signature of Time-averaged Numerosity Perception.","authors":"Irene Togoli, Olivier Collignon, Domenica Bueti, Michele Fornaciai","doi":"10.1162/jocn_a_02263","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn_a_02263","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The animal brain is endowed with an innate sense of number allowing to intuitively perceive the approximate quantity of items in a scene, or \"numerosity.\" This ability is not limited to items distributed in space, but also to events unfolding in time and to the average numerosity of dynamic scenes. How the brain computes and represents the average numerosity over time, however, remains unclear. Here, we investigate the mechanisms and EEG signature of the perception of average numerosity over time. To do so, we used stimuli composed of a variable number (3-12) of briefly presented dot arrays (50 msec each) and asked participants to judge the average numerosity of the sequence. We first show that the weight of different portions of the stimuli in determining the judgment depends on how many arrays are included in the sequence itself: the longer the sequence, the lower the weight of the latest arrays. Second, we show systematic adaptation effects across stimuli in consecutive trials. Importantly, the EEG results highlight two processing stages whereby the amplitude of occipital ERPs reflects the adaptation effect (∼300 msec after stimulus onset) and the accuracy and precision of average numerosity judgments (∼450-700 msec). These two stages are consistent with processes involved with the representation of perceived average numerosity and with perceptual decision-making, respectively. Overall, our findings provide new evidence showing how the visual system computes the average numerosity of dynamic visual stimuli, and support the existence of a dedicated, relatively low-level perceptual mechanism mediating this process.</p>","PeriodicalId":51081,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience","volume":" ","pages":"1-17"},"PeriodicalIF":3.1,"publicationDate":"2024-10-18","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142480183","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The Suppression Mechanisms of Passive Memory in Visual Working Memory: The Evidence from Electrophysiology. 视觉工作记忆中被动记忆的抑制机制:电生理学的证据
IF 3.1 3区 医学 Q2 NEUROSCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-10-18 DOI: 10.1162/jocn_a_02265
Ziyuan Li, Wenjin Guo, Na Zhao, Qiang Liu

Recent studies of visual working memory (VWM) underscore a structured hierarchy of storage states. Memories that are not immediately relevant to the task at hand but are essential for later use are transferred to a passive state, which operates independently of actively maintaining and manipulating current memories. Note that stimulating passive memory forcefully can reactivate it into an active state, resulting in a competition with active memory. Thus, to remain stable representations for both states within VWM, passive memory might involve sustained suppression during activity-silent maintenance to prevent reactivation from disrupting the current active storage. To investigate this, we analyzed lateralized electrophysiology signals while human participants (both women and men) were engaged in a sequential presentation memory task across two experiments. The results revealed positive contralateral delayed activity components and lateralized alpha enhancement for passive memory, neural indicative of suppression on passive storage. In addition, the suppression effect was independent of the memory load in both the active and the passive states. These findings support the notion of sustained suppression during activity-silent maintenance of passive memory, facilitating the stable maintenance of distinct storage states and advancing our understanding of the dynamic coding framework in VWM.

最近对视觉工作记忆(VWM)的研究强调了存储状态的结构层次。与当前任务不直接相关但对以后使用至关重要的记忆会被转移到被动状态,这种状态的运作与主动保持和操纵当前记忆无关。需要注意的是,强行刺激被动记忆会使其重新进入主动状态,从而导致与主动记忆的竞争。因此,为了在 VWM 中保持两种状态的稳定表征,被动记忆可能需要在活动-静默维持期间进行持续抑制,以防止重新激活破坏当前的主动存储。为了研究这一点,我们在两次实验中分析了人类参与者(包括女性和男性)进行顺序呈现记忆任务时的侧向电生理学信号。结果显示,被动记忆的对侧延迟活动成分呈阳性,侧向α增强,这表明神经抑制了被动存储。此外,在主动和被动状态下,抑制效果与记忆负荷无关。这些发现支持了在被动记忆的活动-静默维持过程中持续抑制的概念,促进了不同存储状态的稳定维持,并推进了我们对大众汽车记忆动态编码框架的理解。
{"title":"The Suppression Mechanisms of Passive Memory in Visual Working Memory: The Evidence from Electrophysiology.","authors":"Ziyuan Li, Wenjin Guo, Na Zhao, Qiang Liu","doi":"10.1162/jocn_a_02265","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn_a_02265","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Recent studies of visual working memory (VWM) underscore a structured hierarchy of storage states. Memories that are not immediately relevant to the task at hand but are essential for later use are transferred to a passive state, which operates independently of actively maintaining and manipulating current memories. Note that stimulating passive memory forcefully can reactivate it into an active state, resulting in a competition with active memory. Thus, to remain stable representations for both states within VWM, passive memory might involve sustained suppression during activity-silent maintenance to prevent reactivation from disrupting the current active storage. To investigate this, we analyzed lateralized electrophysiology signals while human participants (both women and men) were engaged in a sequential presentation memory task across two experiments. The results revealed positive contralateral delayed activity components and lateralized alpha enhancement for passive memory, neural indicative of suppression on passive storage. In addition, the suppression effect was independent of the memory load in both the active and the passive states. These findings support the notion of sustained suppression during activity-silent maintenance of passive memory, facilitating the stable maintenance of distinct storage states and advancing our understanding of the dynamic coding framework in VWM.</p>","PeriodicalId":51081,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience","volume":" ","pages":"1-11"},"PeriodicalIF":3.1,"publicationDate":"2024-10-18","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142480185","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The Neural Specificity of Interference Resolution in Phonological, Semantic, and Visual Domains at Different Ages. 不同年龄段语音、语义和视觉领域干扰解析的神经特异性
IF 3.1 3区 医学 Q2 NEUROSCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-10-18 DOI: 10.1162/jocn_a_02260
Coline Grégoire, Lucie Attout, Christophe Phillips, Lucas Rifon, Louis Hody, Steve Majerus

The question of whether cognitive control is specific to certain domains or domain-general remains an extensively debated question at both cognitive and neural levels. This study examined the neural substrates associated with resistance to interference (RI) in phonological, semantic, and visual domains by using strictly matched tasks and determining the domain-general or domain-specific manner in which aging affects the neural substrates associated with RI. In an fMRI experiment, young and older participants performed a similarity judgment task with phonological, semantic, or visual interference buildup. For both age groups, domain-specific RI effects were observed at the univariate level, with increased involvement in the phonological domain of the right angular gyrus and the right lingual gyrus, in the semantic domain of the bilateral inferior frontal gyrus, the bilateral superior parietal and angular gyri and the left middle temporal gyrus, and in the visual domain of the middle/superior frontal gyri and occipital gyri. At the multivariate level, although RI effects could be decoded from neural patterns in the bilateral inferior frontal gyrus for all domains and age groups, between-domain prediction of RI conditions was associated with Bayesian evidence for the null hypothesis. This study supports the domain specificity of neural substrates associated with RI while stressing its age independency.

认知控制是针对特定领域的,还是针对一般领域的,这个问题在认知和神经水平上都存在广泛争议。本研究通过使用严格匹配的任务,研究了与语音、语义和视觉领域抗干扰(RI)相关的神经基质,并确定了衰老影响与抗干扰(RI)相关的神经基质的领域通用性或领域特异性方式。在一项 fMRI 实验中,年轻和年长的参与者分别完成了一项带有语音、语义或视觉干扰的相似性判断任务。在单变量水平上,两个年龄组都观察到了特定领域的RI效应,在语音领域,右侧角回和右侧舌回的参与度增加;在语义领域,双侧额叶下回、双侧顶叶上回、角回和左侧颞中回的参与度增加;在视觉领域,额叶中/上回和枕回的参与度增加。在多变量水平上,虽然在所有领域和年龄组中,RI效应都可以从双侧额叶下回的神经模式中解码,但对RI条件的领域间预测与贝叶斯证据的零假设有关。本研究支持与 RI 相关的神经基质的领域特异性,同时强调了其与年龄的独立性。
{"title":"The Neural Specificity of Interference Resolution in Phonological, Semantic, and Visual Domains at Different Ages.","authors":"Coline Grégoire, Lucie Attout, Christophe Phillips, Lucas Rifon, Louis Hody, Steve Majerus","doi":"10.1162/jocn_a_02260","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn_a_02260","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The question of whether cognitive control is specific to certain domains or domain-general remains an extensively debated question at both cognitive and neural levels. This study examined the neural substrates associated with resistance to interference (RI) in phonological, semantic, and visual domains by using strictly matched tasks and determining the domain-general or domain-specific manner in which aging affects the neural substrates associated with RI. In an fMRI experiment, young and older participants performed a similarity judgment task with phonological, semantic, or visual interference buildup. For both age groups, domain-specific RI effects were observed at the univariate level, with increased involvement in the phonological domain of the right angular gyrus and the right lingual gyrus, in the semantic domain of the bilateral inferior frontal gyrus, the bilateral superior parietal and angular gyri and the left middle temporal gyrus, and in the visual domain of the middle/superior frontal gyri and occipital gyri. At the multivariate level, although RI effects could be decoded from neural patterns in the bilateral inferior frontal gyrus for all domains and age groups, between-domain prediction of RI conditions was associated with Bayesian evidence for the null hypothesis. This study supports the domain specificity of neural substrates associated with RI while stressing its age independency.</p>","PeriodicalId":51081,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience","volume":" ","pages":"1-27"},"PeriodicalIF":3.1,"publicationDate":"2024-10-18","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142480184","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Announcing the Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience Discussion Forum. 认知神经科学杂志》讨论论坛揭晓。
IF 3.1 3区 医学 Q2 NEUROSCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-10-18 DOI: 10.1162/jocn_e_00259
Bradley R Postle
{"title":"Announcing the Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience Discussion Forum.","authors":"Bradley R Postle","doi":"10.1162/jocn_e_00259","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn_e_00259","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":51081,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience","volume":" ","pages":"1"},"PeriodicalIF":3.1,"publicationDate":"2024-10-18","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142480179","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Rhythm-based Temporal Expectations: Unique Contributions of Predictability and Periodicity. 基于节奏的时间预期:可预测性和周期性的独特贡献。
IF 3.1 3区 医学 Q2 NEUROSCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-10-18 DOI: 10.1162/jocn_a_02261
Sandra Solli, Anne Danielsen, Sabine Leske, Alejandro O Blenkmann, Keith B Doelling, Anne-Kristin Solbakk, Tor Endestad

Anticipating events and focusing attention accordingly are crucial for navigating our dynamic environment. Rhythmic patterns of sensory input offer valuable cues for temporal expectations and facilitate perceptual processing. Rhythm-based temporal expectations may rely on oscillatory entrainment, where neural activity and perceptual sensitivity synchronize with periodic stimuli. However, whether entrainment models can account for aperiodic predictable rhythms remains unclear. Our study aimed to delineate the distinct roles of predictability and periodicity in rhythm-based expectations. Participants performed a pitch-identification task preceded by periodic predictable, aperiodic predictable, or aperiodic unpredictable temporal sequences. By manipulating the temporal position of the target sound, we observed how auditory perceptual performance was modulated by the target position's relative phase relationship to the preceding sequences. Results revealed a significant performance advantage for predictable sequences, both periodic and aperiodic, compared with unpredictable ones. However, only the periodic sequence induced an entrained modulation pattern, with performance peaking in synchrony with the inherent sequence continuation. Event-related brain potentials corroborated these findings. The target-evoked P3b, possibly a neural marker of attention allocation, mirrored the behavioral performance patterns. This supports our hypothesis that temporal attention guided by rhythm-based expectations modulates perceptual performance. Furthermore, the predictive sequences were associated with enhanced target-preceding negativity (akin to the contingent negative variation), indicating enhanced target preparation. The periodic-specific modulation likely reflects more precise temporal expectations, potentially involving neural entrainment and/or more focused attention. Our findings suggest that predictability and periodicity influence perception through distinct mechanisms.

预测事件并相应地集中注意力,对我们在动态环境中的导航至关重要。感觉输入的节奏模式为时间预期提供了宝贵的线索,并促进了知觉处理。基于节奏的时间预期可能依赖于振荡夹带,即神经活动和知觉敏感度与周期性刺激同步。然而,夹带模型能否解释非周期性的可预测节奏仍不清楚。我们的研究旨在明确可预测性和周期性在基于节奏的预期中的不同作用。参与者在周期性可预测、非周期性可预测或非周期性不可预测的时间序列前进行音高识别任务。通过操纵目标声音的时间位置,我们观察了目标位置与前面序列的相对相位关系如何调节听觉知觉表现。结果显示,与不可预测序列相比,可预测序列(包括周期性和非周期性序列)具有明显的性能优势。然而,只有周期性序列能诱导出内含的调制模式,其表现与固有序列的延续同步达到峰值。事件相关脑电位证实了这些发现。目标诱发的 P3b 可能是注意力分配的神经标记,它反映了行为表现模式。这支持了我们的假设,即由基于节奏的预期所引导的时间注意力会调节知觉表现。此外,预测序列与目标前否定性增强(类似于或然否定变异)相关,表明目标准备增强。周期性特异性调节可能反映了更精确的时间预期,可能涉及神经诱导和/或更集中的注意力。我们的研究结果表明,可预测性和周期性通过不同的机制影响知觉。
{"title":"Rhythm-based Temporal Expectations: Unique Contributions of Predictability and Periodicity.","authors":"Sandra Solli, Anne Danielsen, Sabine Leske, Alejandro O Blenkmann, Keith B Doelling, Anne-Kristin Solbakk, Tor Endestad","doi":"10.1162/jocn_a_02261","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn_a_02261","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Anticipating events and focusing attention accordingly are crucial for navigating our dynamic environment. Rhythmic patterns of sensory input offer valuable cues for temporal expectations and facilitate perceptual processing. Rhythm-based temporal expectations may rely on oscillatory entrainment, where neural activity and perceptual sensitivity synchronize with periodic stimuli. However, whether entrainment models can account for aperiodic predictable rhythms remains unclear. Our study aimed to delineate the distinct roles of predictability and periodicity in rhythm-based expectations. Participants performed a pitch-identification task preceded by periodic predictable, aperiodic predictable, or aperiodic unpredictable temporal sequences. By manipulating the temporal position of the target sound, we observed how auditory perceptual performance was modulated by the target position's relative phase relationship to the preceding sequences. Results revealed a significant performance advantage for predictable sequences, both periodic and aperiodic, compared with unpredictable ones. However, only the periodic sequence induced an entrained modulation pattern, with performance peaking in synchrony with the inherent sequence continuation. Event-related brain potentials corroborated these findings. The target-evoked P3b, possibly a neural marker of attention allocation, mirrored the behavioral performance patterns. This supports our hypothesis that temporal attention guided by rhythm-based expectations modulates perceptual performance. Furthermore, the predictive sequences were associated with enhanced target-preceding negativity (akin to the contingent negative variation), indicating enhanced target preparation. The periodic-specific modulation likely reflects more precise temporal expectations, potentially involving neural entrainment and/or more focused attention. Our findings suggest that predictability and periodicity influence perception through distinct mechanisms.</p>","PeriodicalId":51081,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience","volume":" ","pages":"1-27"},"PeriodicalIF":3.1,"publicationDate":"2024-10-18","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142480182","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Relevance of Prosodic Focus and Lexical Stress for Discourse Comprehension in Turkish: Evidence from Psychometric and Electrophysiological Data. 语法重点和词汇重音与土耳其语话语理解的相关性:来自心理测量和电生理数据的证据。
IF 3.1 3区 医学 Q2 NEUROSCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-10-18 DOI: 10.1162/jocn_a_02262
Hatice Zora, Barış Kabak, Peter Hagoort

Prosody underpins various linguistic domains ranging from semantics and syntax to discourse. For instance, prosodic information in the form of lexical stress modifies meanings and, as such, syntactic contexts of words as in Turkish kaz-má "pickaxe" (noun) versus káz-ma "do not dig" (imperative). Likewise, prosody indicates the focused constituent of an utterance as the noun phrase filling the wh-spot in a dialogue like What did you eat? I ate----. In the present study, we investigated the relevance of such prosodic variations for discourse comprehension in Turkish. We aimed at answering how lexical stress and prosodic focus mismatches on critical noun phrases-resulting in grammatical anomalies involving both semantics and syntax and discourse-level anomalies, respectively-affect the perceived correctness of an answer to a question in a given context. To that end, 80 native speakers of Turkish, 40 participating in a psychometric experiment and 40 participating in an EEG experiment, were asked to judge the acceptability of prosodic mismatches that occur either separately or concurrently. Psychometric results indicated that lexical stress mismatch led to a lower correctness score than prosodic focus mismatch, and combined mismatch received the lowest score. Consistent with the psychometric data, EEG results revealed an N400 effect to combined mismatch, and this effect was followed by a P600 response to lexical stress mismatch. Conjointly, these results suggest that every source of prosodic information is immediately available and codetermines the interpretation of an utterance; however, semantically and syntactically relevant lexical stress information is assigned more significance by the language comprehension system compared with prosodic focus information.

前音支撑着从语义学、句法到话语的各个语言领域。例如,以词汇重音形式出现的前音信息会改变词义,从而改变词的句法上下文,如土耳其语中的 kaz-má "镐"(名词)与 káz-ma "不要挖"(祈使句)。同样,在对话中,前音表示语篇中的重点成分,如名词短语填wh-spot,如What did you eat?我吃了----。在本研究中,我们探讨了这种拟声变化与土耳其语话语理解的相关性。我们旨在回答关键名词短语的词性重音和前音重点不匹配--分别导致语法异常(涉及语义和句法)和话语层面的异常--是如何影响特定语境中问题答案的感知正确性的。为此,80 位以土耳其语为母语的人,其中 40 人参加了心理测量实验,40 人参加了脑电图实验,被要求判断单独或同时出现的前音不匹配的可接受性。心理测量结果表明,词性重音错配导致的正确性得分低于前音重心错配,而组合错配得到的得分最低。与心理测量数据一致的是,脑电图结果显示了对组合错配的 N400 效应,这种效应之后是对词汇重音错配的 P600 反应。这些结果表明,每一种前音信息源都是即时可用的,并共同决定了对语篇的解释;然而,与前音焦点信息相比,语言理解系统赋予语义和句法相关的词汇重音信息更重要的意义。
{"title":"Relevance of Prosodic Focus and Lexical Stress for Discourse Comprehension in Turkish: Evidence from Psychometric and Electrophysiological Data.","authors":"Hatice Zora, Barış Kabak, Peter Hagoort","doi":"10.1162/jocn_a_02262","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn_a_02262","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Prosody underpins various linguistic domains ranging from semantics and syntax to discourse. For instance, prosodic information in the form of lexical stress modifies meanings and, as such, syntactic contexts of words as in Turkish kaz-má \"pickaxe\" (noun) versus káz-ma \"do not dig\" (imperative). Likewise, prosody indicates the focused constituent of an utterance as the noun phrase filling the wh-spot in a dialogue like What did you eat? I ate----. In the present study, we investigated the relevance of such prosodic variations for discourse comprehension in Turkish. We aimed at answering how lexical stress and prosodic focus mismatches on critical noun phrases-resulting in grammatical anomalies involving both semantics and syntax and discourse-level anomalies, respectively-affect the perceived correctness of an answer to a question in a given context. To that end, 80 native speakers of Turkish, 40 participating in a psychometric experiment and 40 participating in an EEG experiment, were asked to judge the acceptability of prosodic mismatches that occur either separately or concurrently. Psychometric results indicated that lexical stress mismatch led to a lower correctness score than prosodic focus mismatch, and combined mismatch received the lowest score. Consistent with the psychometric data, EEG results revealed an N400 effect to combined mismatch, and this effect was followed by a P600 response to lexical stress mismatch. Conjointly, these results suggest that every source of prosodic information is immediately available and codetermines the interpretation of an utterance; however, semantically and syntactically relevant lexical stress information is assigned more significance by the language comprehension system compared with prosodic focus information.</p>","PeriodicalId":51081,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience","volume":" ","pages":"1-44"},"PeriodicalIF":3.1,"publicationDate":"2024-10-18","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142480181","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
EEG Responses to the Number of Objects in Partially Occluded and Uncovered Scenes. 部分遮挡和未遮挡场景中物体数量的脑电图反应
IF 3.1 3区 医学 Q2 NEUROSCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-10-18 DOI: 10.1162/jocn_a_02264
Cemre Baykan, Alexander C Schütz

Perceptual completion is ubiquitous when estimating properties such as the shape, size, or number of objects in partially occluded scenes. Behavioral experiments showed that the number of hidden objects is underestimated in partially occluded scenes compared with an estimation based on the density of visible objects and the amount of occlusion. It is still unknown at which processing level this (under)estimation of the number of hidden objects occurs. We studied this question using a passive viewing task in which observers viewed a game board that was initially partially occluded and later was uncovered to reveal its hidden parts. We simultaneously measured the electroencephalographic responses to the partially occluded board presentation and its uncovering. We hypothesized that if the underestimation is a result of early sensory processing, it would be observed in the activities of P1 and N1, whereas if it is because of higher level processes such as expectancy, it would be reflected in P3 activities. Our data showed that P1 amplitude increased with numerosity in both occluded and uncovered states, indicating a link between P1 and simple stimulus features. The N1 amplitude was highest when both the initially visible and uncovered areas of the board were completely filled with game pieces, suggesting that the N1 component is sensitive to the overall Gestalt. Finally, we observed that P3 activity was reduced when the density of game pieces in the uncovered parts matched the initially visible parts, implying a relationship between the P3 component and expectation mismatch. Overall, our results suggest that inferences about the number of hidden items are reflected in high-level processing.

在估计部分遮挡场景中物体的形状、大小或数量等属性时,感知完备性无处不在。行为实验表明,与根据可见物体的密度和遮挡程度进行的估计相比,部分遮挡场景中隐藏物体的数量被低估了。这种对隐藏物体数量的(低估)发生在哪个处理层次,目前仍是未知数。我们通过被动观看任务来研究这个问题,在这个任务中,观察者观看的是一个游戏板,游戏板最初是部分遮挡的,后来被揭开,露出了隐藏的部分。我们同时测量了观察者对部分遮挡的棋盘和揭开棋盘时的脑电反应。我们假设,如果低估是早期感官加工的结果,则会在 P1 和 N1 活动中观察到,而如果低估是由于更高层次的过程(如预期)造成的,则会在 P3 活动中反映出来。我们的数据显示,在遮蔽和不遮蔽状态下,P1 振幅都会随着数字的增加而增加,这表明 P1 与简单刺激特征之间存在联系。当棋盘上最初可见的区域和未被遮盖的区域都完全被棋子填满时,N1振幅最高,这表明N1成分对整体格式塔很敏感。最后,我们观察到,当无遮挡部分的棋子密度与最初可见部分的棋子密度一致时,P3 活动减少,这意味着 P3 成分与期望不匹配之间存在关系。总之,我们的研究结果表明,对隐藏物品数量的推断反映在高级处理过程中。
{"title":"EEG Responses to the Number of Objects in Partially Occluded and Uncovered Scenes.","authors":"Cemre Baykan, Alexander C Schütz","doi":"10.1162/jocn_a_02264","DOIUrl":"10.1162/jocn_a_02264","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Perceptual completion is ubiquitous when estimating properties such as the shape, size, or number of objects in partially occluded scenes. Behavioral experiments showed that the number of hidden objects is underestimated in partially occluded scenes compared with an estimation based on the density of visible objects and the amount of occlusion. It is still unknown at which processing level this (under)estimation of the number of hidden objects occurs. We studied this question using a passive viewing task in which observers viewed a game board that was initially partially occluded and later was uncovered to reveal its hidden parts. We simultaneously measured the electroencephalographic responses to the partially occluded board presentation and its uncovering. We hypothesized that if the underestimation is a result of early sensory processing, it would be observed in the activities of P1 and N1, whereas if it is because of higher level processes such as expectancy, it would be reflected in P3 activities. Our data showed that P1 amplitude increased with numerosity in both occluded and uncovered states, indicating a link between P1 and simple stimulus features. The N1 amplitude was highest when both the initially visible and uncovered areas of the board were completely filled with game pieces, suggesting that the N1 component is sensitive to the overall Gestalt. Finally, we observed that P3 activity was reduced when the density of game pieces in the uncovered parts matched the initially visible parts, implying a relationship between the P3 component and expectation mismatch. Overall, our results suggest that inferences about the number of hidden items are reflected in high-level processing.</p>","PeriodicalId":51081,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience","volume":" ","pages":"1-12"},"PeriodicalIF":3.1,"publicationDate":"2024-10-18","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142480180","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Locus Coeruleus Is Associated with Higher Openness to Experience and IQ: Implications for the Noradrenergic System for Novelty Seeking in Daily Life. 鳞状上皮细胞与较高的经验开放度和智商有关:诺拉肾上腺素能系统对日常生活中寻求新奇事物的影响
IF 3.1 3区 医学 Q2 NEUROSCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-10-13 DOI: 10.1162/jocn_a_02245
Emanuele R G Plini, Ian H Robertson, Méadhbh B Brosnan, Paul M Dockree

Novelty exposure and the upregulation of the noradrenergic (NA) system have been suggested as crucial for developing cognitive reserve and resilience against neurodegeneration. Openness to experience (OE), a personality trait associated with interest in novel experiences, may play a key role in facilitating this process. High-OE individuals tend to be more curious and encounter a wider range of novel stimuli throughout their lifespan. To investigate the relationship between OE and the main core of the NA system, the locus coeruleus (LC), as well as its potential mediation of IQ-a measure of cognitive reserve-MRI structural analyses were conducted on 135 healthy young adults. Compared with other neuromodulators' seeds, such as dorsal and median raphe-5-HT, ventral tegmental area-DA-, and nucleus basalis of Meynert-Ach-, the results indicated that higher LC signal intensity correlated with greater OE and IQ. Furthermore, mediation analyses revealed that only the LC played a mediating role between OE and IQ. These findings shed light on the neurobiology of personality and emphasize the importance of LC-NA system integrity in a novelty-seeking behavior. They provide a psychobiological explanation for how OE expression can contribute to the maintenance of the NA system, enhancing cognitive reserve and resilience against neurodegeneration.

新奇体验和去甲肾上腺素能(NA)系统的上调被认为是发展认知储备和抵御神经变性的关键。体验开放性(OE)是一种与对新奇体验的兴趣相关的人格特质,它可能在促进这一过程中发挥关键作用。经验开放度高的人往往好奇心更强,在一生中会遇到更多的新奇刺激。为了研究OE与NA系统的主要核心--小脑定位点(LC)--之间的关系,以及它对IQ--认知储备的潜在中介作用,研究人员对135名健康的年轻人进行了核磁共振成像结构分析。结果表明,与其他神经调节因子的种子相比,如背侧和正中油菜花-5-HT、腹侧被盖区-DA-和麦氏基底核-Ach-,更高的LC信号强度与更高的OE和IQ相关。此外,中介分析显示,只有LC在OE和IQ之间起中介作用。这些发现揭示了人格的神经生物学,并强调了LC-NA系统的完整性在寻求新奇行为中的重要性。它们提供了一种心理生物学解释,说明OE的表达如何有助于维持NA系统,增强认知储备和对神经变性的复原力。
{"title":"Locus Coeruleus Is Associated with Higher Openness to Experience and IQ: Implications for the Noradrenergic System for Novelty Seeking in Daily Life.","authors":"Emanuele R G Plini, Ian H Robertson, Méadhbh B Brosnan, Paul M Dockree","doi":"10.1162/jocn_a_02245","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn_a_02245","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Novelty exposure and the upregulation of the noradrenergic (NA) system have been suggested as crucial for developing cognitive reserve and resilience against neurodegeneration. Openness to experience (OE), a personality trait associated with interest in novel experiences, may play a key role in facilitating this process. High-OE individuals tend to be more curious and encounter a wider range of novel stimuli throughout their lifespan. To investigate the relationship between OE and the main core of the NA system, the locus coeruleus (LC), as well as its potential mediation of IQ-a measure of cognitive reserve-MRI structural analyses were conducted on 135 healthy young adults. Compared with other neuromodulators' seeds, such as dorsal and median raphe-5-HT, ventral tegmental area-DA-, and nucleus basalis of Meynert-Ach-, the results indicated that higher LC signal intensity correlated with greater OE and IQ. Furthermore, mediation analyses revealed that only the LC played a mediating role between OE and IQ. These findings shed light on the neurobiology of personality and emphasize the importance of LC-NA system integrity in a novelty-seeking behavior. They provide a psychobiological explanation for how OE expression can contribute to the maintenance of the NA system, enhancing cognitive reserve and resilience against neurodegeneration.</p>","PeriodicalId":51081,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience","volume":" ","pages":"1-24"},"PeriodicalIF":3.1,"publicationDate":"2024-10-13","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142512538","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Semantic Context Effects in Picture and Sound Naming: Evidence from Event-related Potentials and Pupillometric Data. 图像和声音命名中的语义语境效应:来自事件相关电位和瞳孔测量数据的证据
IF 3.1 3区 医学 Q2 NEUROSCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-10-04 DOI: 10.1162/jocn_a_02255
Magdalena Gruner, Andreas Widmann, Stefan Wöhner, Erich Schröger, Jörg D Jescheniak

When a picture is repeatedly named in the context of semantically related pictures, (homogeneous context) responses are slower than when the picture is repeatedly named in the context of unrelated pictures (heterogeneous context). This semantic interference effect in blocked-cyclic naming plays an important role in devising theories of word production. Wöhner, Mädebach, and Jescheniak [2021; Wöhner, S., Mädebach, A., & Jescheniak, J. D. Naming pictures and sounds: Stimulus type affects semantic context effects. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 47, 716-730, 2021] have shown that the effect is substantially larger when participants name environmental sounds than when they name pictures. We investigated possible reasons for this difference, using EEG and pupillometry. The behavioral data replicated Wöhner and colleagues. ERPs were more positive in the homogeneous compared with the heterogeneous context over central electrode locations between 140-180 msec and 250-350 msec for picture naming and between 250 and 350 msec for sound naming, presumably reflecting semantic interference during semantic and lexical processing. The later component was of similar size for pictures and sounds. ERPs were more negative in the homogeneous compared with the heterogeneous context over frontal electrode locations between 400 and 600 msec only for sounds. The pupillometric data showed a stronger pupil dilation in the homogeneous compared with the heterogeneous context only for sounds. The amplitudes of the late ERP negativity and pupil dilation predicted naming latencies for sounds in the homogeneous context. The latency of the effects indicates that the difference in semantic interference between picture and sound naming arises at later, presumably postlexical processing stages closer to articulation. We suggest that the processing of the auditory stimuli interferes with phonological response preparation and self-monitoring, leading to enhanced semantic interference.

当一幅图片在语义相关的图片语境中被重复命名时,(同质语境)反应比该图片在不相关的图片语境中被重复命名时(异质语境)要慢。这种阻塞循环命名中的语义干扰效应在设计单词生成理论时发挥了重要作用。Wöhner、Mädebach 和 Jescheniak [2021; Wöhner, S., Mädebach, A., & Jescheniak, J. D. Naming pictures and sounds:刺激类型影响语义语境效应。实验心理学杂志:人类感知与表现》,47, 716-730, 2021 年]显示,当参与者说出环境声音时,其效果远远大于说出图片时。我们使用脑电图和瞳孔测量法研究了造成这种差异的可能原因。行为数据重复了 Wöhner 及其同事的研究。与异质环境相比,同质环境下的ERP在图片命名的140-180毫秒和250-350毫秒之间,以及声音命名的250-350毫秒之间的中心电极位置呈阳性,这可能反映了语义和词汇处理过程中的语义干扰。图片和声音的后一分量大小相似。在同质情境下,ERPs 在额叶电极位置的负值比在异质情境下的负值要大,仅在声音命名的 400 到 600 毫秒之间。瞳孔测量数据显示,同质背景下的瞳孔放大比异质背景下的瞳孔放大更强(仅针对声音)。晚期ERP负性和瞳孔放大的幅度可以预测同质情境中声音的命名潜伏期。这些效应的潜伏期表明,图片和声音命名之间的语义干扰差异出现在后期,也就是更接近发音的词汇后处理阶段。我们认为,对听觉刺激的处理会干扰语音反应准备和自我监控,从而导致语义干扰增强。
{"title":"Semantic Context Effects in Picture and Sound Naming: Evidence from Event-related Potentials and Pupillometric Data.","authors":"Magdalena Gruner, Andreas Widmann, Stefan Wöhner, Erich Schröger, Jörg D Jescheniak","doi":"10.1162/jocn_a_02255","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn_a_02255","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>When a picture is repeatedly named in the context of semantically related pictures, (homogeneous context) responses are slower than when the picture is repeatedly named in the context of unrelated pictures (heterogeneous context). This semantic interference effect in blocked-cyclic naming plays an important role in devising theories of word production. Wöhner, Mädebach, and Jescheniak [2021; Wöhner, S., Mädebach, A., & Jescheniak, J. D. Naming pictures and sounds: Stimulus type affects semantic context effects. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 47, 716-730, 2021] have shown that the effect is substantially larger when participants name environmental sounds than when they name pictures. We investigated possible reasons for this difference, using EEG and pupillometry. The behavioral data replicated Wöhner and colleagues. ERPs were more positive in the homogeneous compared with the heterogeneous context over central electrode locations between 140-180 msec and 250-350 msec for picture naming and between 250 and 350 msec for sound naming, presumably reflecting semantic interference during semantic and lexical processing. The later component was of similar size for pictures and sounds. ERPs were more negative in the homogeneous compared with the heterogeneous context over frontal electrode locations between 400 and 600 msec only for sounds. The pupillometric data showed a stronger pupil dilation in the homogeneous compared with the heterogeneous context only for sounds. The amplitudes of the late ERP negativity and pupil dilation predicted naming latencies for sounds in the homogeneous context. The latency of the effects indicates that the difference in semantic interference between picture and sound naming arises at later, presumably postlexical processing stages closer to articulation. We suggest that the processing of the auditory stimuli interferes with phonological response preparation and self-monitoring, leading to enhanced semantic interference.</p>","PeriodicalId":51081,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience","volume":" ","pages":"1-21"},"PeriodicalIF":3.1,"publicationDate":"2024-10-04","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142395032","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Electrophysiological Correlates of Visual Memory Search. 视觉记忆搜索的电生理相关性
IF 3.1 3区 医学 Q2 NEUROSCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-10-04 DOI: 10.1162/jocn_a_02256
Lauren H Williams, Iris Wiegand, Mark Lavelle, Jeremy M Wolfe, Keisuke Fukuda, Marius V Peelen, Trafton Drew

In everyday life, we frequently engage in 'hybrid' visual and memory search, where we look for multiple items stored in memory (e.g., a mental shopping list) in our visual environment. Across three experiments, we used event-related potentials to better understand the contributions of visual working memory (VWM) and long-term memory (LTM) during the memory search component of hybrid search. Experiments 1 and 2 demonstrated that the FN400 - an index of LTM recognition - and the CDA -an index of VWM load - increased with memory set size (target load), suggesting that both VWM and LTM are involved in memory search, even when target load exceeds capacity limitations of VWM. In Experiment 3, we used these electrophysiological indices to test how categorical similarity of targets and distractors affects memory search. The CDA and FN400 were modulated by memory set size only if items resembled targets. This suggests that dissimilar distractor items can be rejected before eliciting a memory search. Together, our findings demonstrate the interplay of VWM and LTM processes during memory search for multiple targets.

在日常生活中,我们经常会进行 "混合 "视觉和记忆搜索,即在视觉环境中寻找存储在记忆中的多个项目(如心理购物清单)。在三项实验中,我们利用事件相关电位来更好地了解视觉工作记忆(VWM)和长时程记忆(LTM)在混合搜索的记忆搜索部分中的贡献。实验 1 和 2 表明,作为 LTM 识别指数的 FN400 和作为 VWM 负荷指数的 CDA 会随着记忆集大小(目标负荷)的增加而增加,这表明 VWM 和 LTM 都参与了记忆搜索,即使目标负荷超过了 VWM 的容量限制。在实验 3 中,我们使用这些电生理指标来测试目标和分心物的分类相似性如何影响记忆搜索。只有当项目与目标相似时,CDA 和 FN400 才会受到记忆集大小的调节。这表明,在引起记忆搜索之前,不相似的分心项会被剔除。总之,我们的研究结果证明了在对多个目标进行记忆搜索时,VWM 和 LTM 过程之间的相互作用。
{"title":"Electrophysiological Correlates of Visual Memory Search.","authors":"Lauren H Williams, Iris Wiegand, Mark Lavelle, Jeremy M Wolfe, Keisuke Fukuda, Marius V Peelen, Trafton Drew","doi":"10.1162/jocn_a_02256","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn_a_02256","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>In everyday life, we frequently engage in 'hybrid' visual and memory search, where we look for multiple items stored in memory (e.g., a mental shopping list) in our visual environment. Across three experiments, we used event-related potentials to better understand the contributions of visual working memory (VWM) and long-term memory (LTM) during the memory search component of hybrid search. Experiments 1 and 2 demonstrated that the FN400 - an index of LTM recognition - and the CDA -an index of VWM load - increased with memory set size (target load), suggesting that both VWM and LTM are involved in memory search, even when target load exceeds capacity limitations of VWM. In Experiment 3, we used these electrophysiological indices to test how categorical similarity of targets and distractors affects memory search. The CDA and FN400 were modulated by memory set size only if items resembled targets. This suggests that dissimilar distractor items can be rejected before eliciting a memory search. Together, our findings demonstrate the interplay of VWM and LTM processes during memory search for multiple targets.</p>","PeriodicalId":51081,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience","volume":" ","pages":"1-23"},"PeriodicalIF":3.1,"publicationDate":"2024-10-04","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142395030","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1