首页 > 最新文献

British Journal of Developmental Psychology最新文献

英文 中文
Student–teacher relationships and sense of academic futility: Longitudinal associations among early adolescents of immigrant and non-immigrant background 师生关系与学业失败感:移民和非移民背景青少年的纵向关系。
IF 2.6 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-06-18 DOI: 10.1111/bjdp.12504
Beatrice Bobba, Takuya Yanagida, Maria Wiertsema, Diana Miconi, Adebunmi Oyekola, Ifunanya Chukwueke, Sevgi Bayram Özdemir

Sense of academic futility entails feelings of having no control over ones' educational success. Although mounting evidence points to its negative consequences for students' educational outcomes, less is known about its socio-contextual antecedents. Relatedly, the current study explored how fair and supportive relationships with teachers are related to the sense of academic futility and if class belonging mediates this link in a sample of adolescents with immigrant and non-immigrant backgrounds. A total of 1065 seventh-grade students (Mage = 13.12; SD = 0.42; 45% girls) from 55 classrooms completed questionnaires at two time points 1 year apart. Results of multilevel analyses indicated that fair and supportive relationships with teachers contributed to decreases in sense of academic futility at the individual but not at the classroom level. No mediation or moderation effects emerged. These findings highlight the crucial role of democratic student–teacher relationships in supporting the positive school adjustment of all students in increasingly multicultural societies.

学业无用感是指对自己的教育成功无法控制的感觉。尽管越来越多的证据表明,学业无用感会对学生的教育成果产生负面影响,但人们对其社会背景的前因却知之甚少。因此,本研究以具有移民和非移民背景的青少年为样本,探讨了与教师之间的公平和支持性关系与学业无用感之间的关系,以及阶级归属感是否会调节这种关系。来自 55 个班级的 1065 名七年级学生(Mage = 13.12;SD = 0.42;45% 为女生)在相隔一年的两个时间点填写了调查问卷。多层次分析的结果表明,公平和支持性的师生关系有助于降低个人的学业无用感,但对班级的影响不大。没有出现中介或调节效应。这些研究结果凸显了在日益多元文化的社会中,民主的师生关系在支持所有学生积极适应学校生活方面的重要作用。
{"title":"Student–teacher relationships and sense of academic futility: Longitudinal associations among early adolescents of immigrant and non-immigrant background","authors":"Beatrice Bobba,&nbsp;Takuya Yanagida,&nbsp;Maria Wiertsema,&nbsp;Diana Miconi,&nbsp;Adebunmi Oyekola,&nbsp;Ifunanya Chukwueke,&nbsp;Sevgi Bayram Özdemir","doi":"10.1111/bjdp.12504","DOIUrl":"10.1111/bjdp.12504","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Sense of academic futility entails feelings of having no control over ones' educational success. Although mounting evidence points to its negative consequences for students' educational outcomes, less is known about its socio-contextual antecedents. Relatedly, the current study explored how fair and supportive relationships with teachers are related to the sense of academic futility and if class belonging mediates this link in a sample of adolescents with immigrant and non-immigrant backgrounds. A total of 1065 seventh-grade students (<i>M</i><sub>age</sub> = 13.12; <i>SD</i> = 0.42; 45% girls) from 55 classrooms completed questionnaires at two time points 1 year apart. Results of multilevel analyses indicated that fair and supportive relationships with teachers contributed to decreases in sense of academic futility at the individual but not at the classroom level. No mediation or moderation effects emerged. These findings highlight the crucial role of democratic student–teacher relationships in supporting the positive school adjustment of all students in increasingly multicultural societies.</p>","PeriodicalId":51418,"journal":{"name":"British Journal of Developmental Psychology","volume":"42 4","pages":"439-460"},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2024-06-18","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141421837","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The influence of semantics on long-term visual memory capacity in children and adults 语义对儿童和成人长期视觉记忆能力的影响。
IF 2.6 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-06-10 DOI: 10.1111/bjdp.12498
Priti Gupta, Marin Vogelsang, Lukas Vogelsang, Pragya Shah, Sharon Gilad-Gutnick, Pawan Sinha

Human visual memory capacity has a rapid developmental progression. Here we examine whether image semantics modulate this progression. We assessed the performance of children (6–14 years) and young adults (19–36 years) on a visual memory task using real-world (or meaningful) as well as abstract image sets, which were matched in low-level image attributes. For real images, we find comparable performance across the two age groups, consistent with previously reported results. However, for abstract images, we find a clear age-related difference indicating greater reliance of children's memory processes on semantics, suggesting that strategies for encoding abstract patterns keep improving even into late childhood. We complemented these studies with computational experiments designed to examine the role of increasing experience with real-world images on real and abstract image encoding, to examine whether the observed age-related differences, as well as the general privilege of real over abstract images, can emerge directly through experience with meaningful images. Our results provide support for this possibility and set the stage for a finer-grained investigation of the timeline along which children's memory capacity for abstract images reaches adult levels.

人类的视觉记忆能力发展迅速。在此,我们研究了图像语义是否会调节这种进步。我们评估了儿童(6-14 岁)和年轻人(19-36 岁)在使用真实世界(或有意义的)和抽象图像集进行视觉记忆任务时的表现,这些图像集的低级图像属性是匹配的。对于真实图像,我们发现两个年龄组的表现相当,这与之前报道的结果一致。然而,对于抽象图像,我们发现了明显的年龄差异,表明儿童的记忆过程更依赖于语义,这表明即使到了儿童后期,抽象模式的编码策略仍在不断改进。作为对这些研究的补充,我们还进行了计算实验,旨在检验真实世界图像经验的增加对真实和抽象图像编码的作用,以检验观察到的与年龄有关的差异,以及真实图像相对于抽象图像的普遍优势,是否可以通过有意义图像的经验直接产生。我们的研究结果为这种可能性提供了支持,并为更细致地研究儿童的抽象图像记忆能力达到成人水平的时间线奠定了基础。
{"title":"The influence of semantics on long-term visual memory capacity in children and adults","authors":"Priti Gupta,&nbsp;Marin Vogelsang,&nbsp;Lukas Vogelsang,&nbsp;Pragya Shah,&nbsp;Sharon Gilad-Gutnick,&nbsp;Pawan Sinha","doi":"10.1111/bjdp.12498","DOIUrl":"10.1111/bjdp.12498","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Human visual memory capacity has a rapid developmental progression. Here we examine whether image semantics modulate this progression. We assessed the performance of children (6–14 years) and young adults (19–36 years) on a visual memory task using real-world (or meaningful) as well as abstract image sets, which were matched in low-level image attributes. For real images, we find comparable performance across the two age groups, consistent with previously reported results. However, for abstract images, we find a clear age-related difference indicating greater reliance of children's memory processes on semantics, suggesting that strategies for encoding abstract patterns keep improving even into late childhood. We complemented these studies with computational experiments designed to examine the role of increasing experience with real-world images on real and abstract image encoding, to examine whether the observed age-related differences, as well as the general privilege of real over abstract images, can emerge directly through experience with meaningful images. Our results provide support for this possibility and set the stage for a finer-grained investigation of the timeline along which children's memory capacity for abstract images reaches adult levels.</p>","PeriodicalId":51418,"journal":{"name":"British Journal of Developmental Psychology","volume":"42 3","pages":"392-408"},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2024-06-10","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141301998","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Parents' mental state language and child gender: A scoping review of developmental evidence. 父母的心理状态语言与儿童性别:对发展证据的范围审查。
IF 2.3 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-06-06 DOI: 10.1111/bjdp.12502
Callyn Farrell, Ellen Sanderson, Aisling Mulvihill, Michael Thai, Virginia Slaughter

Mental state language (MSL) is an important mechanism through which children learn about their social world and place within it. Previous research has suggested that parents may use MSL differently towards children based on their child's gender. However, findings are inconsistent. This scoping review explores the consistency of reported differences in parents' MSL use as a function of children's gender while exploring the methodological variables that may provide insights into these differences. Based on a review of the 27 studies included, 12 found a significant relationship between child gender and parents' MSL, while the remaining did not. The included studies used a range of methodological approaches to elicit MSL. This scoping review allows researchers and practitioners to reflect upon assumptions regarding the associations between child gender and parents' MSL. Further, we call for the use of diverse and informed approaches when studying these associations from a developmental perspective in the future.

心理状态语言(MSL)是儿童了解自己的社会世界和社会地位的重要机制。以往的研究表明,父母可能会根据孩子的性别对孩子使用不同的心理状态语言。然而,研究结果并不一致。本范围综述探讨了父母在使用 MSL 时因儿童性别而产生的差异的一致性,同时还探讨了可能有助于深入了解这些差异的方法学变量。在对所纳入的 27 项研究进行回顾的基础上,12 项研究发现儿童性别与父母的 MSL 之间存在显著关系,而其余研究则没有发现这种关系。所纳入的研究采用了一系列方法来激发 MSL。本范围综述使研究人员和从业人员能够对儿童性别与父母 MSL 之间关系的假设进行反思。此外,我们呼吁今后在从发展的角度研究这些关联时,应使用多样化的知情方法。
{"title":"Parents' mental state language and child gender: A scoping review of developmental evidence.","authors":"Callyn Farrell, Ellen Sanderson, Aisling Mulvihill, Michael Thai, Virginia Slaughter","doi":"10.1111/bjdp.12502","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1111/bjdp.12502","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Mental state language (MSL) is an important mechanism through which children learn about their social world and place within it. Previous research has suggested that parents may use MSL differently towards children based on their child's gender. However, findings are inconsistent. This scoping review explores the consistency of reported differences in parents' MSL use as a function of children's gender while exploring the methodological variables that may provide insights into these differences. Based on a review of the 27 studies included, 12 found a significant relationship between child gender and parents' MSL, while the remaining did not. The included studies used a range of methodological approaches to elicit MSL. This scoping review allows researchers and practitioners to reflect upon assumptions regarding the associations between child gender and parents' MSL. Further, we call for the use of diverse and informed approaches when studying these associations from a developmental perspective in the future.</p>","PeriodicalId":51418,"journal":{"name":"British Journal of Developmental Psychology","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.3,"publicationDate":"2024-06-06","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141262596","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Effects of conflicting emotional cues on toddlers’ emotion perception 相互冲突的情绪线索对幼儿情绪感知的影响。
IF 2.6 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-06-04 DOI: 10.1111/bjdp.12501
Shannon M. Brady, Marissa Ogren, Scott P. Johnson

The communication of emotion is dynamic and occurs across multiple channels, such as facial expression and tone of voice. When cues are in conflict, interpreting emotion can become challenging. Here, we examined the effects of incongruent emotional cues on toddlers’ interpretation of emotions. We presented 33 children (22–26 months, Mage = 23.8 months, 15 female) with side-by-side images of faces along with sentences spoken in a tone of voice that conflicted with semantic content. One of the two faces matched the emotional tone of the audio, whereas the other matched the semantic content. For both congruent and incongruent trials, toddlers showed no overall looking preference to either type of face stimuli. However, during the second exposure to the sentences of incongruent trials, older children tended to look longer to the face matching semantic content when listening to happy vs. angry content. Results inform our understanding of the early development of complex emotion understanding.

情感的交流是动态的,通过面部表情和语调等多种渠道进行。当线索发生冲突时,解读情绪就会变得具有挑战性。在此,我们研究了不一致的情绪线索对幼儿解读情绪的影响。我们向 33 名幼儿(22-26 个月,平均年龄 = 23.8 个月,其中 15 名为女性)展示了并排的人脸图像,以及与语义内容相冲突的语音语调句子。两张人脸中的一张与语音的情绪语调相吻合,而另一张则与语义内容相吻合。在一致和不一致的试验中,学步儿童对这两种人脸刺激都没有表现出整体的偏好。然而,在第二次接触不一致试验的句子时,年龄较大的幼儿在听快乐与愤怒的内容时,倾向于看与语义内容相匹配的面孔更长的时间。这些结果有助于我们理解复杂情绪理解的早期发展。
{"title":"Effects of conflicting emotional cues on toddlers’ emotion perception","authors":"Shannon M. Brady,&nbsp;Marissa Ogren,&nbsp;Scott P. Johnson","doi":"10.1111/bjdp.12501","DOIUrl":"10.1111/bjdp.12501","url":null,"abstract":"<p>The communication of emotion is dynamic and occurs across multiple channels, such as facial expression and tone of voice. When cues are in conflict, interpreting emotion can become challenging. Here, we examined the effects of incongruent emotional cues on toddlers’ interpretation of emotions. We presented 33 children (22–26 months, <i>M</i><sub>age</sub> = 23.8 months, 15 female) with side-by-side images of faces along with sentences spoken in a tone of voice that conflicted with semantic content. One of the two faces matched the emotional tone of the audio, whereas the other matched the semantic content. For both congruent and incongruent trials, toddlers showed no overall looking preference to either type of face stimuli. However, during the second exposure to the sentences of incongruent trials, older children tended to look longer to the face matching semantic content when listening to happy vs. angry content. Results inform our understanding of the early development of complex emotion understanding.</p>","PeriodicalId":51418,"journal":{"name":"British Journal of Developmental Psychology","volume":"42 3","pages":"376-391"},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2024-06-04","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1111/bjdp.12501","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141262504","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Differing gender diverse children have differing experiences with same- and other-gender peers. 不同性别的儿童在与同性和异性同伴相处时有不同的经历。
IF 2.3 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-05-21 DOI: 10.1111/bjdp.12500
Carol Lynn Martin, Sonya Xinyue Xiao, Dawn DeLay, Richard A Fabes, Laura D Hanish, Krista Oswalt

How gender diversity is exhibited varies: some individuals feel similar to the other gender; others experience little similarity to either gender, and some feel similar to both genders. For children, do these variations relate to differing relationships with peers? The goal was to assess whether a community sample of children (884, Mage = 9.04, SD = .90, 51% boys/1 transgender boy; 57% non-Latinx) with differing types of gender diversity have differing relationship experiences and beliefs about same- and other-gender peers. Gender diversity was determined by gender self-concepts (Both-Gender Similar, Cross-Gender Similar, Low-Gender Similar); these were compared among themselves and to gender-typical children (Own-Gender Similar). Results confirmed that children who exhibited differing gender diversity patterns varied in their peer experiences such that gender self-concept matching was found: Children who felt more similar to other-gender peers reported more contact and felt included and efficacious with other-gender peers; children who felt more similar to same-gender peers reported more contact and felt included and efficacious with same-gender peers. These findings suggest that children with two of the atypical patterns (i.e., Cross-Gender and Both-Gender) may experience social benefits that gender typical children do not. These findings illustrate the variability and strengths among gender diverse children.

性别多样性的表现方式各不相同:有些人觉得自己与另一种性别相似;有些人则觉得自己与任何一种性别都不相似,还有些人觉得自己与两种性别都相似。对于儿童来说,这些差异是否与不同的同伴关系有关?我们的目标是评估一个社区样本(884 人,Mage = 9.04,SD = .90,51% 为男孩/1 名变性男孩;57% 为非拉丁裔)中具有不同性别多样性的儿童是否对同性和异性同伴有不同的关系体验和看法。性别多样性由性别自我概念(双性别相似、跨性别相似、低性别相似)决定;这些概念在儿童之间以及与性别典型儿童(自身性别相似)之间进行比较。结果证实,表现出不同性别多样性模式的儿童在他们的同伴经历中各不相同,从而发现了性别自我概念匹配:感觉与其他性别同伴更相似的儿童与其他性别同伴有更多的接触,并感觉与其他性别同伴在一起是包容和有效的;感觉与同性别同伴更相似的儿童与同性别同伴有更多的接触,并感觉与同性别同伴在一起是包容和有效的。这些发现表明,具有两种非典型模式(即跨性别和双性别)的儿童可能会体验到性别典型儿童所没有的社交益处。这些发现说明了不同性别儿童之间的差异和优势。
{"title":"Differing gender diverse children have differing experiences with same- and other-gender peers.","authors":"Carol Lynn Martin, Sonya Xinyue Xiao, Dawn DeLay, Richard A Fabes, Laura D Hanish, Krista Oswalt","doi":"10.1111/bjdp.12500","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1111/bjdp.12500","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>How gender diversity is exhibited varies: some individuals feel similar to the other gender; others experience little similarity to either gender, and some feel similar to both genders. For children, do these variations relate to differing relationships with peers? The goal was to assess whether a community sample of children (884, M<sub>age</sub> = 9.04, SD = .90, 51% boys/1 transgender boy; 57% non-Latinx) with differing types of gender diversity have differing relationship experiences and beliefs about same- and other-gender peers. Gender diversity was determined by gender self-concepts (Both-Gender Similar, Cross-Gender Similar, Low-Gender Similar); these were compared among themselves and to gender-typical children (Own-Gender Similar). Results confirmed that children who exhibited differing gender diversity patterns varied in their peer experiences such that gender self-concept matching was found: Children who felt more similar to other-gender peers reported more contact and felt included and efficacious with other-gender peers; children who felt more similar to same-gender peers reported more contact and felt included and efficacious with same-gender peers. These findings suggest that children with two of the atypical patterns (i.e., Cross-Gender and Both-Gender) may experience social benefits that gender typical children do not. These findings illustrate the variability and strengths among gender diverse children.</p>","PeriodicalId":51418,"journal":{"name":"British Journal of Developmental Psychology","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.3,"publicationDate":"2024-05-21","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141072172","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Children's attitudes about transgender identity disclosure and concealment. 儿童对公开和隐瞒变性身份的态度。
IF 2.3 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-05-18 DOI: 10.1111/bjdp.12493
Daniel J Alonso, Ashley E Jordan, Selin Gülgöz

Supportive peers are crucial for transgender children's well-being. Transgender children who live in their affirmed gender face decisions surrounding concealment and disclosure of their transgender identity. We sought to understand how cisgender (N = 115) and gender-diverse children (N = 127), and siblings of gender-diverse children (N = 63) think about transition disclosure and concealment. All groups viewed transition disclosure and concealment positively. However, gender-diverse children showed greater acceptance of transition concealment and had stronger liking of transition concealers (relative to non-transition concealers). Additionally, children generally expected transgender peers to be selective about who they disclose to, valuing trustworthiness and diverse friend groups in such decisions. Our findings suggest that regardless of gender identity, children are sensitive to the potential costs of disclosure and may support trans children however they choose to navigate these decisions.

支持他们的同伴对变性儿童的福祉至关重要。生活在自己被肯定的性别中的变性儿童面临着隐瞒和公开自己变性身份的抉择。我们试图了解顺性别儿童(115 人)、性别多元化儿童(127 人)以及性别多元化儿童的兄弟姐妹(63 人)是如何看待变性身份的披露和隐瞒的。所有群体都积极看待变性披露和隐瞒。不过,性别多元化儿童对变性隐瞒的接受度更高,而且更喜欢变性隐瞒者(相对于非变性隐瞒者而言)。此外,孩子们普遍认为变性同伴在向谁披露时会有所选择,他们在做此类决定时更看重可信度和多样化的朋友群体。我们的研究结果表明,无论性别认同如何,儿童都会对披露信息的潜在代价保持敏感,并可能会支持变性儿童做出这些决定。
{"title":"Children's attitudes about transgender identity disclosure and concealment.","authors":"Daniel J Alonso, Ashley E Jordan, Selin Gülgöz","doi":"10.1111/bjdp.12493","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1111/bjdp.12493","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Supportive peers are crucial for transgender children's well-being. Transgender children who live in their affirmed gender face decisions surrounding concealment and disclosure of their transgender identity. We sought to understand how cisgender (N = 115) and gender-diverse children (N = 127), and siblings of gender-diverse children (N = 63) think about transition disclosure and concealment. All groups viewed transition disclosure and concealment positively. However, gender-diverse children showed greater acceptance of transition concealment and had stronger liking of transition concealers (relative to non-transition concealers). Additionally, children generally expected transgender peers to be selective about who they disclose to, valuing trustworthiness and diverse friend groups in such decisions. Our findings suggest that regardless of gender identity, children are sensitive to the potential costs of disclosure and may support trans children however they choose to navigate these decisions.</p>","PeriodicalId":51418,"journal":{"name":"British Journal of Developmental Psychology","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.3,"publicationDate":"2024-05-18","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140960680","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Who holds the social power? The development of children's social power perceptions in China 谁掌握着社会权力?中国儿童社会权力认知的发展。
IF 2.6 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-05-15 DOI: 10.1111/bjdp.12499
Chenglong Wang, Yunqiang Lin, Yijin Yang, Tingyu Li, Nanhua Cheng, Congcong Yan

This study examined the development of social power perceptions among Chinese children aged 3–5 years (N = 105). After watching videos about various social power cues, such as resource possession, resource control, goal achievement, permission, giving orders, setting norms and popularity, the children were asked to identify the powerful agents (whom do you believe is the more powerful person?) in the videos and provide explanations (why do you think he (she) is a powerful person?). Three-year-olds can recognize powerful agents who can grant ‘permission’ to other agents. By the age of 4, children begin to associate ‘popularity’, ‘resource possession’ and ‘goal achievement’ with social power. Five-year olds demonstrated the ability to recognize agents who control resources as being more powerful. Analysis of the reasons the children provided for their judgements revealed that for almost every cue (except giving orders), more than 14% of the responses highlighted ‘possession of material resources’ as an indicator of power. For children aged 3–5 years, ‘resource possession’ cues may be their preferred basis for inferring and explaining power differences. These results would facilitate researchers to further unravel the mechanisms underlying the development of children's social power perceptions.

本研究考察了 3-5 岁中国儿童(人数 = 105)对社会权力认知的发展情况。在观看了有关资源占有、资源控制、目标实现、许可、发号施令、制定规范和受欢迎程度等各种社会权力线索的视频后,研究人员要求儿童识别视频中的强势主体(你认为谁是更强势的人?3 岁的幼儿能够辨认出能给予其他代理人 "许可 "的有权势的代理人。4 岁时,儿童开始将 "受欢迎程度"、"资源占有 "和 "目标实现 "与社会权力联系起来。5 岁的儿童能够识别出控制资源的代理更强大。对儿童作出判断的原因分析表明,对于几乎所有的线索(除发号施令外),超过 14% 的回答都强调 "拥有物质资源 "是权力的标志。对于 3-5 岁的儿童来说,"资源占有 "线索可能是他们推断和解释权力差异的首选依据。这些结果将有助于研究人员进一步揭示儿童社会权力认知的发展机制。
{"title":"Who holds the social power? The development of children's social power perceptions in China","authors":"Chenglong Wang,&nbsp;Yunqiang Lin,&nbsp;Yijin Yang,&nbsp;Tingyu Li,&nbsp;Nanhua Cheng,&nbsp;Congcong Yan","doi":"10.1111/bjdp.12499","DOIUrl":"10.1111/bjdp.12499","url":null,"abstract":"<p>This study examined the development of social power perceptions among Chinese children aged 3–5 years (<i>N</i> = 105). After watching videos about various social power cues, such as resource possession, resource control, goal achievement, permission, giving orders, setting norms and popularity, the children were asked to identify the powerful agents (whom do you believe is the more powerful person?) in the videos and provide explanations (why do you think he (she) is a powerful person?). Three-year-olds can recognize powerful agents who can grant ‘permission’ to other agents. By the age of 4, children begin to associate ‘popularity’, ‘resource possession’ and ‘goal achievement’ with social power. Five-year olds demonstrated the ability to recognize agents who control resources as being more powerful. Analysis of the reasons the children provided for their judgements revealed that for almost every cue (except giving orders), more than 14% of the responses highlighted ‘possession of material resources’ as an indicator of power. For children aged 3–5 years, ‘resource possession’ cues may be their preferred basis for inferring and explaining power differences. These results would facilitate researchers to further unravel the mechanisms underlying the development of children's social power perceptions.</p>","PeriodicalId":51418,"journal":{"name":"British Journal of Developmental Psychology","volume":"42 3","pages":"359-375"},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2024-05-15","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140923793","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
What do measures of gender identity tell us about gender identity over time? 随着时间的推移,性别认同的测量结果能告诉我们什么?
IF 2.3 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-05-07 DOI: 10.1111/bjdp.12491
Ellena Fisher, Sarah Wright, Cora Sargeant

Gender identity is a multifaceted concept and is represented by a wide range of measures and constructs including both self-report and researcher observations of preferences and behaviours. However, despite their similar theoretical underpinning, gender identity measures are rarely found to correlate with one another, and contrasting patterns and trajectories are often found for each construct (Egan & Perry, Developmental Psychology, 37, 2001, 451). Therefore, this systematic review aimed to present a review of the longitudinal research evidence surrounding gender identity development in the absence of formal intervention. Using a systematic search strategy, 21 studies were identified. Narrative synthesis was used to synthesize the data collected in these studies and trajectories were explored for (1) self-identification measures of gender identity, (2) clothing preferences, (3) peer preferences, and (4) object/activity preferences. Overall, the results of this systematic review are consistent with wider research suggesting that distinct developmental patterns can be observed when using different constructs and measures of gender identity.

性别认同是一个多层面的概念,有多种测量方法和建构,包括自我报告和研究人员对偏好和行为的观察。然而,尽管性别认同的理论基础相似,但很少发现性别认同的测量指标之间存在相互关联,而且每种建构的模式和轨迹往往截然不同(Egan 和 Perry,《发展心理学》,37,2001,451)。因此,本系统综述旨在回顾在没有正式干预的情况下,有关性别认同发展的纵向研究证据。通过系统性搜索策略,我们确定了 21 项研究。我们采用叙事综合法对这些研究中收集的数据进行了综合,并探讨了以下方面的发展轨迹:(1)性别认同的自我认同测量;(2)服装偏好;(3)同伴偏好;以及(4)对象/活动偏好。总体而言,本系统综述的结果与更广泛的研究相一致,表明在使用不同的性别认同建构和测量方法时,可以观察到不同的发展模式。
{"title":"What do measures of gender identity tell us about gender identity over time?","authors":"Ellena Fisher, Sarah Wright, Cora Sargeant","doi":"10.1111/bjdp.12491","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1111/bjdp.12491","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Gender identity is a multifaceted concept and is represented by a wide range of measures and constructs including both self-report and researcher observations of preferences and behaviours. However, despite their similar theoretical underpinning, gender identity measures are rarely found to correlate with one another, and contrasting patterns and trajectories are often found for each construct (Egan & Perry, Developmental Psychology, 37, 2001, 451). Therefore, this systematic review aimed to present a review of the longitudinal research evidence surrounding gender identity development in the absence of formal intervention. Using a systematic search strategy, 21 studies were identified. Narrative synthesis was used to synthesize the data collected in these studies and trajectories were explored for (1) self-identification measures of gender identity, (2) clothing preferences, (3) peer preferences, and (4) object/activity preferences. Overall, the results of this systematic review are consistent with wider research suggesting that distinct developmental patterns can be observed when using different constructs and measures of gender identity.</p>","PeriodicalId":51418,"journal":{"name":"British Journal of Developmental Psychology","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.3,"publicationDate":"2024-05-07","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140861381","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The link between early adolescents' gender discrimination and gender attitudes about peers: Does gender similarity matter? 早期青少年的性别歧视与对同伴的性别态度之间的联系:性别相似性重要吗?
IF 2.3 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-05-03 DOI: 10.1111/bjdp.12492
Sonya Xinyue Xiao, Flóra Faragó, Erin T. Clancy, Anne J. Maheux, Kasandra Bermúdez
Gender is one of the most salient social identities, particularly during early adolescence. However, factors related to adolescents' gender attitudes remain underexamined. We examined links between adolescents' gender discrimination, felt‐gender similarity, and intergroup gender attitudes. Participants were 270 adolescents in the United States (Mage = 12.95 years, SD = 1.33; 47.4% adolescent girls; 63.7% White, 12.2% Latinx, 10.7% Black, 4.1% Asian, 5.6% multiracial, and 3% indigenous). Path analyses showed that gender discrimination negatively predicted adolescents' attitudes towards own‐ and other‐gender peers. Felt own‐gender similarity positively predicted own‐gender attitudes as expected, but other‐gender similarity did not predict other‐gender attitudes. Further, own‐ and other‐gender similarity did not interact to predict adolescents' gender attitudes. However, adolescents' attitudes towards other‐gender peers were more negatively impacted by gender discrimination for those who felt highly similar to own‐gender peers than for those with average or low own‐gender similarity. Findings inform potential strategies to improve adolescents' gender attitudes.
性别是最突出的社会身份之一,尤其是在青春期早期。然而,与青少年性别态度相关的因素仍未得到充分研究。我们研究了青少年的性别歧视、感觉到的性别相似性和群体间性别态度之间的联系。参与者为美国的 270 名青少年(平均年龄 = 12.95 岁,标准差 = 1.33;47.4% 为少女;63.7% 为白人,12.2% 为拉丁裔,10.7% 为黑人,4.1% 为亚裔,5.6% 为多种族,3% 为原住民)。路径分析显示,性别歧视会对青少年对待同性和异性同伴的态度产生负面影响。正如预期的那样,感觉到的自身性别相似性会对自身性别态度产生积极的预测作用,但其他性别相似性并不会对其他性别态度产生预测作用。此外,自身性别相似性和其他性别相似性在预测青少年的性别态度方面没有相互作用。然而,与自身性别相似度一般或较低的青少年相比,那些与自身性别相似度较高的青少年对其他性别同伴的态度更容易受到性别歧视的负面影响。研究结果为改善青少年性别态度的潜在策略提供了参考。
{"title":"The link between early adolescents' gender discrimination and gender attitudes about peers: Does gender similarity matter?","authors":"Sonya Xinyue Xiao, Flóra Faragó, Erin T. Clancy, Anne J. Maheux, Kasandra Bermúdez","doi":"10.1111/bjdp.12492","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1111/bjdp.12492","url":null,"abstract":"Gender is one of the most salient social identities, particularly during early adolescence. However, factors related to adolescents' gender attitudes remain underexamined. We examined links between adolescents' gender discrimination, felt‐gender similarity, and intergroup gender attitudes. Participants were 270 adolescents in the United States (<jats:italic>M</jats:italic><jats:sub>age</jats:sub> = 12.95 years, SD = 1.33; 47.4% adolescent girls; 63.7% White, 12.2% Latinx, 10.7% Black, 4.1% Asian, 5.6% multiracial, and 3% indigenous). Path analyses showed that gender discrimination negatively predicted adolescents' attitudes towards own‐ and other‐gender peers. Felt own‐gender similarity positively predicted own‐gender attitudes as expected, but other‐gender similarity did not predict other‐gender attitudes. Further, own‐ and other‐gender similarity did not interact to predict adolescents' gender attitudes. However, adolescents' attitudes towards other‐gender peers were more negatively impacted by gender discrimination for those who felt highly similar to own‐gender peers than for those with average or low own‐gender similarity. Findings inform potential strategies to improve adolescents' gender attitudes.","PeriodicalId":51418,"journal":{"name":"British Journal of Developmental Psychology","volume":"295 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.3,"publicationDate":"2024-05-03","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140827706","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Memory biases for gender‐typed images in a gender‐diverse group of children 不同性别儿童对性别图像的记忆偏差
IF 2.3 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-04-27 DOI: 10.1111/bjdp.12490
Selin Gülgöz, Daniel J. Alonso, Kristina R. Olson, Carol Lynn Martin
Self‐socialization accounts of gender development suggest that children attend more to people of their own gender, activities associated with their own gender and stereotype‐consistent examples in their environment. Evidence comes from research showing children's memory biases for such stimuli. This study sought to replicate these memory biases in 367 6‐ to 11‐year‐old transgender, cisgender and nonbinary children. Children were shown stereotype‐consistent and counter‐stereotypical images related to feminine‐ and masculine‐typed activities performed by girls/women or boys/men. Results showed that transgender and cisgender children showed better recall for activities related to their own gender than the other gender. Neither group showed better recall for own‐gender characters, and transgender children better recalled other‐gender characters. None of the three groups better recalled stereotype‐consistent than counter‐stereotypical images in probed recall, although all groups showed better recall for counter‐stereotypical than stereotype‐consistent images in free recall. These findings provide partial support for self‐socialization accounts of gender development.
关于性别发展的自我社会化观点认为,儿童会更多地关注与自己性别相同的人、与自己性别相关的活动以及环境中与定型观念一致的例子。研究表明,儿童对这些刺激会产生记忆偏差。本研究试图在 367 名 6-11 岁的跨性别、顺性别和非二元性儿童身上复制这些记忆偏差。研究人员向儿童展示了与刻板印象一致的和反刻板印象的图像,这些图像与女孩/妇女或男孩/男子所从事的女性和男性类型的活动有关。结果表明,变性和双性儿童对与自己性别相关的活动的回忆能力优于其他性别的儿童。两组儿童对自己性别角色的回忆能力都没有变性儿童强,而变性儿童对其他性别角色的回忆能力更强。在探究性回忆中,三组儿童对与刻板印象一致的形象的回忆效果均好于对与刻板印象相反的形象的回忆效果,但在自由回忆中,所有组别对与刻板印象相反的形象的回忆效果均好于对与刻板印象一致的形象的回忆效果。这些发现为性别发展的自我社会化观点提供了部分支持。
{"title":"Memory biases for gender‐typed images in a gender‐diverse group of children","authors":"Selin Gülgöz, Daniel J. Alonso, Kristina R. Olson, Carol Lynn Martin","doi":"10.1111/bjdp.12490","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1111/bjdp.12490","url":null,"abstract":"Self‐socialization accounts of gender development suggest that children attend more to people of their own gender, activities associated with their own gender and stereotype‐consistent examples in their environment. Evidence comes from research showing children's memory biases for such stimuli. This study sought to replicate these memory biases in 367 6‐ to 11‐year‐old transgender, cisgender and nonbinary children. Children were shown stereotype‐consistent and counter‐stereotypical images related to feminine‐ and masculine‐typed activities performed by girls/women or boys/men. Results showed that transgender and cisgender children showed better recall for activities related to their own gender than the other gender. Neither group showed better recall for own‐gender characters, and transgender children better recalled other‐gender characters. None of the three groups better recalled stereotype‐consistent than counter‐stereotypical images in probed recall, although all groups showed better recall for counter‐stereotypical than stereotype‐consistent images in free recall. These findings provide partial support for self‐socialization accounts of gender development.","PeriodicalId":51418,"journal":{"name":"British Journal of Developmental Psychology","volume":"60 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.3,"publicationDate":"2024-04-27","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140812068","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
British Journal of Developmental Psychology
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1