首页 > 最新文献

Cognitive Development最新文献

英文 中文
The role of sharing and information type in children’s categorization of privileged and conventional information 共享和信息类型在儿童对特权信息和常规信息进行分类中的作用
IF 1.8 3区 心理学 Q3 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-02-09 DOI: 10.1016/j.cogdev.2024.101420
Helana Girgis , Douglas A. Behrend

One domain that has not been thoroughly investigated is children’s ability to categorize information, specifically conventional (known to others, no restrictions on sharing) and privileged (not known to others, restrictions on sharing). In Study 1, 73 four- and five-year-olds and adults classified conventional and privileged information by how it is shared. All age groups accurately classified the information types, though accuracy improved with age. In Study 2, 68 four- and 6-year-olds and adults were presented with scenarios where information type and how it was shared did not match (privileged but shared) and asked if it was conventional or privileged. Four-year-olds and adults categorized conventional information by its label and did so more than 6-year-olds, while there was no pattern for privileged information. These results support that even 4-year-olds can distinguish between conventional and privileged information, and categorization strategies may differ across age and type of information.

儿童对信息进行分类的能力,特别是对常规信息(为他人所知,分享时无限制)和特权信息(不为他人所知,分享时有限制)的分类能力,是一个尚未得到深入研究的领域。在 "研究 1 "中,73 名四、五岁的儿童和成人根据信息的共享方式对常规信息和特权信息进行了分类。所有年龄组的儿童都能准确地对信息类型进行分类,但准确率随年龄的增长而提高。在研究 2 中,68 名四到六岁的儿童和成人被展示了信息类型和共享方式不一致的情 景(有特权但被共享),并被问及是常规信息还是特权信息。4 岁儿童和成人根据标签对常规信息进行分类的比例高于 6 岁儿童。这些结果证明,即使是 4 岁的幼儿也能区分常规信息和特权信息,而且不同年龄和不同类型的信息的分类策略也可能不同。
{"title":"The role of sharing and information type in children’s categorization of privileged and conventional information","authors":"Helana Girgis ,&nbsp;Douglas A. Behrend","doi":"10.1016/j.cogdev.2024.101420","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogdev.2024.101420","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>One domain that has not been thoroughly investigated is children’s ability to categorize information, specifically conventional (known to others, no restrictions on sharing) and privileged (not known to others, restrictions on sharing). In Study 1, 73 four- and five-year-olds and adults classified conventional and privileged information by how it is shared. All age groups accurately classified the information types, though accuracy improved with age. In Study 2, 68 four- and 6-year-olds and adults were presented with scenarios where information type and how it was shared did not match (privileged but shared) and asked if it was conventional or privileged. Four-year-olds and adults categorized conventional information by its label and did so more than 6-year-olds, while there was no pattern for privileged information. These results support that even 4-year-olds can distinguish between conventional and privileged information, and categorization strategies may differ across age and type of information.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":51422,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Development","volume":"70 ","pages":"Article 101420"},"PeriodicalIF":1.8,"publicationDate":"2024-02-09","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139714228","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Preschool children’s intuitions of parallelism 学龄前儿童的平行直觉
IF 1.8 3区 心理学 Q3 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-02-07 DOI: 10.1016/j.cogdev.2024.101423
Shaojing Gao , Qingfen Hu , Yi Shao

Existing evidence has revealed that geometrical intuitions develop spontaneously without formal education. However, empirical research exploring the development of specific intuitive geometric concepts, such as parallelism, is still lacking. To explore preschoolers’ intuition of parallelism, one of the most fundamental concepts in geometry, a series of deviant-detection items were presented to 96 3- to 5-year-old children. Children showed the ability to discriminate between parallel and intersecting lines at least from the age of 4. We grouped children as discriminators and non-discriminators based on their performance in discriminating parallel and intersecting lines without a cross point. For the children who succeeded in this discrimination, they could distinguish parallel lines from intersecting lines both with and without a visible cross point, although they were easily interfered with the visible cross point. Children also showed some naïve understanding of parallel curves and could extract common features from parallel lines and parallel curves, suggesting that the extension of their intuitions of parallelism might be more general than the definition in Euclidean geometry.

现有证据表明,几何直觉是在没有接受正规教育的情况下自发形成的。然而,对于几何直观概念(如平行)的具体发展仍缺乏实证研究。平行是几何学中最基本的概念之一,为了探索学龄前儿童对平行的直觉,我们向 96 名 3 至 5 岁的儿童展示了一系列偏差检测项目。我们根据儿童对无交叉点的平行线和相交线的判别能力,将他们分为判别能力强和判别能力弱两类。对于成功分辨的儿童来说,他们既能分辨出有交叉点的平行线,也能分辨出无交叉点的相交线,尽管他们很容易受到可见交叉点的干扰。儿童对平行曲线也表现出了一些天真的理解,并能从平行线和平行曲 线中提取共同特征,这表明他们对平行的直觉延伸可能比欧几里得几何中的定义更广。
{"title":"Preschool children’s intuitions of parallelism","authors":"Shaojing Gao ,&nbsp;Qingfen Hu ,&nbsp;Yi Shao","doi":"10.1016/j.cogdev.2024.101423","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogdev.2024.101423","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Existing evidence has revealed that geometrical intuitions develop spontaneously without formal education. However, empirical research exploring the development of specific intuitive geometric concepts, such as parallelism, is still lacking. To explore preschoolers’ intuition of parallelism, one of the most fundamental concepts in geometry, a series of deviant-detection items were presented to 96 3- to 5-year-old children. Children showed the ability to discriminate between parallel and intersecting lines at least from the age of 4. We grouped children as discriminators and non-discriminators based on their performance in discriminating parallel and intersecting lines without a cross point. For the children who succeeded in this discrimination, they could distinguish parallel lines from intersecting lines both with and without a visible cross point, although they were easily interfered with the visible cross point. Children also showed some naïve understanding of parallel curves and could extract common features from parallel lines and parallel curves, suggesting that the extension of their intuitions of parallelism might be more general than the definition in Euclidean geometry.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":51422,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Development","volume":"70 ","pages":"Article 101423"},"PeriodicalIF":1.8,"publicationDate":"2024-02-07","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139699589","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The development of relational language during early childhood: Comprehension and production of cardinal, ordinal, and spatial labels 幼儿期关系语言的发展:心形、序数和空间标签的理解与制作
IF 1.8 3区 心理学 Q3 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-02-02 DOI: 10.1016/j.cogdev.2024.101421
Alycia M. Hund, Alexis R. Colwell

The goal was to specify the developmental trajectory of cardinal, ordinal, and spatial relational language comprehension and production. One hundred sixty-four 3-, 4-, and 5-year-old English-speaking children viewed a row of toy cars and were asked to place the appropriate car(s) into a toy garage based on the label provided (Give Me) or to produce the correct label for the specified car(s) (Tell Me). Children were tested using cardinal (one, three, five), ordinal (first, third, fifth), and spatial (front, middle, back) labels. Language performance improved with age, especially for spatial labels. Language performance was more accurate for cardinal labels than for spatial and ordinal labels. Performance was quite accurate for cardinal labels regardless of condition, whereas comprehension was higher than production for spatial and ordinal labels.

我们的目标是明确心形、序数和空间关系语言理解和制作的发展轨迹。164 名 3、4 和 5 岁讲英语的儿童观看了一排玩具车,并被要求根据提供的标签将相应的汽车放入玩具车库(Give Me)或为指定的汽车贴上正确的标签(Tell Me)。测试中,儿童使用的标签包括心形标签(一、三、五)、序数标签(一、三、五)和空间标签(前、中、后)。语言表达能力随着年龄的增长而提高,尤其是空间标签。与空间标签和顺序标签相比,单词标签的语言表达更为准确。无论在什么条件下,对单词标签的表现都相当准确,而对空间和顺序标签的理解能力则高于制作能力。
{"title":"The development of relational language during early childhood: Comprehension and production of cardinal, ordinal, and spatial labels","authors":"Alycia M. Hund,&nbsp;Alexis R. Colwell","doi":"10.1016/j.cogdev.2024.101421","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.cogdev.2024.101421","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>The goal was to specify the developmental trajectory of cardinal, ordinal, and spatial relational language comprehension and production. One hundred sixty-four 3-, 4-, and 5-year-old English-speaking children viewed a row of toy cars and were asked to place the appropriate car(s) into a toy garage based on the label provided (Give Me) or to produce the correct label for the specified car(s) (Tell Me). Children were tested using cardinal (one, three, five), ordinal (first, third, fifth), and spatial (front, middle, back) labels. Language performance improved with age, especially for spatial labels. Language performance was more accurate for cardinal labels than for spatial and ordinal labels. Performance was quite accurate for cardinal labels regardless of condition, whereas comprehension was higher than production for spatial and ordinal labels.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":51422,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Development","volume":"70 ","pages":"Article 101421"},"PeriodicalIF":1.8,"publicationDate":"2024-02-02","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139669393","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Variability in the relationship between parenting and executive functions: The role of environmental sensitivity 养育子女与执行功能之间关系的变异性:环境敏感性的作用
IF 1.8 3区 心理学 Q3 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-02-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.cogdev.2024.101418
Niamh Oeri , Nora Tilda Kunz , Michael Pluess

Previous research suggests that children differ substantially in their sensitivity to positive and negative parenting qualities. In a Swiss sample of N = 264 (Mage: 6.0 years, 50.4% female, 15% migration background), we examined the interaction between parenting and children’s sensitivity on executive functions (EF). Results showed that EF performance tended to be higher for sensitive children whose parents reported more involved parenting and tended to be lower for sensitive children whose parents reported the use of corporal punishment. No such effects emerged for less sensitive children. The results suggest that parenting quality may be more strongly related to EF performance in more sensitive children compared to less sensitive children.

以往的研究表明,儿童对父母积极和消极养育方式的敏感度存在很大差异。在瑞士的一个 264 人样本(年龄:6.0 岁,50.4% 为女性,15% 有移民背景)中,我们研究了教养方式与儿童对执行功能(EF)的敏感性之间的相互作用。结果表明,对于敏感的儿童来说,如果父母的教养方式更有针对性,他们的执行功能表现往往会更高,而对于敏感的儿童来说,如果父母使用体罚,他们的执行功能表现往往会更低。而敏感度较低的儿童则没有这种影响。结果表明,与敏感度较低的儿童相比,养育质量可能与敏感度较高的儿童的 EF 表现有更密切的关系。
{"title":"Variability in the relationship between parenting and executive functions: The role of environmental sensitivity","authors":"Niamh Oeri ,&nbsp;Nora Tilda Kunz ,&nbsp;Michael Pluess","doi":"10.1016/j.cogdev.2024.101418","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.cogdev.2024.101418","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Previous research suggests that children differ substantially in their sensitivity to positive and negative parenting qualities. In a Swiss sample of <em>N</em> = 264 (<em>M</em><sub>age</sub>: 6.0 years, 50.4% female, 15% migration background), we examined the interaction between parenting and children’s sensitivity on executive functions (EF). Results showed that EF performance tended to be higher for sensitive children whose parents reported more involved parenting and tended to be lower for sensitive children whose parents reported the use of corporal punishment. No such effects emerged for less sensitive children. The results suggest that parenting quality may be more strongly related to EF performance in more sensitive children compared to less sensitive children.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":51422,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Development","volume":"70 ","pages":"Article 101418"},"PeriodicalIF":1.8,"publicationDate":"2024-02-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0885201424000030/pdfft?md5=6c2099f942214479a3f1d28f3c6c7801&pid=1-s2.0-S0885201424000030-main.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139669855","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Influences of social and non-social rewards on cognitive control in childhood 社会和非社会奖励对儿童认知控制的影响
IF 1.8 3区 心理学 Q3 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-01-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.cogdev.2023.101413
Xiaoyu Jin , Da Zhang , Nicolas Chevalier

The modulation of cognitive control by rewards has long been discussed, but there is scarce evidence of how social and non-social rewards influence cognitive control in childhood, especially in the preschool years. Critically, sociality has often been confounded with other important reward dimensions (e.g., tangibility) in prior studies, hence potentially misestimating the effect of social rewards. Thus, the present study re-examined the effects of social and non-social rewards on cognitive control, particularly on proactive and reactive control engagement during childhood. Thirty 5- to 6-year-olds and thirty 9- to 10-year-olds completed an AX-Continuous Performance Task (AX-CPT) during an online session in three conditions: control, social reward, and non-social (i.e., monetary) reward conditions. Social rewards increased younger and older children’s response accuracy, suggesting greater cognitive control. However, no influence on how children engage cognitive control (i.e., proactively or reactively) was observed. The provision of non-social rewards did not influence cognitive performance in either group of children. When controlling for other reward dimension, we found evidence that social rewards, but not non-social rewards, can promote cognitive control performance in childhood.

长期以来,人们一直在讨论奖励对认知控制的调节作用,但关于社会性奖励和非社会性奖励如何影响儿童期,尤其是学龄前儿童的认知控制的证据却很少。重要的是,在以往的研究中,社交性常常与其他重要的奖励维度(如有形性)相混淆,因此可能会误估社交性奖励的效果。因此,本研究重新探讨了社会性和非社会性奖励对认知控制的影响,尤其是对儿童期主动和被动控制参与的影响。30名5至6岁的儿童和30名9至10岁的儿童在网上完成了AX-连续表现任务(AX-CPT),该任务分为三种情况:控制、社交奖励和非社交(即金钱)奖励。社交奖励提高了年龄较小和年龄较大的儿童的反应准确性,这表明他们的认知控制能力更强。但是,没有观察到儿童如何进行认知控制(即主动还是被动)。提供非社会性奖励对两组儿童的认知表现均无影响。在对其他奖励维度进行控制后,我们发现有证据表明,社会奖励而非非社会奖励可以促进儿童的认知控制能力。
{"title":"Influences of social and non-social rewards on cognitive control in childhood","authors":"Xiaoyu Jin ,&nbsp;Da Zhang ,&nbsp;Nicolas Chevalier","doi":"10.1016/j.cogdev.2023.101413","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.cogdev.2023.101413","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>The modulation of cognitive control by rewards has long been discussed, but there is scarce evidence of how social and non-social rewards influence cognitive control in childhood, especially in the preschool years. Critically, sociality has often been confounded with other important reward dimensions (e.g., tangibility) in prior studies, hence potentially misestimating the effect of social rewards. Thus, the present study re-examined the effects of social and non-social rewards on cognitive control, particularly on proactive and reactive control engagement during childhood. Thirty 5- to 6-year-olds and thirty 9- to 10-year-olds completed an AX-Continuous Performance Task (AX-CPT) during an online session in three conditions: control, social reward, and non-social (i.e., monetary) reward conditions. Social rewards increased younger and older children’s response accuracy, suggesting greater cognitive control. However, no influence on how children engage cognitive control (i.e., proactively or reactively) was observed. The provision of non-social rewards did not influence cognitive performance in either group of children. When controlling for other reward dimension, we found evidence that social rewards, but not non-social rewards, can promote cognitive control performance in childhood.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":51422,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Development","volume":"69 ","pages":"Article 101413"},"PeriodicalIF":1.8,"publicationDate":"2024-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139079131","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Rational number representation, math anxiety, and algebra performance in college students 大学生的有理数表示、数学焦虑和代数成绩
IF 1.8 3区 心理学 Q3 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-01-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.cogdev.2024.101417
Sangmi Park, Alena G. Esposito

Understanding the magnitude of rational numbers is crucial for mathematical development. However, children do not readily integrate the quantity of fractions and decimals and are even less likely to show a linear representation of fractions and decimals. Thus, the current study examined whether college-aged individuals show spontaneous quantity integration across distinct notations of rational numbers, whether rational number representation would resemble a number line, and whether their rational number representation predicts algebra performance. Further, we examined whether math anxiety plays a role. We found that college students do develop linear representation integrating the quantity between fractions and decimals. Quantitative representation of rational numbers predicted higher algebra scores. Linearity was not a significant predictor for algebra performance. The relations remained even after controlling for math anxiety. Implications for numerical understanding as well as math achievement are discussed.

理解有理数的大小对数学发展至关重要。然而,儿童并不容易整合分数和小数的数量,更不可能对分数和小数进行线性表示。因此,本研究考察了大学年龄段的学生是否会在不同的有理数符号中表现出自发的数量整合,有理数表示是否会类似于数线,以及他们的有理数表示是否会预测代数成绩。此外,我们还研究了数学焦虑是否起了作用。我们发现,大学生确实发展出了线性表示法,将分数和小数之间的数量进行了整合。有理数的数量表示预测了较高的代数分数。线性并不是代数成绩的重要预测因素。即使在控制了数学焦虑之后,这种关系依然存在。本文讨论了对数字理解和数学成绩的影响。
{"title":"Rational number representation, math anxiety, and algebra performance in college students","authors":"Sangmi Park,&nbsp;Alena G. Esposito","doi":"10.1016/j.cogdev.2024.101417","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.cogdev.2024.101417","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Understanding the magnitude of rational numbers is crucial for mathematical development. However, children do not readily integrate the quantity of fractions and decimals and are even less likely to show a linear representation of fractions and decimals. Thus, the current study examined whether college-aged individuals show spontaneous quantity integration across distinct notations of rational numbers, whether rational number representation would resemble a number line, and whether their rational number representation predicts algebra performance. Further, we examined whether math anxiety plays a role. We found that college students do develop linear representation integrating the quantity between fractions and decimals. Quantitative representation of rational numbers predicted higher algebra scores. Linearity was not a significant predictor for algebra performance. The relations remained even after controlling for math anxiety. Implications for numerical understanding as well as math achievement are discussed.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":51422,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Development","volume":"69 ","pages":"Article 101417"},"PeriodicalIF":1.8,"publicationDate":"2024-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139561095","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Non-numerical features fail to predict numerical performance in real-world stimuli 非数字特征无法预测真实世界刺激中的数字表现
IF 1.8 3区 心理学 Q3 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-01-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.cogdev.2023.101415
Emily M. Sanford, Justin Halberda

It has been proposed that humans use non-numerical features (such as convex hull and surface area) to estimate the number of objects in a scene. This would be an evolutionarily advantageous strategy if such features truly patterned with number in the world, but this has never been empirically tested. Here, we quantify the strength of the relationship between number and non-numerical features in two relevant image sets: the illustrations from children’s counting books, and real-world photographs. We find that non-numerical features are much less predictive of the number of objects in counting books than in photographs, despite the former being specifically designed for use in teaching children about numbers. Then, across three behavioral experiments, we ask whether the stronger relationship in photographs predicts better number estimation performance in adults (N = 120) and in children (N = 94; M age = 7;2 years). Our experiments reveal that number estimation is easier from the counting books than the photographs, even though non-numerical features are less predictive of number in books. This analysis uses real-world stimuli and draws into question the claim that non-numerical features are intrinsically involved in number extraction.

有人提出,人类利用非数字特征(例如凸壳和表面积)来估计场景中物体的数量。如果这些特征真的与世界中的物体数量相关,那么这将是一种有利的进化策略,但这一观点从未经过实证检验。在这里,我们量化了两组相关图像中数字特征与非数字特征之间关系的强度:儿童数数书中的插图和现实世界中的照片。我们发现,与照片相比,算数书中的非数字特征对物体数量的预测性要弱得多,尽管前者是专门为教授儿童数字而设计的。然后,通过三个行为实验,我们询问照片中更强的关系是否能预测成人(人数=120)和儿童(人数=94;平均年龄=7;2 岁)更好的数字估计成绩。我们的实验表明,尽管书籍中的非数字特征对数字的预测性较弱,但从计数书籍中估计数字比从照片中估计数字更容易。这项分析使用了真实世界的刺激物,并对非数字特征本质上参与数字提取的说法提出了质疑。
{"title":"Non-numerical features fail to predict numerical performance in real-world stimuli","authors":"Emily M. Sanford,&nbsp;Justin Halberda","doi":"10.1016/j.cogdev.2023.101415","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogdev.2023.101415","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>It has been proposed that humans use non-numerical features (such as convex hull and surface area) to estimate the number of objects in a scene. This would be an evolutionarily advantageous strategy if such features truly patterned with number in the world, but this has never been empirically tested. Here, we quantify the strength of the relationship between number and non-numerical features in two relevant image sets: the illustrations from children’s counting books, and real-world photographs. We find that non-numerical features are much less predictive of the number of objects in counting books than in photographs, despite the former being specifically designed for use in teaching children about numbers. Then, across three behavioral experiments, we ask whether the stronger relationship in photographs predicts better number estimation performance in adults (N = 120) and in children (N = 94; <em>M</em> age = 7;2 years). Our experiments reveal that number estimation is easier from the counting books than the photographs, even though non-numerical features are <em>less</em> predictive of number in books. This analysis uses real-world stimuli and draws into question the claim that non-numerical features are intrinsically involved in number extraction.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":51422,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Development","volume":"69 ","pages":"Article 101415"},"PeriodicalIF":1.8,"publicationDate":"2024-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S088520142300120X/pdfft?md5=aa4642a2fd32df13965f71913eab3090&pid=1-s2.0-S088520142300120X-main.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139433904","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Understanding cognitive and language development in refugees: Evidence from displaced syrian children in Turkey 了解难民的认知和语言发展:来自土耳其流离失所的叙利亚儿童的证据
IF 1.8 3区 心理学 Q3 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-01-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.cogdev.2023.101412
Özlem Yeter , Ebru Evcen , Hugh Rabagliati , Duygu Özge

The present study introduces systematic data on the cognitive and linguistic abilities of refugee children. We tested 9–10 year-old Syrian refugee children (N = 25) on their cognitive abilities (i.e., working memory, shifting, inhibitory control, and fluid intelligence) and vocabulary knowledge in Arabic and Turkish. We compared their performance to two non-refugee control groups with low socioeconomic status, matched on age and mother’s education: Arabic-Turkish bilinguals (N = 29) and Turkish monolinguals (N = 19). Refugee children lagged behind both non-refugee groups in the fluid intelligence task. Compared to their bilingual peers, they showed poorer performance in working memory and shifting tasks. On the other hand, these scores matched their monolingual peers, with only slower performance in the shifting task. Greater exposure to trauma and poverty were predictors for lower scores in refugee children’s cognitive tasks. On the language tests, refugee children exhibited a smaller Turkish vocabulary size compared to both non-refugee controls. Trauma exposure, poverty and kindergarten attendance factors were significant predictors for this difference. As for the Arabic language skills, Syrian children outperformed their bilingual peers in Arabic. Although Syrian children displayed a more balanced bilingual profile, their performance in their dominant language (Arabic) was poorer than the bilingual control group’s performance in their dominant language (Turkish). Overall, the results suggest that refugee children’s working memory, shifting and fluid intelligence abilities, as well as mother tongue development, were negatively affected by forced displacement, but they were able to develop Turkish vocabulary skills and match Turkish monolinguals on both working memory and shifting abilities. This is the first piece of evidence suggesting that while being a refugee has adverse effects on children’s cognitive and linguistic development, holding bilingual status may actually create a protective shield in some cognitive abilities for disadvantaged refugee children.

本研究介绍了有关难民儿童认知和语言能力的系统数据。我们测试了 9-10 岁叙利亚难民儿童(25 人)的认知能力(即工作记忆、移位、抑制控制和流体智能)以及阿拉伯语和土耳其语词汇知识。我们将他们的表现与两个社会经济地位较低的非难民对照组进行了比较,这两个对照组的年龄和母亲的教育程度相匹配:阿拉伯语-土耳其语双语组(29 人)和土耳其语单语组(19 人)。难民儿童在流体智力任务中落后于两组非难民儿童。与双语同龄人相比,他们在工作记忆和移位任务中表现较差。另一方面,他们的得分与单语同龄人相当,只是在移位任务中表现较慢。更多的创伤和贫困是难民儿童认知任务得分较低的预测因素。在语言测试中,与非难民对照组相比,难民儿童的土耳其语词汇量较小。创伤暴露、贫困和幼儿园入学率是造成这一差异的重要预测因素。至于阿拉伯语技能,叙利亚儿童在阿拉伯语方面的表现优于他们的双语同龄人。虽然叙利亚儿童的双语能力更均衡,但他们的主导语言(阿拉伯语)表现却比双语对照组的主导语言(土耳其语)表现差。总之,研究结果表明,难民儿童的工作记忆、移位和流体智能能力以及母语发展受到被迫流离失所的负面影响,但他们能够发展土耳其语词汇技能,并在工作记忆和移位能力方面与土耳其单语儿童不相上下。这是第一个证据表明,虽然难民身份会对儿童的认知和语言发展产生不利影响,但持有双语身份实际上可能会对弱势难民儿童的某些认知能力产生保护作用。
{"title":"Understanding cognitive and language development in refugees: Evidence from displaced syrian children in Turkey","authors":"Özlem Yeter ,&nbsp;Ebru Evcen ,&nbsp;Hugh Rabagliati ,&nbsp;Duygu Özge","doi":"10.1016/j.cogdev.2023.101412","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.cogdev.2023.101412","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>The present study introduces systematic data on the cognitive and linguistic abilities of refugee children. We tested 9–10 year-old Syrian refugee children (<em>N</em><span> = 25) on their cognitive abilities (i.e., working memory, shifting, inhibitory control, and fluid intelligence) and vocabulary knowledge in Arabic and Turkish. We compared their performance to two non-refugee control groups with low socioeconomic status, matched on age and mother’s education: Arabic-Turkish bilinguals (</span><em>N</em> = 29) and Turkish monolinguals (<em>N</em> = 19). Refugee children lagged behind both non-refugee groups in the fluid intelligence task. Compared to their bilingual peers, they showed poorer performance in working memory and shifting tasks. On the other hand, these scores matched their monolingual peers, with only slower performance in the shifting task. Greater exposure to trauma and poverty were predictors for lower scores in refugee children’s cognitive tasks. On the language tests, refugee children exhibited a smaller Turkish vocabulary size compared to both non-refugee controls. Trauma exposure, poverty and kindergarten attendance factors were significant predictors for this difference. As for the Arabic language skills, Syrian children outperformed their bilingual peers in Arabic. Although Syrian children displayed a more balanced bilingual profile, their performance in their dominant language (Arabic) was poorer than the bilingual control group’s performance in their dominant language (Turkish). Overall, the results suggest that refugee children’s working memory, shifting and fluid intelligence abilities, as well as mother tongue development, were negatively affected by forced displacement, but they were able to develop Turkish vocabulary skills and match Turkish monolinguals on both working memory and shifting abilities. This is the first piece of evidence suggesting that while being a refugee has adverse effects on children’s cognitive and linguistic development, holding bilingual status may actually create a protective shield in some cognitive abilities for disadvantaged refugee children.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":51422,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Development","volume":"69 ","pages":"Article 101412"},"PeriodicalIF":1.8,"publicationDate":"2024-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139061730","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Preschoolers’ ability to build inferences about a film protagonist’s emotional state 学龄前儿童推断电影主人公情绪状态的能力
IF 1.8 3区 心理学 Q3 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-01-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.cogdev.2023.101414
Marie Sophie Hunze , Franziska Freudenberger , Yvonne Gerigk , Gerhild Nieding , Peter Ohler , Anna-Katharina Diergarten

In this study, we investigated whether preschool children can draw inferences about the emotional state of a film protagonist. A sample of 59 children aged 4 and 5 watched 20 scenes from a television show. Their ability to understand the protagonist’s emotional states was assessed using a reaction time task. Media sign literacy, theory of mind abilities, and the general understanding of the scenes were also evaluated. Our results show that even children aged 4 drew emotional inferences but only on a valence-based level (i.e., distinguishing between emotions of different valences). No exact inferences (i.e., distinguishing between emotions of the same valence) were drawn. There were no significant influences of media sign literacy or theory of mind on the inference generation, but general understanding of the film scenes was important.

在本研究中,我们调查了学龄前儿童能否推断出电影主人公的情绪状态。59 名 4 岁和 5 岁的儿童观看了一部电视节目中的 20 个场景。他们理解主人公情绪状态的能力通过反应时间任务进行评估。此外,还对儿童的媒体符号素养、心智理论能力以及对场景的总体理解能力进行了评估。我们的研究结果表明,即使是 4 岁的儿童也能进行情绪推断,但仅限于以情绪为基础的推断(即区分不同情绪)。他们不会做出精确的推断(即区分相同情绪)。媒体符号素养或心智理论对推断的产生没有明显影响,但对电影场景的一般理解非常重要。
{"title":"Preschoolers’ ability to build inferences about a film protagonist’s emotional state","authors":"Marie Sophie Hunze ,&nbsp;Franziska Freudenberger ,&nbsp;Yvonne Gerigk ,&nbsp;Gerhild Nieding ,&nbsp;Peter Ohler ,&nbsp;Anna-Katharina Diergarten","doi":"10.1016/j.cogdev.2023.101414","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.cogdev.2023.101414","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>In this study, we investigated whether preschool children can draw inferences about the emotional state of a film protagonist. A sample of 59 children aged 4 and 5 watched 20 scenes from a television show. Their ability to understand the protagonist’s emotional states was assessed using a reaction time task. Media sign literacy, theory of mind abilities, and the general understanding of the scenes were also evaluated. Our results show that even children aged 4 drew emotional inferences but only on a valence-based level (i.e., distinguishing between emotions of different valences). No exact inferences (i.e., distinguishing between emotions of the same valence) were drawn. There were no significant influences of media sign literacy or theory of mind on the inference generation, but general understanding of the film scenes was important.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":51422,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Development","volume":"69 ","pages":"Article 101414"},"PeriodicalIF":1.8,"publicationDate":"2024-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139092957","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Self-derivation through memory integration: a longitudinal examination of performance and relations with academic achievements in elementary classrooms 通过记忆整合进行自我激励:对小学课堂表现及其与学业成绩关系的纵向研究
IF 1.8 3区 心理学 Q3 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-01-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.cogdev.2024.101416
Alena G. Esposito , Patricia J. Bauer

Self-derivation through memory integration is the cognitive process of generating new knowledge by integrating individual facts. Across two studies, we longitudinally examined developmental change, individual stability, and relations with academic performance in a diverse agricultural community. We documented children’s self-derivation in their classrooms and examined the relation with self-derivation and academic performance a year later. In Study 1, we examined self-derivation (n = 94; Mage= 6.67; initially grades K and 1) using the same paradigm at both time points. We found evidence of developmental change from Time 1 to Time 2. However, self-derivation accounted for a small portion of the variance in self-derivation (reflecting individual stability) and academic performance measured one year later. In Study 2, we examined self-derivation across two different paradigms with children beginning in Grades 2 and 3 (n = 82; Mage= 8.60). Even across paradigms, we found evidence for individual stability. Year 1 self-derivation also predicted Year 2 academic performance. We posit that self-derivation through integration is a domain-general construct related to academic performance.

通过记忆整合进行自我衍生是通过整合个体事实产生新知识的认知过程。在两项研究中,我们纵向考察了一个多元化农业社区的发展变化、个体稳定性以及与学习成绩的关系。我们记录了儿童在课堂上的自我激励情况,并考察了一年后自我激励与学习成绩之间的关系。在研究 1 中,我们在两个时间点使用相同的范式对自我激励(n = 94;Mage= 6.67;最初为 K 年级和 1 年级)进行了研究。我们发现了从时间 1 到时间 2 发展变化的证据。然而,在一年后测量的自我激励(反映个体稳定性)和学习成绩差异中,自我激励只占很小一部分。在研究 2 中,我们对从二年级和三年级开始的儿童(n = 82;Mage= 8.60)在两种不同范式下的自我激励进行了研究。即使在不同的范式中,我们也发现了个体稳定性的证据。一年级的自我激励也能预测二年级的学习成绩。我们认为,通过整合进行自我激励是一种与学习成绩相关的领域通用建构。
{"title":"Self-derivation through memory integration: a longitudinal examination of performance and relations with academic achievements in elementary classrooms","authors":"Alena G. Esposito ,&nbsp;Patricia J. Bauer","doi":"10.1016/j.cogdev.2024.101416","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogdev.2024.101416","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p><span><span>Self-derivation through memory integration is the cognitive process of generating new knowledge by integrating individual facts. Across two studies, we longitudinally examined developmental change, individual stability, and relations with </span>academic performance in a diverse agricultural community. We documented children’s self-derivation in their classrooms and examined the relation with self-derivation and academic performance a year later. In Study 1, we examined self-derivation (</span><em>n</em> = 94; <em>M</em><sub>age</sub>= 6.67; initially grades K and 1) using the same paradigm at both time points. We found evidence of developmental change from Time 1 to Time 2. However, self-derivation accounted for a small portion of the variance in self-derivation (reflecting individual stability) and academic performance measured one year later. In Study 2, we examined self-derivation across two different paradigms with children beginning in Grades 2 and 3 (<em>n</em> = 82; <em>M</em><sub>age</sub>= 8.60). Even across paradigms, we found evidence for individual stability. Year 1 self-derivation also predicted Year 2 academic performance. We posit that self-derivation through integration is a domain-general construct related to academic performance.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":51422,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Development","volume":"69 ","pages":"Article 101416"},"PeriodicalIF":1.8,"publicationDate":"2024-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139480052","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Cognitive Development
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1