首页 > 最新文献

中国社会科学最新文献

英文 中文
The Chinese Strategy for Actively Responding to Population Ageing 积极应对人口老龄化的中国战略
IF 1.1 Q3 SOCIAL SCIENCES, INTERDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2022-10-02 DOI: 10.1080/02529203.2022.2166288
Hu Zhan, Peng Xizhe, Wu Yushao
Abstract The course of population ageing in China has unique features, and the country’s fundamental national conditions, development path and governance model impose special requirements on the country’s response. A path with Chinese characteristics that actively addresses this issue is urgently needed. The Chinese strategy is built on solid foundations, comprising the governance paradigm advantage of “one core and multiple components”; the structural advantage of “state-family-society” governance arrangements; the national endowment of being a major country with a rich culture; and the late-mover advantage conferred by the scientific and technological innovations of our changing times. It is necessary to clarify the relationship between the national strategy of actively responding to an population ageing and the population development strategy; on this basis, a positive view of ageing need to be cultivated to provide a better environment for the implementation of the national strategy; and to establish a holistic governance framework in which individuals, families, communities, the state, and the world are interconnected by integrating Chinese advantages in the governance of an ageing society. Thus, a systemic Chinese strategy for actively responding to population ageing can be continuously refined.
摘要中国人口老龄化进程具有独特的特点,国家的基本国情、发展道路和治理模式对国家的应对提出了特殊要求。迫切需要一条积极解决这一问题的具有中国特色的道路。中国战略是建立在坚实基础上的,包括“一核多部”的治理范式优势;“国家-家庭-社会”治理安排的结构性优势;作为一个拥有丰富文化的大国的民族禀赋;以及我们这个时代的科技创新所赋予的后发优势。有必要澄清积极应对人口老龄化的国家战略与人口发展战略之间的关系;在此基础上,需要培养积极的老龄化观,为实施国家战略提供更好的环境;通过整合中国在老龄化社会治理方面的优势,建立一个个人、家庭、社区、国家和世界相互联系的整体治理框架。因此,中国积极应对人口老龄化的系统战略可以不断完善。
{"title":"The Chinese Strategy for Actively Responding to Population Ageing","authors":"Hu Zhan, Peng Xizhe, Wu Yushao","doi":"10.1080/02529203.2022.2166288","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/02529203.2022.2166288","url":null,"abstract":"Abstract The course of population ageing in China has unique features, and the country’s fundamental national conditions, development path and governance model impose special requirements on the country’s response. A path with Chinese characteristics that actively addresses this issue is urgently needed. The Chinese strategy is built on solid foundations, comprising the governance paradigm advantage of “one core and multiple components”; the structural advantage of “state-family-society” governance arrangements; the national endowment of being a major country with a rich culture; and the late-mover advantage conferred by the scientific and technological innovations of our changing times. It is necessary to clarify the relationship between the national strategy of actively responding to an population ageing and the population development strategy; on this basis, a positive view of ageing need to be cultivated to provide a better environment for the implementation of the national strategy; and to establish a holistic governance framework in which individuals, families, communities, the state, and the world are interconnected by integrating Chinese advantages in the governance of an ageing society. Thus, a systemic Chinese strategy for actively responding to population ageing can be continuously refined.","PeriodicalId":51743,"journal":{"name":"中国社会科学","volume":"43 1","pages":"57 - 76"},"PeriodicalIF":1.1,"publicationDate":"2022-10-02","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"42575724","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The Ontology of Verbs and the Horizon of Creators 动词本体与创造者视界
IF 1.1 Q3 SOCIAL SCIENCES, INTERDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2022-10-02 DOI: 10.1080/02529203.2022.2166289
Zhao Tingyang
Abstract Philosophy based on epistemology is a philosophy of nouns which is restricted by the horizon of the knower, and cannot explain creation by human beings within its subjectobject frame. Therefore, we propose a philosophy of verbs that establishes itself on creational ontology and re-understands existence and origin from the horizon of the creator, so as to make that “I do” (facio) the starting point for the understanding of all order, ideas, and history that require consideration.
抽象哲学是一种以认识论为基础的名词哲学,它受知者视野的限制,不能在其主客体框架内解释人的创造。因此,我们提出了一种基于创造本体论的动词哲学,并从创造者的视野中重新理解存在和起源,从而使“我愿意”(facio)成为理解所有需要考虑的秩序、思想和历史的起点。
{"title":"The Ontology of Verbs and the Horizon of Creators","authors":"Zhao Tingyang","doi":"10.1080/02529203.2022.2166289","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/02529203.2022.2166289","url":null,"abstract":"Abstract Philosophy based on epistemology is a philosophy of nouns which is restricted by the horizon of the knower, and cannot explain creation by human beings within its subjectobject frame. Therefore, we propose a philosophy of verbs that establishes itself on creational ontology and re-understands existence and origin from the horizon of the creator, so as to make that “I do” (facio) the starting point for the understanding of all order, ideas, and history that require consideration.","PeriodicalId":51743,"journal":{"name":"中国社会科学","volume":"43 1","pages":"24 - 40"},"PeriodicalIF":1.1,"publicationDate":"2022-10-02","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"43362934","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
A Critique of the Philosophical Presuppositions of Cognitive Psychology 认知心理学哲学预设批判
IF 1.1 Q3 SOCIAL SCIENCES, INTERDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2022-10-02 DOI: 10.1080/02529203.2022.2166294
Shu Yueyu, Gao Shenchun
Abstract In the historical interaction of science and philosophy, empirical science has used cumulative research to shed new light on how the science of philosophy understands fundamental scientific issues. As an effort of self-renewal, the initial impetus for the birth of cognitive science was dialogue among disciplines, with a view to making it possible to unlock the mysteries of cognition. In other words, cognitive science was to pave the way for fundamentally updating the philosophical premises of the science of mind or even making possible a new scientific worldview. As a branch of cognitive science, psychology was to have contributed to the renovation of the ontological, methodological and scientific worldview of cognitive science through its unique interpretation of human nature. However, in the course of the development of cognitive science and psychology, psychology has failed to employ its disciplinary endeavors to renew the ontology, methodology and values of the philosophical foundations of cognitive science, and has also failed to contribute to the promotion of cognitive science. On the contrary, cognitive science’s mechanistic worldview and positivist methodology are the sole determinants of the background of cognitive science that shapes psychology’s model of development—that is, cognitive psychology (if one sees cognitive psychology as a concrete manifestation of the interaction of cognitive science and psychology). This study examines cognitive psychology’s ontological, methodological and value presuppositions and scientific worldview and reflects on the blind and passive nature of the discipline’s development. Moreover, from a history of science perspective, it considers how the development of this discipline can contribute to the renewal of scientific concepts in the philosophy of science through the interaction of the mature natural sciences and philosophy and thence provide possibilities for handling the relationship between psychology and philosophy in psychology’s future development.
摘要在科学与哲学的历史互动中,实证科学利用累积研究为哲学科学如何理解基本科学问题提供了新的视角。作为一种自我更新的努力,认知科学诞生的最初动力是学科之间的对话,以期解开认知的奥秘。换句话说,认知科学为从根本上更新心理科学的哲学前提,甚至使新的科学世界观成为可能铺平了道路。心理学作为认知科学的一个分支,通过对人性的独特解读,为认知科学的本体论、方法论和科学世界观的更新做出了贡献。然而,在认知科学和心理学的发展过程中,心理学未能运用其学科努力来更新认知科学哲学基础的本体论、方法论和价值观,也未能为认知科学的发展做出贡献。相反,认知科学的机械世界观和实证主义方法论是塑造心理学发展模式的认知科学背景的唯一决定因素,即认知心理学(如果人们将认知心理学视为认知科学与心理学互动的具体表现)。本研究考察了认知心理学的本体论、方法论和价值预设以及科学世界观,并反思了该学科发展的盲目性和被动性。此外,从科学史的角度,思考了该学科的发展如何通过成熟的自然科学与哲学的互动,促进科学哲学中科学概念的更新,从而为心理学未来发展中处理心理学与哲学的关系提供可能。
{"title":"A Critique of the Philosophical Presuppositions of Cognitive Psychology","authors":"Shu Yueyu, Gao Shenchun","doi":"10.1080/02529203.2022.2166294","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/02529203.2022.2166294","url":null,"abstract":"Abstract In the historical interaction of science and philosophy, empirical science has used cumulative research to shed new light on how the science of philosophy understands fundamental scientific issues. As an effort of self-renewal, the initial impetus for the birth of cognitive science was dialogue among disciplines, with a view to making it possible to unlock the mysteries of cognition. In other words, cognitive science was to pave the way for fundamentally updating the philosophical premises of the science of mind or even making possible a new scientific worldview. As a branch of cognitive science, psychology was to have contributed to the renovation of the ontological, methodological and scientific worldview of cognitive science through its unique interpretation of human nature. However, in the course of the development of cognitive science and psychology, psychology has failed to employ its disciplinary endeavors to renew the ontology, methodology and values of the philosophical foundations of cognitive science, and has also failed to contribute to the promotion of cognitive science. On the contrary, cognitive science’s mechanistic worldview and positivist methodology are the sole determinants of the background of cognitive science that shapes psychology’s model of development—that is, cognitive psychology (if one sees cognitive psychology as a concrete manifestation of the interaction of cognitive science and psychology). This study examines cognitive psychology’s ontological, methodological and value presuppositions and scientific worldview and reflects on the blind and passive nature of the discipline’s development. Moreover, from a history of science perspective, it considers how the development of this discipline can contribute to the renewal of scientific concepts in the philosophy of science through the interaction of the mature natural sciences and philosophy and thence provide possibilities for handling the relationship between psychology and philosophy in psychology’s future development.","PeriodicalId":51743,"journal":{"name":"中国社会科学","volume":"43 1","pages":"119 - 136"},"PeriodicalIF":1.1,"publicationDate":"2022-10-02","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"49503755","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Chinese Culture and Cognitive Science 中国文化与认知科学
IF 1.1 Q3 SOCIAL SCIENCES, INTERDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2022-10-02 DOI: 10.1080/02529203.2022.2166299
Su Jiajia, Ye Haosheng
Abstract In the civilizations of the axial age, Chinese and Western cultures gave birth to different scientific traditions—the image sciences and the natural sciences—within the knowledge frameworks of the union of man and nature and of the subject–object dichotomy respectively. Therefore, there are inevitably insurmountable barriers to explaining the differences between Chinese and Western cultures in terms of a natural science concerned with “things.” However, human nature is universal, and it is possible to make direct comparisons and interpretations of the differences between Chinese and Western cultures in terms of the cognitive sciences dealing with the mind. One important way to compare and interpret Chinese culture and cognitive science is the mutual interpretation of Chinese and Western cultures. In view of this, we have integrated enactive cognition (the frontier of cognitive science) and the free energy principle to propose the predictive mind model of enactive cognition and have used this mutual interpretation to study the two cultures using this model of cognitive science as a truly unified cross-cultural paradigm for cognitive science. First, the theory of enaction embedded in Chinese culture is actually closely related to enactive cognition, the frontier of Western cognitive science. Laozi’s theory of universal enaction (cosmology) shows that Chinese culture has an ancient theory of enaction. He firmly planted in the minds of Chinese people a way of thinking dealing with union of man and nature. Western enactive cognition, as a modern theory, advocates continuity of life and mind and emphasizes that life is both autopoietic and cognitive, which is actually a scientific cognitive expression of the Chinese cultural idea of the harmony of man and nature. This is actually a scientific cognitive expression of Chinese culture’s idea of the harmony of man and nature. Second, the qi in Chinese culture is similar to the free energy in Western cognitive science. In terms of concept definition, qi is a mixture of matter, function and principles (information), while free energy is the synthesis of energy and information. From the point of view of the mode of operation, qi is the contradictory harmony between yin and yang, while free energy is the unity of top-down and bottom-up approaches. In terms of kinetic purpose, the kinetic purpose of qi is the doctrine of benevolence, while the kinetic purpose of free energy has gradually changed from representation of nature to construction of meaning. Through comparison and interpretation, we find that enactive cognition is closely related to Chinese culture on the cognitive science route, in what will be a practical path for the rejuvenation of Chinese culture. In addition, through the association of qi in Chinese culture and free energy in Western cognitive science, Chinese and Western cultures can be expected to be truly connected through cognitive science.
摘要在轴心时代的文明中,中西文化分别在人与自然的结合和主客二分的知识框架内,产生了不同的科学传统——图像科学和自然科学。因此,用关注“物”的自然科学来解释中西文化的差异,不可避免地存在着不可逾越的障碍。然而,人性是普遍的,用关注心灵的认知科学来直接比较和解释中西文化之间的差异是可能的。中西文化互释是比较和解读中国文化和认知科学的一个重要途径。鉴于此,我们将行为认知(认知科学的前沿)和自由能原理相结合,提出了行为认知的预测思维模型,并利用这种相互解释,将这种认知科学模型作为认知科学真正统一的跨文化范式来研究两种文化。首先,植根于中国文化中的行为理论实际上与西方认知科学的前沿——行为认知密切相关。老子的宇宙观表明,中国文化有着古老的宇宙观。他在中国人心中牢固地树立了一种处理人与自然结合问题的思维方式。西方的行为认知作为一种现代理论,主张生命和心灵的连续性,强调生命既是自我生成的,又是认知的,这实际上是中国人与自然和谐文化思想的科学认知表达。这实际上是中国文化人与自然和谐思想的科学认知表达。第二,中国文化中的气与西方认知科学中的自由能相似。在概念定义上,气是物质、功能和原理(信息)的混合物,而自由能是能量和信息的合成。从运行方式来看,气是阴阳矛盾的和谐,而自由能是自上而下和自下而上的统一。在动力目的方面,气的动力目的是仁学,而自由能的动力目的则逐渐从对自然的表征转变为对意义的建构。通过比较和阐释,我们发现,在认知科学的道路上,行为认知与中国文化密切相关,这将是中华文化复兴的一条实践道路。此外,通过中国文化中的气和西方认知科学中的自由能的结合,中西文化可以通过认知科学真正联系起来。
{"title":"Chinese Culture and Cognitive Science","authors":"Su Jiajia, Ye Haosheng","doi":"10.1080/02529203.2022.2166299","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/02529203.2022.2166299","url":null,"abstract":"Abstract In the civilizations of the axial age, Chinese and Western cultures gave birth to different scientific traditions—the image sciences and the natural sciences—within the knowledge frameworks of the union of man and nature and of the subject–object dichotomy respectively. Therefore, there are inevitably insurmountable barriers to explaining the differences between Chinese and Western cultures in terms of a natural science concerned with “things.” However, human nature is universal, and it is possible to make direct comparisons and interpretations of the differences between Chinese and Western cultures in terms of the cognitive sciences dealing with the mind. One important way to compare and interpret Chinese culture and cognitive science is the mutual interpretation of Chinese and Western cultures. In view of this, we have integrated enactive cognition (the frontier of cognitive science) and the free energy principle to propose the predictive mind model of enactive cognition and have used this mutual interpretation to study the two cultures using this model of cognitive science as a truly unified cross-cultural paradigm for cognitive science. First, the theory of enaction embedded in Chinese culture is actually closely related to enactive cognition, the frontier of Western cognitive science. Laozi’s theory of universal enaction (cosmology) shows that Chinese culture has an ancient theory of enaction. He firmly planted in the minds of Chinese people a way of thinking dealing with union of man and nature. Western enactive cognition, as a modern theory, advocates continuity of life and mind and emphasizes that life is both autopoietic and cognitive, which is actually a scientific cognitive expression of the Chinese cultural idea of the harmony of man and nature. This is actually a scientific cognitive expression of Chinese culture’s idea of the harmony of man and nature. Second, the qi in Chinese culture is similar to the free energy in Western cognitive science. In terms of concept definition, qi is a mixture of matter, function and principles (information), while free energy is the synthesis of energy and information. From the point of view of the mode of operation, qi is the contradictory harmony between yin and yang, while free energy is the unity of top-down and bottom-up approaches. In terms of kinetic purpose, the kinetic purpose of qi is the doctrine of benevolence, while the kinetic purpose of free energy has gradually changed from representation of nature to construction of meaning. Through comparison and interpretation, we find that enactive cognition is closely related to Chinese culture on the cognitive science route, in what will be a practical path for the rejuvenation of Chinese culture. In addition, through the association of qi in Chinese culture and free energy in Western cognitive science, Chinese and Western cultures can be expected to be truly connected through cognitive science.","PeriodicalId":51743,"journal":{"name":"中国社会科学","volume":"43 1","pages":"184 - 202"},"PeriodicalIF":1.1,"publicationDate":"2022-10-02","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"42163908","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Introduction 介绍
IF 1.1 Q3 SOCIAL SCIENCES, INTERDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2022-10-02 DOI: 10.1080/02529203.2022.2166300
Ye Haosheng
{"title":"Introduction","authors":"Ye Haosheng","doi":"10.1080/02529203.2022.2166300","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/02529203.2022.2166300","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":51743,"journal":{"name":"中国社会科学","volume":"43 1","pages":"98 - 104"},"PeriodicalIF":1.1,"publicationDate":"2022-10-02","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"43838144","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
On the Dichotomy of the Elegant and the Popular 论雅俗共赏的二分法
IF 1.1 Q3 SOCIAL SCIENCES, INTERDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2022-10-02 DOI: 10.1080/02529203.2022.2166290
Nan Fan
Abstract The theoretical dichotomy of the elegant (ya) and the popular or common (su) has a long history and many branches, provoking Babel-like confusion of tongues. Both have their own aesthetic origins, and each is interpreted in relation to sociological categories such as social class and social strata. No abstract and absolute definition of the two concepts exists. The yayue (formal ceremonial music) of ancient China is an important early example of the elegant or refined, whereas the popular usually prevails at the lower levels of mass society. In the course of history, the accumulation of professional knowledge, selected classics, avant garde “art for art’s sake,” and leisure class activities have often been considered “elegant,” while the experience of the masses, folk revelry, and the content of mass media have been considered “popular.” In modern literary history, the concepts of enlightenment and class were drawn into the dichotomy of the elegant and the popular in different ways. In Chinese culture in the second half of the twentieth century, this dichotomy began to be reorganized around the notions of “art” and “commodity.” The reconceptualization of elegance and popularity at each stage of history has often indicated a reconnection between aesthetic tastes and history. In this sense, the differentiation of elegance and popularity has always testified to culture’s participation in history.
雅俗二分的理论历史悠久,分支众多,引发了巴别塔式的语言混乱。两者都有自己的美学渊源,并且都与社会阶级和社会阶层等社会学范畴有关。这两个概念没有抽象和绝对的定义。中国古代的雅乐(正式的仪式音乐)是高雅或精致的重要早期例子,而通俗音乐通常在大众社会的下层盛行。在历史进程中,专业知识的积累、经典作品的精选、“为艺术而艺术”的先锋派和休闲阶级活动往往被认为是“高雅的”,而大众的体验、民间的狂欢和大众传媒的内容则被认为是“通俗的”。在现代文学史上,启蒙和阶级的概念以不同的方式被卷入高雅与通俗的二分法中。在20世纪下半叶的中国文化中,这种二分法开始围绕“艺术”和“商品”的概念进行重组。在历史的每个阶段,对优雅和流行的重新概念化往往表明审美品味与历史之间的重新联系。从这个意义上说,雅俗共赏的区分一直是文化参与历史的证明。
{"title":"On the Dichotomy of the Elegant and the Popular","authors":"Nan Fan","doi":"10.1080/02529203.2022.2166290","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/02529203.2022.2166290","url":null,"abstract":"Abstract The theoretical dichotomy of the elegant (ya) and the popular or common (su) has a long history and many branches, provoking Babel-like confusion of tongues. Both have their own aesthetic origins, and each is interpreted in relation to sociological categories such as social class and social strata. No abstract and absolute definition of the two concepts exists. The yayue (formal ceremonial music) of ancient China is an important early example of the elegant or refined, whereas the popular usually prevails at the lower levels of mass society. In the course of history, the accumulation of professional knowledge, selected classics, avant garde “art for art’s sake,” and leisure class activities have often been considered “elegant,” while the experience of the masses, folk revelry, and the content of mass media have been considered “popular.” In modern literary history, the concepts of enlightenment and class were drawn into the dichotomy of the elegant and the popular in different ways. In Chinese culture in the second half of the twentieth century, this dichotomy began to be reorganized around the notions of “art” and “commodity.” The reconceptualization of elegance and popularity at each stage of history has often indicated a reconnection between aesthetic tastes and history. In this sense, the differentiation of elegance and popularity has always testified to culture’s participation in history.","PeriodicalId":51743,"journal":{"name":"中国社会科学","volume":"43 1","pages":"41 - 56"},"PeriodicalIF":1.1,"publicationDate":"2022-10-02","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"44017842","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The Integration of Psychology and Linguistics from the Perspective of Cognitive Science—The Example of the Psychological Reality of Language 认知科学视野下的心理学与语言学整合——以语言心理现实为例
IF 1.1 Q3 SOCIAL SCIENCES, INTERDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2022-10-02 DOI: 10.1080/02529203.2022.2166295
Wang Bo, Qiongpeng Luo
Abstract In the traditional debate centered on the nature of language, the question of “the psychological reality of language” occupies a pivotal position. The key lies in the recognition of language (competence) as a natural entity that exists in the brain in some physical way. The empirical research perspectives supporting the psychological reality of language fall into four main categories: (a) linguistic perspectives based on introspective linguistic intuitions; (b) developmental psychology perspectives; (c) psycholinguistic perspectives; (d) neurolinguistic perspectives. In analyzing the four areas of research, it is clear that psychological realism does indeed reveal the physiological basis of language ability, but it does not fully explain how “reality” is expressed, i.e., at what level the operation and representation of language can be linked to human neurobiological mechanisms, and how the psychological reality of language is related to the semantic “material” reality of language. In essence, the ontological foundation of psychological realism is the logocentric tradition of “In the beginning was the Word,” while Chinese thought, which espouses “In the beginning was the Deed,” provides a transcendental framework for solving the dilemma of psychological realism by virtue of its self-cultivation theory of “heaven, in its motion, shows strength.” The focus is not on how language precisely maps reality, but how to mobilize the energy of language through the cultivation of the body and mind. This implies that human language and even its physiological basis develop according to how one learns to change the natural world, and that the most essential basis of language is the change in the world caused by its “doing” (self-cultivation), thus providing an adequate explanation for the problem of the psychological reality of language.
摘要在传统的以语言本质为中心的争论中,“语言的心理现实”问题占有举足轻重的地位。关键在于识别语言(能力)是以某种物理方式存在于大脑中的自然实体。支持语言心理现实的实证研究视角可分为四大类:(a)基于内省语言直觉的语言学视角;(b) 发展心理学视角;(c) 心理语言学视角;(d) 神经语言学观点。在分析这四个研究领域时,很明显,心理现实主义确实揭示了语言能力的生理基础,但它并没有完全解释“现实”是如何表达的,即语言的操作和表征在什么程度上可以与人类的神经生物学机制相联系,以及语言的心理现实与语言的语义“物质”现实之间的关系。从本质上讲,心理现实主义的本体论基础是“以言为始”的语源传统,而信奉“以行为始”思想的中国思想,则凭借其“天在动,力在显”的修身理论,为解决心理现实主义困境提供了一个超越性的框架。“重点不在于语言如何准确地映射现实,而在于如何通过身心的培养来调动语言的能量。这意味着,人类语言甚至其生理基础是根据一个人如何学会改变自然世界而发展的,而语言最本质的基础是其“作为”(修身)所引起的世界变化,从而为语言的心理现实问题提供了充分的解释。
{"title":"The Integration of Psychology and Linguistics from the Perspective of Cognitive Science—The Example of the Psychological Reality of Language","authors":"Wang Bo, Qiongpeng Luo","doi":"10.1080/02529203.2022.2166295","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/02529203.2022.2166295","url":null,"abstract":"Abstract In the traditional debate centered on the nature of language, the question of “the psychological reality of language” occupies a pivotal position. The key lies in the recognition of language (competence) as a natural entity that exists in the brain in some physical way. The empirical research perspectives supporting the psychological reality of language fall into four main categories: (a) linguistic perspectives based on introspective linguistic intuitions; (b) developmental psychology perspectives; (c) psycholinguistic perspectives; (d) neurolinguistic perspectives. In analyzing the four areas of research, it is clear that psychological realism does indeed reveal the physiological basis of language ability, but it does not fully explain how “reality” is expressed, i.e., at what level the operation and representation of language can be linked to human neurobiological mechanisms, and how the psychological reality of language is related to the semantic “material” reality of language. In essence, the ontological foundation of psychological realism is the logocentric tradition of “In the beginning was the Word,” while Chinese thought, which espouses “In the beginning was the Deed,” provides a transcendental framework for solving the dilemma of psychological realism by virtue of its self-cultivation theory of “heaven, in its motion, shows strength.” The focus is not on how language precisely maps reality, but how to mobilize the energy of language through the cultivation of the body and mind. This implies that human language and even its physiological basis develop according to how one learns to change the natural world, and that the most essential basis of language is the change in the world caused by its “doing” (self-cultivation), thus providing an adequate explanation for the problem of the psychological reality of language.","PeriodicalId":51743,"journal":{"name":"中国社会科学","volume":"43 1","pages":"137 - 151"},"PeriodicalIF":1.1,"publicationDate":"2022-10-02","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"47320529","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
From Function to Structure: A Reverse Solution to the Mind-Brain Problem 从功能到结构:思维问题的逆向解法
IF 1.1 Q3 SOCIAL SCIENCES, INTERDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2022-10-02 DOI: 10.1080/02529203.2022.2166298
Jiang Ke, Liao Qiwei
Abstract Starting from the tension and integration between cognitive neuroscience and cognitive psychology, this paper analyzes the waxing and waning of physicalism versus mentalism over the last century and defends an anti-reductionist ontology of the mind. It first argues that “function logically precedes structure” and then refutes two classic arguments about the mind-brain problem. The paper shows that if structure is treated as the logical premise of function, discussions of the mind-brain problem will inevitably lead to dualism or reductionism. Structure cannot explain its own occurrence; instead, if function, which logically precedes structure, is taken as the starting point of the explanation, then the principle of the “ladder of explanation” will be satisfied. A higher-level theory can explain lower-level observed facts, and lower-level observed facts are the cognitive starting point leading to a theory through induction. We conclude that function logically precedes structure; therefore, logically, “the mind shapes the brain.” The logical sequence from function to structure represents a reverse strategy for solving the mind-brain problem.
本文从认知神经科学与认知心理学之间的紧张与融合出发,分析了上个世纪物理主义与心灵主义的兴衰,并为反还原论的心灵本体论进行了辩护。它首先论证了“功能逻辑上先于结构”,然后驳斥了关于心智-大脑问题的两个经典论点。如果把结构作为功能的逻辑前提,对心脑问题的讨论将不可避免地导致二元论或还原论。结构不能解释其自身的发生;相反,如果以逻辑上先于结构的功能作为解释的起点,那么就满足了“解释阶梯”的原则。较高层次的理论可以解释较低层次的观察事实,较低层次的观察事实是通过归纳法导致理论的认知起点。我们得出结论:功能在逻辑上先于结构;因此,从逻辑上讲,“思想塑造大脑”。从功能到结构的逻辑顺序代表了解决心智-大脑问题的相反策略。
{"title":"From Function to Structure: A Reverse Solution to the Mind-Brain Problem","authors":"Jiang Ke, Liao Qiwei","doi":"10.1080/02529203.2022.2166298","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/02529203.2022.2166298","url":null,"abstract":"Abstract Starting from the tension and integration between cognitive neuroscience and cognitive psychology, this paper analyzes the waxing and waning of physicalism versus mentalism over the last century and defends an anti-reductionist ontology of the mind. It first argues that “function logically precedes structure” and then refutes two classic arguments about the mind-brain problem. The paper shows that if structure is treated as the logical premise of function, discussions of the mind-brain problem will inevitably lead to dualism or reductionism. Structure cannot explain its own occurrence; instead, if function, which logically precedes structure, is taken as the starting point of the explanation, then the principle of the “ladder of explanation” will be satisfied. A higher-level theory can explain lower-level observed facts, and lower-level observed facts are the cognitive starting point leading to a theory through induction. We conclude that function logically precedes structure; therefore, logically, “the mind shapes the brain.” The logical sequence from function to structure represents a reverse strategy for solving the mind-brain problem.","PeriodicalId":51743,"journal":{"name":"中国社会科学","volume":"43 1","pages":"168 - 183"},"PeriodicalIF":1.1,"publicationDate":"2022-10-02","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"44968409","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The Roots of Great Power Competition: An Analysis Based on the Increasing Returns Mechanism 大国竞争的根源:基于收益递增机制的分析
IF 1.1 Q3 SOCIAL SCIENCES, INTERDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2022-10-02 DOI: 10.1080/02529203.2022.2166286
Tianzhi Ye
Abstract Geopolitics, technology and international institutions are three major areas of great power competition. In the analysis of great power relations, both the balance of power theory and the hegemony theory rely on the assumption of diminishing returns and ignore the possibility of increasing returns. Increasing returns play an extensive role in geographical location, technological evolution and institutional change; accordingly, the increasing returns mechanism in great power relations is reflected in the geopolitical competition, technological competition and international institutional competition of great powers. The rational expectation that initial advantages will be transformed into long-term advantages through the mechanism of increasing returns drives great powers into competition whether they are rivals or allies. As specific mechanisms leading to increasing returns, coordination effects operate in geopolitical competition, technological competition and international institution competition; learning effects are found in technological competition and international institution competition; and adaptive expectations play a part in international institution competition. Various cases, ranging from the Anglo-Dutch rivalry over maritime hegemony in the 17th century to the current US-European competition over international investment arbitration regimes, show that great power competition derives from the logic of increasing returns.
地缘政治、技术和国际制度是大国竞争的三大领域。在大国关系分析中,均势理论和霸权理论都依赖于收益递减假设,而忽略了收益递增的可能性。收益增加在地理位置、技术演进和制度变迁中发挥着广泛的作用;因此,大国关系中的收益递增机制体现在大国的地缘政治竞争、技术竞争和国际制度竞争上。对初始优势通过收益递增机制转化为长期优势的理性预期,驱使大国之间无论是对手还是盟友,都进入竞争状态。协调效应作为导致收益递增的具体机制,在地缘政治竞争、技术竞争和国际制度竞争中发挥作用;在技术竞争和国际制度竞争中存在学习效应;适应性期望在国际制度竞争中发挥了作用。从17世纪英荷争夺海上霸权,到当前美欧争夺国际投资仲裁制度,各种案例都表明,大国竞争源自回报递增的逻辑。
{"title":"The Roots of Great Power Competition: An Analysis Based on the Increasing Returns Mechanism","authors":"Tianzhi Ye","doi":"10.1080/02529203.2022.2166286","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/02529203.2022.2166286","url":null,"abstract":"Abstract Geopolitics, technology and international institutions are three major areas of great power competition. In the analysis of great power relations, both the balance of power theory and the hegemony theory rely on the assumption of diminishing returns and ignore the possibility of increasing returns. Increasing returns play an extensive role in geographical location, technological evolution and institutional change; accordingly, the increasing returns mechanism in great power relations is reflected in the geopolitical competition, technological competition and international institutional competition of great powers. The rational expectation that initial advantages will be transformed into long-term advantages through the mechanism of increasing returns drives great powers into competition whether they are rivals or allies. As specific mechanisms leading to increasing returns, coordination effects operate in geopolitical competition, technological competition and international institution competition; learning effects are found in technological competition and international institution competition; and adaptive expectations play a part in international institution competition. Various cases, ranging from the Anglo-Dutch rivalry over maritime hegemony in the 17th century to the current US-European competition over international investment arbitration regimes, show that great power competition derives from the logic of increasing returns.","PeriodicalId":51743,"journal":{"name":"中国社会科学","volume":"43 1","pages":"77 - 97"},"PeriodicalIF":1.1,"publicationDate":"2022-10-02","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"41913962","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
We Are One of “The Others” 我们是“其他人”中的一员
IF 1.1 Q3 SOCIAL SCIENCES, INTERDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2022-07-03 DOI: 10.1080/02529203.2022.2122215
Wang Mingming
Abstract Why should human beings be divided into “us” or “them”? This question underlies the basic proposition of anthropology. In fact, it has never received a decisive answer. In the dialogue between Marshall Sahlins and Wang Mingming, the two agree that “we are part of the other,” because it is to the other that we owe the constitution of our full selves. The modernity of diversity and plurality supports the validity of this judgment.
人类为什么要分为“我们”和“他们”?这个问题构成了人类学的基本命题。事实上,这个问题从来没有得到一个决定性的答案。在马歇尔·萨林斯(Marshall Sahlins)与王明明(Wang Mingming)的对话中,两人都同意“我们是他者的一部分”,因为正是他者构成了我们完整的自我。多样性和多元性的现代性支持了这一判断的有效性。
{"title":"We Are One of “The Others”","authors":"Wang Mingming","doi":"10.1080/02529203.2022.2122215","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/02529203.2022.2122215","url":null,"abstract":"Abstract Why should human beings be divided into “us” or “them”? This question underlies the basic proposition of anthropology. In fact, it has never received a decisive answer. In the dialogue between Marshall Sahlins and Wang Mingming, the two agree that “we are part of the other,” because it is to the other that we owe the constitution of our full selves. The modernity of diversity and plurality supports the validity of this judgment.","PeriodicalId":51743,"journal":{"name":"中国社会科学","volume":"43 1","pages":"162 - 179"},"PeriodicalIF":1.1,"publicationDate":"2022-07-03","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"49027784","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
中国社会科学
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1