首页 > 最新文献

Journal of Comparative Psychology最新文献

英文 中文
Supplemental Material for Evidence of Motor Intentions in Plants: A Kinematical Study 植物运动意图证据补充材料:运动学研究
4区 心理学 Q2 Medicine Pub Date : 2023-05-11 DOI: 10.1037/com0000351.supp
{"title":"Supplemental Material for Evidence of Motor Intentions in Plants: A Kinematical Study","authors":"","doi":"10.1037/com0000351.supp","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/com0000351.supp","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":54861,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Comparative Psychology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2023-05-11","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"135473757","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Are you as fooled as I am? Visual illusions in human (Homo) and nonhuman (Sapajus, Gorilla, Pan, Pongo) primate species. 你和我一样被愚弄了吗?人类(Homo)和非人类(Sapajus, Gorilla, Pan, Pongo)灵长类物种的视觉错觉。
IF 1.4 4区 心理学 Q2 Medicine Pub Date : 2023-05-01 DOI: 10.1037/com0000326
Daniel Hanus, Valentina Truppa, Josep Call

It has been argued that humans' susceptibility to visual illusions does not simply reflect cognitive flaws but rather specific functional adaptations of our perceptual system. The data on cross-cultural differences in the perception of geometric illusions seemingly support this explanation. Little is known, however, about the developmental trajectories of such adaptations in humans, let alone a conclusive picture of the illusionary susceptibility in other primate species. So far, most developmental or comparative studies have tested single illusions with varying procedural implementations. The current study aims at overcoming these limitations by testing human subjects of four different age classes (3- to 5 year-old children and adults) and five nonhuman primate species (capuchin monkeys, bonobos, chimpanzees, gorillas, and orangutans) with an identical setup in five well-known geometric illusions (horizontal-vertical, Ebbinghaus, Mueller-Lyer, Ponzo, and Sander). Two food items of identical size were presented on separate trays with surrounding paintings eliciting the illusion of size differences and subjects were required to choose one of the items. Four of the five illusions elicited a strong effect in adult humans, and older children showed a greater susceptibility to illusions than younger ones. In contrast, only two illusions (Ebbingaus and horizontal-vertical) elicited a mild effect on nonhuman primates with high variation within species and little variation between species. Our results suggests that humans learn to see illusions as they develop during childhood. They also suggest that future work should address how nonhuman primates' experience of these illusion changes throughout their development. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2023 APA, all rights reserved).

人们一直认为,人类对视觉错觉的敏感性并不仅仅反映了认知缺陷,而是反映了我们感知系统的特定功能适应。关于几何错觉感知的跨文化差异的数据似乎支持这一解释。然而,我们对人类这种适应的发展轨迹知之甚少,更不用说对其他灵长类动物的错觉易感性的结论性描述了。到目前为止,大多数发展或比较研究都测试了具有不同程序实现的单一错觉。目前的研究旨在克服这些限制,通过测试四个不同年龄段的人类受试者(3至5岁的儿童和成人)和五种非人类灵长类动物(卷尾猴,倭黑猩猩,黑猩猩,大猩猩和猩猩),在五种著名的几何错觉(水平-垂直,艾宾浩斯,穆勒-莱耶,庞佐和桑德)中使用相同的设置。两种大小相同的食物放在不同的托盘上,周围有绘画,引起大小差异的错觉,受试者被要求选择一种食物。这五种错觉中有四种对成年人产生了强烈的影响,年龄较大的儿童比年幼的儿童更容易受到错觉的影响。相比之下,只有两种错觉(Ebbingaus幻觉和水平-垂直幻觉)对非人类灵长类动物产生了轻微的影响,物种内差异很大,物种间差异很小。我们的研究结果表明,人类在童年时期就学会了看到幻觉。他们还建议,未来的工作应该解决非人类灵长类动物在整个发育过程中对这些幻觉的体验是如何变化的。(PsycInfo数据库记录(c) 2023 APA,版权所有)。
{"title":"Are you as fooled as I am? Visual illusions in human (Homo) and nonhuman (Sapajus, Gorilla, Pan, Pongo) primate species.","authors":"Daniel Hanus,&nbsp;Valentina Truppa,&nbsp;Josep Call","doi":"10.1037/com0000326","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/com0000326","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>It has been argued that humans' susceptibility to visual illusions does not simply reflect cognitive flaws but rather specific functional adaptations of our perceptual system. The data on cross-cultural differences in the perception of geometric illusions seemingly support this explanation. Little is known, however, about the developmental trajectories of such adaptations in humans, let alone a conclusive picture of the illusionary susceptibility in other primate species. So far, most developmental or comparative studies have tested single illusions with varying procedural implementations. The current study aims at overcoming these limitations by testing human subjects of four different age classes (3- to 5 year-old children and adults) and five nonhuman primate species (capuchin monkeys, bonobos, chimpanzees, gorillas, and orangutans) with an identical setup in five well-known geometric illusions (horizontal-vertical, Ebbinghaus, Mueller-Lyer, Ponzo, and Sander). Two food items of identical size were presented on separate trays with surrounding paintings eliciting the illusion of size differences and subjects were required to choose one of the items. Four of the five illusions elicited a strong effect in adult humans, and older children showed a greater susceptibility to illusions than younger ones. In contrast, only two illusions (Ebbingaus and horizontal-vertical) elicited a mild effect on nonhuman primates with high variation within species and little variation between species. Our results suggests that humans learn to see illusions as they develop during childhood. They also suggest that future work should address how nonhuman primates' experience of these illusion changes throughout their development. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2023 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":54861,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Comparative Psychology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.4,"publicationDate":"2023-05-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9614895","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
Vocal repertoire and auditory sensitivity of white-throated woodrats (Neotoma albigula). 白喉木鼠的声乐曲目和听觉敏感性。
IF 1.4 4区 心理学 Q2 Medicine Pub Date : 2023-05-01 DOI: 10.1037/com0000330
Anastasiya Kobrina, Mariah E Letowt, Bret Pasch

Rodents produce a variety of acoustic signals to communicate different types of information such as identity, reproductive state, or danger. The degree to which hearing sensitivity matches particular frequencies of conspecific vocalizations may provide insight into the relative importance of different acoustic signals. In this experiment, we characterized vocal and footdrumming behaviors of white-throated woodrats (Neotoma albigula) and measured their hearing sensitivity using the auditory brainstem response. Adult and juvenile woodrats produced seven categories of vocalizations, with six categories containing frequencies that overlap their peak hearing sensitivity. In addition, woodrats produced low-frequency footdrumming signals in the presence of same- and opposite-sex social partners and in social isolation. Woodrats varied spectral and temporal characteristics of vocalizations based on social composition of the dyad. Woodrat audition spanned 1 to 42 kHz, with a broad range of best hearing sensitivity between 4 and 20 kHz. Compared to other rodents that primarily produce high-frequency vocalizations in social contexts, woodrat audition was more sensitive to low frequencies that typify their vocal repertoire. Our results suggest that the auditory system of white-throated woodrats is broadly tuned to detect behaviorally relevant acoustic signals. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2023 APA, all rights reserved).

啮齿类动物产生各种各样的声音信号来交流不同类型的信息,如身份、繁殖状态或危险。听觉灵敏度与同一发声的特定频率相匹配的程度,可能有助于了解不同声音信号的相对重要性。在本实验中,我们对白喉木鼠(Neotoma albigula)的发声和跺脚行为进行了表征,并利用听觉脑干反应测量了它们的听觉灵敏度。成年和幼年木鼠产生了七种发声类型,其中六种类型的频率与它们的峰值听觉灵敏度重叠。此外,木鼠在有同性和异性社会伙伴在场以及社会孤立的情况下都会发出低频的跺脚信号。木鼠根据双元组的社会组成变化了发声的光谱和时间特征。Woodrat试听范围为1至42千赫,最佳听力灵敏度范围在4至20千赫之间。与其他主要在社会环境中产生高频发声的啮齿动物相比,木鼠的听觉对低频更敏感,这是它们发声曲目的典型特征。我们的研究结果表明,白喉木鼠的听觉系统被广泛地调谐以检测与行为相关的声音信号。(PsycInfo数据库记录(c) 2023 APA,版权所有)。
{"title":"Vocal repertoire and auditory sensitivity of white-throated woodrats (Neotoma albigula).","authors":"Anastasiya Kobrina,&nbsp;Mariah E Letowt,&nbsp;Bret Pasch","doi":"10.1037/com0000330","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/com0000330","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Rodents produce a variety of acoustic signals to communicate different types of information such as identity, reproductive state, or danger. The degree to which hearing sensitivity matches particular frequencies of conspecific vocalizations may provide insight into the relative importance of different acoustic signals. In this experiment, we characterized vocal and footdrumming behaviors of white-throated woodrats (<i>Neotoma albigula</i>) and measured their hearing sensitivity using the auditory brainstem response. Adult and juvenile woodrats produced seven categories of vocalizations, with six categories containing frequencies that overlap their peak hearing sensitivity. In addition, woodrats produced low-frequency footdrumming signals in the presence of same- and opposite-sex social partners and in social isolation. Woodrats varied spectral and temporal characteristics of vocalizations based on social composition of the dyad. Woodrat audition spanned 1 to 42 kHz, with a broad range of best hearing sensitivity between 4 and 20 kHz. Compared to other rodents that primarily produce high-frequency vocalizations in social contexts, woodrat audition was more sensitive to low frequencies that typify their vocal repertoire. Our results suggest that the auditory system of white-throated woodrats is broadly tuned to detect behaviorally relevant acoustic signals. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2023 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":54861,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Comparative Psychology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.4,"publicationDate":"2023-05-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9614897","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
A reflection on noncognitive factors affecting spatial cognitive testing: Examples from nonmodel species. 影响空间认知测试的非认知因素的反思:以非模式物种为例。
IF 1.4 4区 心理学 Q2 Medicine Pub Date : 2023-05-01 DOI: 10.1037/com0000325
Lara D LaDage, Victoria A Gould, Jennifer P Sturgill, Brian D Holsinger, Tracie E Cobb Irvin

Probing for spatial cognitive processes in model rodent species has a long history in the psychological literature, with well-established protocols and paradigms successfully revealing the mechanisms underlying spatial learning and memory. There has also been much interest in examining the ecological and evolutionary context of spatial cognition, with a focus on how selection has molded spatial cognitive abilities in nonmodel species, how spatial cognitive traits vary across species, the neural mechanisms underlying spatial cognitive abilities, and the fitness outcomes of spatial cognition. Behavioral ecologists have been able to take advantage of paradigms from experimental psychology's rich history of spatial cognitive testing for use in nonmodel species. However, as the field advances, it is important to highlight noncognitive factors that can impact performance on spatial cognitive tasks (e.g., motivation to perform the task, switching navigational strategies, variation across protocols, ecological relevance of the task), as these factors may explain discrepancies in findings among some studies. This review highlights how these noncognitive factors can differentially modulate performance on spatial cognitive tests in different nonmodel species. Accounting for these factors when creating protocols and paradigms allows for a more nuanced approach with more explanatory power when probing for spatial cognitive abilities in nonmodel species. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2023 APA, all rights reserved).

在心理学文献中,对模型啮齿动物空间认知过程的探索有着悠久的历史,已经建立了完善的协议和范式,成功地揭示了空间学习和记忆的机制。研究空间认知的生态和进化背景也引起了人们的兴趣,重点是选择如何塑造非模式物种的空间认知能力,不同物种的空间认知特征如何变化,空间认知能力的神经机制以及空间认知的适应性结果。行为生态学家已经能够利用实验心理学丰富的空间认知测试历史中的范式,将其用于非模式物种。然而,随着该领域的发展,重要的是要强调可能影响空间认知任务表现的非认知因素(例如,执行任务的动机,切换导航策略,协议之间的变化,任务的生态相关性),因为这些因素可能解释一些研究结果之间的差异。这篇综述强调了这些非认知因素如何在不同的非模式物种中不同地调节空间认知测试的表现。在创建协议和范式时考虑到这些因素,可以在探索非模式物种的空间认知能力时采用更细致的方法,具有更强的解释力。(PsycInfo数据库记录(c) 2023 APA,版权所有)。
{"title":"A reflection on noncognitive factors affecting spatial cognitive testing: Examples from nonmodel species.","authors":"Lara D LaDage,&nbsp;Victoria A Gould,&nbsp;Jennifer P Sturgill,&nbsp;Brian D Holsinger,&nbsp;Tracie E Cobb Irvin","doi":"10.1037/com0000325","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/com0000325","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Probing for spatial cognitive processes in model rodent species has a long history in the psychological literature, with well-established protocols and paradigms successfully revealing the mechanisms underlying spatial learning and memory. There has also been much interest in examining the ecological and evolutionary context of spatial cognition, with a focus on how selection has molded spatial cognitive abilities in nonmodel species, how spatial cognitive traits vary across species, the neural mechanisms underlying spatial cognitive abilities, and the fitness outcomes of spatial cognition. Behavioral ecologists have been able to take advantage of paradigms from experimental psychology's rich history of spatial cognitive testing for use in nonmodel species. However, as the field advances, it is important to highlight noncognitive factors that can impact performance on spatial cognitive tasks (e.g., motivation to perform the task, switching navigational strategies, variation across protocols, ecological relevance of the task), as these factors may explain discrepancies in findings among some studies. This review highlights how these noncognitive factors can differentially modulate performance on spatial cognitive tests in different nonmodel species. Accounting for these factors when creating protocols and paradigms allows for a more nuanced approach with more explanatory power when probing for spatial cognitive abilities in nonmodel species. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2023 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":54861,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Comparative Psychology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.4,"publicationDate":"2023-05-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9618695","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Assessing the perception of face pareidolia in children (Homo sapiens), rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta), and capuchin monkeys (Sapajus apella). 评估儿童(智人)、恒河猴(猕猴)和卷尾猴(apajus apella)对面部幻想性视错觉的感知。
IF 1.4 4区 心理学 Q2 Medicine Pub Date : 2023-05-01 DOI: 10.1037/com0000320
Molly Flessert, Jessica Taubert, Michael J Beran

Face pareidolia is the misperception of a face in an inanimate object and is a common feature of the face detection system in humans. Whereas there are many similarities in how humans and nonhuman animals such as monkeys perceive and respond to faces, it is still unclear whether other species also perceive certain nonface stimuli as faces. We presented a novel computerized task to capuchin monkeys (Sapajus apella), rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta), and preschool-aged children (Homo sapiens). This task trained subjects to choose faces over nonface images, and then presented pareidolia images with nonface images. All species selected faces most often on trials that included face images. However, only children selected pareidolia images at levels above chance. These results indicate that while children report perceiving face pareidolia, monkeys do not. These species differences could be due to human-unique experiences that result in an increased aptitude for anthropomorphizing objects with face-like patterns. It could also be due to monkeys showing a greater reliance on stimulus features rather than global, holistically organized cues that faces provide. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2023 APA, all rights reserved).

面部视错觉是对无生命物体的面部错误感知,是人类面部检测系统的共同特征。尽管人类和猴子等非人类动物对人脸的感知和反应有许多相似之处,但其他物种是否也将某些非人脸刺激视为人脸,目前尚不清楚。我们向卷尾猴(Sapajus apella)、恒河猴(Macaca mulatta)和学龄前儿童(智人)提出了一项新的计算机化任务。该任务训练受试者在非人脸图像中选择人脸,然后用非人脸图像呈现空想性视错觉图像。在包含人脸图像的试验中,所有物种最常选择人脸。然而,只有儿童在高于偶然性的水平上选择了空想性视错觉图像。这些结果表明,虽然儿童报告感知到面部幻觉,但猴子没有。这些物种差异可能是由于人类独特的经历,导致人类更倾向于将类似人脸的物体拟人化。这也可能是由于猴子更依赖于刺激特征,而不是面部提供的全局、整体组织的线索。(PsycInfo数据库记录(c) 2023 APA,版权所有)。
{"title":"Assessing the perception of face pareidolia in children (Homo sapiens), rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta), and capuchin monkeys (Sapajus apella).","authors":"Molly Flessert,&nbsp;Jessica Taubert,&nbsp;Michael J Beran","doi":"10.1037/com0000320","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/com0000320","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Face pareidolia is the misperception of a face in an inanimate object and is a common feature of the face detection system in humans. Whereas there are many similarities in how humans and nonhuman animals such as monkeys perceive and respond to faces, it is still unclear whether other species also perceive certain nonface stimuli as faces. We presented a novel computerized task to capuchin monkeys (<i>Sapajus apella</i>), rhesus monkeys <i>(Macaca mulatta</i>), and preschool-aged children (<i>Homo sapiens</i>). This task trained subjects to choose faces over nonface images, and then presented pareidolia images with nonface images. All species selected faces most often on trials that included face images. However, only children selected pareidolia images at levels above chance. These results indicate that while children report perceiving face pareidolia, monkeys do not. These species differences could be due to human-unique experiences that result in an increased aptitude for anthropomorphizing objects with face-like patterns. It could also be due to monkeys showing a greater reliance on stimulus features rather than global, holistically organized cues that faces provide. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2023 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":54861,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Comparative Psychology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.4,"publicationDate":"2023-05-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9616121","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 7
Incorporating animal agency into research design could improve behavioral and neuroscience research. 将动物代理纳入研究设计可以改善行为和神经科学研究。
IF 1.4 4区 心理学 Q2 Medicine Pub Date : 2023-05-01 DOI: 10.1037/com0000335
Cédric Sueur, Sarah Zanaz, Marie Pelé

Despite increasing numbers of publications showing that many animals possess the neural substrates involved in emotions and consciousness and exhibit agency in their behavior, many animals are still restrained and forced to take part in applied or fundamental research. However, these restraints and procedures, because they stress animals and limit the expression of adaptive behavior, may result in compromised findings. Researchers should alter their research paradigms to understand the mechanisms and functions of the brain and behavior so that the paradigms incorporate animals' agency. This article discusses how animal agency cannot only be the key to more wide-ranging and improved research in existing domains but can also lead to new research questions about behavior and brain evolution. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2023 APA, all rights reserved).

尽管越来越多的出版物表明,许多动物具有参与情绪和意识的神经基质,并在其行为中表现出代理能力,但许多动物仍然受到限制并被迫参与应用或基础研究。然而,这些限制和程序,因为它们给动物施加压力,限制了适应性行为的表达,可能导致妥协的结果。研究人员应该改变他们的研究范式,以理解大脑和行为的机制和功能,使范式纳入动物的能动性。本文讨论了动物代理如何不仅是在现有领域进行更广泛和改进研究的关键,而且还可以导致有关行为和大脑进化的新研究问题。(PsycInfo数据库记录(c) 2023 APA,版权所有)。
{"title":"Incorporating animal agency into research design could improve behavioral and neuroscience research.","authors":"Cédric Sueur,&nbsp;Sarah Zanaz,&nbsp;Marie Pelé","doi":"10.1037/com0000335","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/com0000335","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Despite increasing numbers of publications showing that many animals possess the neural substrates involved in emotions and consciousness and exhibit agency in their behavior, many animals are still restrained and forced to take part in applied or fundamental research. However, these restraints and procedures, because they stress animals and limit the expression of adaptive behavior, may result in compromised findings. Researchers should alter their research paradigms to understand the mechanisms and functions of the brain and behavior so that the paradigms incorporate animals' agency. This article discusses how animal agency cannot only be the key to more wide-ranging and improved research in existing domains but can also lead to new research questions about behavior and brain evolution. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2023 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":54861,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Comparative Psychology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.4,"publicationDate":"2023-05-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9616420","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 2
I am as fooled as you are, say some primates … but only sometimes. 一些灵长类动物说,我和你一样被愚弄,但只是有时候。
IF 1.4 4区 心理学 Q2 Medicine Pub Date : 2023-05-01 DOI: 10.1037/com0000354
Michael J Beran

The evidence of "cognitive impenetrability" is a byproduct of the fact that minds often must react quickly to sensory stimulation, and they must attempt to make visual stimuli meaningful given what the perceiver knows of the world. Hanus et al. remind us that such immediate decisions may, in fact, help keep us alive, but at the possible cost of sometimes misaligning visual perception and physical reality. That said, not all people fall prey to all illusions, and many individuals may only fall prey to some illusions, but not others. A big question is why this happens. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2023 APA, all rights reserved).

“认知不可穿透性”的证据是这样一个事实的副产品,即大脑通常必须对感官刺激做出快速反应,并且他们必须尝试使视觉刺激有意义,因为感知者对世界的了解。Hanus等人提醒我们,这样的即时决定实际上可以帮助我们活下去,但有时可能会以视觉感知和物理现实不一致为代价。也就是说,并不是所有的人都会成为所有幻想的牺牲品,许多人可能只会成为某些幻想的牺牲品,而不会成为其他幻想的牺牲品。一个大问题是为什么会发生这种情况。(PsycInfo数据库记录(c) 2023 APA,版权所有)。
{"title":"I am as fooled as you are, say some primates … but only sometimes.","authors":"Michael J Beran","doi":"10.1037/com0000354","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/com0000354","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The evidence of \"cognitive impenetrability\" is a byproduct of the fact that minds often must react quickly to sensory stimulation, and they must attempt to make visual stimuli meaningful given what the perceiver knows of the world. Hanus et al. remind us that such immediate decisions may, in fact, help keep us alive, but at the possible cost of sometimes misaligning visual perception and physical reality. That said, not all people fall prey to all illusions, and many individuals may only fall prey to some illusions, but not others. A big question is why this happens. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2023 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":54861,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Comparative Psychology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.4,"publicationDate":"2023-05-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9616525","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Assessing cats' (Felis catus) sensitivity to human pointing gestures. 评估猫(Felis catus)对人类手势的敏感度。
IF 1.4 4区 心理学 Q2 Medicine Pub Date : 2023-02-01 DOI: 10.1037/com0000338
Margaret Mäses, Claudia A F Wascher

A wide range of nonhuman animal species has been shown to be able to respond to human referential signals, such as pointing gestures. The aim of the present study was to replicate previous findings showing cats to be sensitive to human pointing cues (Miklósi et al., 2005). In our study, we presented two types of human pointing gestures-momentary ipsilateral (direct pointing) and momentary cross-body pointing. We tested nine rescue cats in a two-way object-choice task. On a group level, the success rate of cats was 74.4%. Cats performed significantly above chance level in both the ipsilateral and cross-body pointing conditions. Trial number, rewarded side, and type of gesture did not significantly affect the cats' performances in the experiment. On an individual level, five out of seven cats who completed 20 trials performed significantly above chance level. Two cats only completed 10 trials. One of them succeeded in eight and the other in six of these trials. The results of our study replicate previous findings of cats being responsive to human ipsilateral pointing cues and add additional knowledge about their ability to follow cross-body pointing cues. Our results highlight that a domestic species, socialized in a group setting, may possess heterospecific communication skills. Further research is needed to exclude alternative parsimonious explanations, such as local and stimulus enhancements. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2023 APA, all rights reserved).

许多非人类的动物物种已经被证明能够对人类的参考信号做出反应,比如指向的手势。本研究的目的是重复先前的研究结果,即猫对人类的指示线索很敏感(Miklósi et al., 2005)。在我们的研究中,我们提出了两种类型的人类指向手势-瞬间同侧(直接指向)和瞬间跨体指向。我们在一个双向对象选择任务中测试了九只救援猫。在群体层面上,猫的成功率为74.4%。猫在同侧和跨体指向条件下的表现明显高于机会水平。试验次数、奖励方和手势类型对猫在实验中的表现没有显著影响。在个体层面上,完成20次试验的7只猫中有5只的表现明显高于概率水平。两只猫只完成了10次试验。其中一个在8次试验中成功,另一个在6次试验中成功。我们的研究结果重复了先前的发现,即猫对人类同侧指向线索有反应,并增加了关于它们遵循跨体指向线索的能力的额外知识。我们的研究结果强调,在群体环境中社会化的家养物种可能具有异种的沟通技巧。需要进一步的研究来排除其他的简约解释,比如局部和刺激增强。(PsycInfo数据库记录(c) 2023 APA,版权所有)。
{"title":"Assessing cats' (Felis catus) sensitivity to human pointing gestures.","authors":"Margaret Mäses,&nbsp;Claudia A F Wascher","doi":"10.1037/com0000338","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/com0000338","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>A wide range of nonhuman animal species has been shown to be able to respond to human referential signals, such as pointing gestures. The aim of the present study was to replicate previous findings showing cats to be sensitive to human pointing cues (Miklósi et al., 2005). In our study, we presented two types of human pointing gestures-momentary ipsilateral (direct pointing) and momentary cross-body pointing. We tested nine rescue cats in a two-way object-choice task. On a group level, the success rate of cats was 74.4%. Cats performed significantly above chance level in both the ipsilateral and cross-body pointing conditions. Trial number, rewarded side, and type of gesture did not significantly affect the cats' performances in the experiment. On an individual level, five out of seven cats who completed 20 trials performed significantly above chance level. Two cats only completed 10 trials. One of them succeeded in eight and the other in six of these trials. The results of our study replicate previous findings of cats being responsive to human ipsilateral pointing cues and add additional knowledge about their ability to follow cross-body pointing cues. Our results highlight that a domestic species, socialized in a group setting, may possess heterospecific communication skills. Further research is needed to exclude alternative parsimonious explanations, such as local and stimulus enhancements. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2023 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":54861,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Comparative Psychology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.4,"publicationDate":"2023-02-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9287705","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Detouring while foraging up a tree: What bull ants (Myrmecia midas) learn and their reactions to novel sensory cues. 在树上觅食时绕道而行:牛蚁(Myrmecia midas)学习什么以及它们对新感官线索的反应。
IF 1.4 4区 心理学 Q2 Medicine Pub Date : 2023-02-01 DOI: 10.1037/com0000333
Muzahid Islam, Sudhakar Deeti, Zakia Mahmudah, J Frances Kamhi, Ken Cheng

Many animals navigate in a structurally complex environment, which requires them to detour around the physical barriers that they encounter. Although many studies in animal cognition suggest that they are able to adeptly avoid obstacles, it is unclear whether a new route is learned to navigate around these barriers and, if so, what sensory information may be used to do so. We investigated detour learning in traveling up a tree in the Australian bull ant, Myrmecia midas, which primarily uses visual landmarks. We first placed a barrier on the ants' upward path. Initially, 46% of foragers were unsuccessful in detouring the obstacle. On subsequent trips, the ants became more successful and established a new route. We observed up to eight successful foraging trips detouring around the barrier. We then tested the same foragers in a series of manipulations, including changing the position of the barrier, making a new gap in the middle of the obstacle, or removing the barrier altogether. The ants mostly showed the same learned motor routine, detouring with a similar path as in the initial trials, suggesting that foragers were not relying on barrier cues and therefore learned a new route around the obstacle. When foragers encountered new olfactory or tactile cues, or the visual environment was blocked; however, their navigation was profoundly disrupted. These results suggest that changing sensory information drastically affects the foragers' navigational performance. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2023 APA, all rights reserved).

许多动物在结构复杂的环境中导航,这就要求它们绕过遇到的物理障碍。尽管许多关于动物认知的研究表明,它们能够熟练地避开障碍物,但尚不清楚它们是否学会了一条绕过这些障碍物的新路线,如果是的话,它们可能会利用什么感官信息来做到这一点。我们研究了澳大利亚牛蚁(Myrmecia midas)爬树时的绕路学习,它们主要使用视觉地标。我们首先在蚂蚁向上的路上设置了一个障碍物。一开始,46%的觅食者没有成功绕过障碍物。在随后的旅行中,蚂蚁变得更加成功,并建立了一条新的路线。我们观察到多达8次成功的觅食之旅绕过屏障。然后,我们对同样的觅食者进行了一系列的操作测试,包括改变障碍物的位置,在障碍物中间制造一个新的间隙,或者完全移除障碍物。这些蚂蚁大多表现出了相同的习得的运动规律,与最初的试验中相似的路径绕行,这表明觅食者不依赖于障碍物的线索,因此学会了绕过障碍物的新路线。当觅食者遇到新的嗅觉或触觉线索,或视觉环境受阻时;然而,他们的航行被彻底打乱了。这些结果表明,感官信息的改变极大地影响了觅食者的导航表现。(PsycInfo数据库记录(c) 2023 APA,版权所有)。
{"title":"Detouring while foraging up a tree: What bull ants (Myrmecia midas) learn and their reactions to novel sensory cues.","authors":"Muzahid Islam,&nbsp;Sudhakar Deeti,&nbsp;Zakia Mahmudah,&nbsp;J Frances Kamhi,&nbsp;Ken Cheng","doi":"10.1037/com0000333","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/com0000333","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Many animals navigate in a structurally complex environment, which requires them to detour around the physical barriers that they encounter. Although many studies in animal cognition suggest that they are able to adeptly avoid obstacles, it is unclear whether a new route is learned to navigate around these barriers and, if so, what sensory information may be used to do so. We investigated detour learning in traveling up a tree in the Australian bull ant, Myrmecia midas, which primarily uses visual landmarks. We first placed a barrier on the ants' upward path. Initially, 46% of foragers were unsuccessful in detouring the obstacle. On subsequent trips, the ants became more successful and established a new route. We observed up to eight successful foraging trips detouring around the barrier. We then tested the same foragers in a series of manipulations, including changing the position of the barrier, making a new gap in the middle of the obstacle, or removing the barrier altogether. The ants mostly showed the same learned motor routine, detouring with a similar path as in the initial trials, suggesting that foragers were not relying on barrier cues and therefore learned a new route around the obstacle. When foragers encountered new olfactory or tactile cues, or the visual environment was blocked; however, their navigation was profoundly disrupted. These results suggest that changing sensory information drastically affects the foragers' navigational performance. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2023 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":54861,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Comparative Psychology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.4,"publicationDate":"2023-02-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9287704","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
Sound order discrimination in two species of birds-Taeniopygia guttata and Melopsittacus undulatus. 两种鸟类——古塔带翅鸟和波状斑翅鸟的有序区分。
IF 1.4 4区 心理学 Q2 Medicine Pub Date : 2023-02-01 DOI: 10.1037/com0000340
Katherine A Stennette, Adam Fishbein, Nora Prior, Gregory F Ball, Robert J Dooling

Recent psychophysical experiments have shown that zebra finches (Taeniopygia guttata-a songbird) are surprisingly insensitive to syllable sequence changes in their species-specific motifs while budgerigars (Melopsittacus undulatus-a psittacine) do much better when tested on exactly the same sounds. This is unexpected since zebra finch males learn the order of syllables in their songs when young and sing the same song throughout adulthood. Here we probe the limits of this species difference by testing birds on an order change involving just two syllables, hereafter called bi-syllable phrases. Results show budgerigars still perform better than zebra finches on an order change involving just two syllables. An analysis of response latencies shows that both species respond to an order change in a bi-syllable motif at the onset of the first syllable rather than listening to the entire sequence before responding. Additional tests with one syllable omitted or doubled, or with white noise bursts substituted for syllables, indicate that the first syllable in the sequence has a dominant effect on subsequent discrimination of changes in a bi-syllable pattern. These results are surprising in that zebra finch males sing their full motif syllable sequence with a high degree of stereotypy throughout life, suggesting that this consistency in production may not rely on perceptual mechanisms for processing syllable order in adulthood. Budgerigars, on the other hand, are quite sensitive to bi-syllable order changes, an ability that may be related to useful information being encoded in the sequence of syllables in their natural song. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2023 APA, all rights reserved).

最近的心理物理实验表明,斑马雀(Taeniopygia guttata-一种鸣禽)对其物种特有的音节序列变化不敏感,而鹦鹉(Melopsittacus波动-一种鹦鹉)在对完全相同的声音进行测试时表现得更好。这是出乎意料的,因为雄性斑胸草雀在年轻时就学会了歌曲中的音节顺序,并在成年后唱同一首歌。在这里,我们通过测试鸟类在仅涉及两个音节的顺序变化(以下称为双音节短语)来探索这种物种差异的极限。结果显示,在涉及两个音节的顺序变化上,虎皮鹦鹉仍然比斑胸草雀表现得更好。对反应潜伏期的分析表明,这两个物种都对双音节基序的第一个音节开始的顺序变化作出反应,而不是在反应前听整个序列。省略或加倍一个音节,或用白噪音代替音节的额外测试表明,序列中的第一个音节对随后对双音节模式变化的辨别具有主要影响。这些结果令人惊讶,因为雄性斑胸草雀在一生中以高度刻板的方式唱出完整的母基音节序列,这表明这种一致性的生产可能不依赖于成年期处理音节顺序的感知机制。另一方面,虎皮鹦鹉对双音节顺序的变化非常敏感,这种能力可能与它们自然鸣叫中的音节序列中编码的有用信息有关。(PsycInfo数据库记录(c) 2023 APA,版权所有)。
{"title":"Sound order discrimination in two species of birds-Taeniopygia guttata and Melopsittacus undulatus.","authors":"Katherine A Stennette,&nbsp;Adam Fishbein,&nbsp;Nora Prior,&nbsp;Gregory F Ball,&nbsp;Robert J Dooling","doi":"10.1037/com0000340","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/com0000340","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Recent psychophysical experiments have shown that zebra finches (Taeniopygia guttata-a songbird) are surprisingly insensitive to syllable sequence changes in their species-specific motifs while budgerigars (Melopsittacus undulatus-a psittacine) do much better when tested on exactly the same sounds. This is unexpected since zebra finch males learn the order of syllables in their songs when young and sing the same song throughout adulthood. Here we probe the limits of this species difference by testing birds on an order change involving just two syllables, hereafter called bi-syllable phrases. Results show budgerigars still perform better than zebra finches on an order change involving just two syllables. An analysis of response latencies shows that both species respond to an order change in a bi-syllable motif at the onset of the first syllable rather than listening to the entire sequence before responding. Additional tests with one syllable omitted or doubled, or with white noise bursts substituted for syllables, indicate that the first syllable in the sequence has a dominant effect on subsequent discrimination of changes in a bi-syllable pattern. These results are surprising in that zebra finch males sing their full motif syllable sequence with a high degree of stereotypy throughout life, suggesting that this consistency in production may not rely on perceptual mechanisms for processing syllable order in adulthood. Budgerigars, on the other hand, are quite sensitive to bi-syllable order changes, an ability that may be related to useful information being encoded in the sequence of syllables in their natural song. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2023 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":54861,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Comparative Psychology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.4,"publicationDate":"2023-02-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9234076","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Journal of Comparative Psychology
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1