首页 > 最新文献

Journal of Comparative Psychology最新文献

英文 中文
Simplicity and complexity in human and nonhuman communication. 人类与非人类交流的简单与复杂。
IF 1.4 4区 心理学 Q4 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2022-11-01 DOI: 10.1037/com0000334
Todd M Freeberg

Comments on an article by Limor Raviv et al. (see record 2023-07345-001). Raviv et al. argue that the conflicting findings from human language and from studies of communication in nonhuman animals boil down to different levels of analysis used by researchers studying non-humans compared with those studying humans. Researchers studying nonhuman animal communication typically focus on the size of signal repertoires or the structural variation within and among signals within a repertoire. Researchers studying human language, conversely, largely focus on the question of grammatical rules that govern the way units (words and phrases) are put together in speech streams. Rules of composition that govern the way units are put together are considered more complex in nonhuman signaling systems, but simpler in human language systems. The discrepancy here, according to Raviv et al., stems from two sources. According to the commenting authors, the take home message of Raviv et al. is one that will be helpful to future studies of the evolution of communication systems. Raviv et al. recommend that we work harder to avoid terms such as "complex" and "simple" with regard to communication and instead focus on the specifics of what we are analyzing. Phrases such as "larger repertoire size" or "stronger compositional structure" represent cleaner and more neutral phrases for discussions of communication and would better allow findings from non-humans and from humans to be compared. Finally, Raviv et al. advocate for greater collaborative work across nonhuman and human communication systems, and interdisciplinary work has a long history of fundamental discoveries. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA, all rights reserved).

对Limor Raviv等人的文章的评论(见记录2023-07345-001)。拉维夫等人认为,来自人类语言和非人类动物交流研究的相互矛盾的发现,归结为研究非人类的研究人员与研究人类的研究人员使用了不同的分析水平。研究非人类动物交流的研究人员通常关注信号库的大小或信号库内部和之间的结构变化。相反,研究人类语言的研究人员主要关注的是语法规则问题,这些规则决定了单元(单词和短语)在语音流中的组合方式。控制单元组合方式的组合规则在非人类信号系统中被认为更复杂,但在人类语言系统中更简单。根据拉维夫等人的说法,这种差异源于两个方面。根据评论作者的说法,Raviv等人的关键信息将有助于未来对通信系统进化的研究。Raviv等人建议我们更努力地避免使用诸如“复杂”和“简单”之类的术语,而是专注于我们正在分析的细节。“更大的曲目规模”或“更强的作曲结构”等短语代表了讨论交流时更清晰和更中立的短语,并且可以更好地比较来自非人类和人类的发现。最后,Raviv等人提倡在非人类和人类交流系统之间进行更大的合作,跨学科的工作有着悠久的基础发现历史。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA,版权所有)。
{"title":"Simplicity and complexity in human and nonhuman communication.","authors":"Todd M Freeberg","doi":"10.1037/com0000334","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/com0000334","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Comments on an article by Limor Raviv et al. (see record 2023-07345-001). Raviv et al. argue that the conflicting findings from human language and from studies of communication in nonhuman animals boil down to different levels of analysis used by researchers studying non-humans compared with those studying humans. Researchers studying nonhuman animal communication typically focus on the size of signal repertoires or the structural variation within and among signals within a repertoire. Researchers studying human language, conversely, largely focus on the question of grammatical rules that govern the way units (words and phrases) are put together in speech streams. Rules of composition that govern the way units are put together are considered more complex in nonhuman signaling systems, but simpler in human language systems. The discrepancy here, according to Raviv et al., stems from two sources. According to the commenting authors, the take home message of Raviv et al. is one that will be helpful to future studies of the evolution of communication systems. Raviv et al. recommend that we work harder to avoid terms such as \"complex\" and \"simple\" with regard to communication and instead focus on the specifics of what we are analyzing. Phrases such as \"larger repertoire size\" or \"stronger compositional structure\" represent cleaner and more neutral phrases for discussions of communication and would better allow findings from non-humans and from humans to be compared. Finally, Raviv et al. advocate for greater collaborative work across nonhuman and human communication systems, and interdisciplinary work has a long history of fundamental discoveries. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":54861,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Comparative Psychology","volume":"136 4","pages":"213-214"},"PeriodicalIF":1.4,"publicationDate":"2022-11-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10474440","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Recognition of visual kinship signals in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) by humans (Homo sapiens). 人类(智人)对黑猩猩(类人猿)视觉亲缘关系信号的识别。
IF 1.4 4区 心理学 Q4 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2022-11-01 DOI: 10.1037/com0000327
Hella Péter, Marion Laporte, Nicholas E Newton-Fisher, Vernon Reynolds, Liran Samuni, Adrian Soldati, Linda Vigilant, Jakob Villioth, Kirsty E Graham, Klaus Zuberbühler, Catherine Hobaiter

Associating with kin provides individual benefits but requires that these relationships be detectable. In humans, facial phenotype matching might help assess paternity; however, evidence for it is mixed. In chimpanzees, concealing visual cues of paternity may be beneficial due to their promiscuous mating system and the considerable risk of infanticide by males. On the other hand, detecting kin can also aid chimpanzees in avoiding inbreeding and in forming alliances that improve kin-mediated fitness. Although previous studies assessing relatedness based on facial resemblance in chimpanzees exist, they used images of captive populations in whom selection pressures and reproductive opportunities are controlled and only assessed maternity or paternity of adult offspring. In natural populations, the chances of infanticide are highest during early infancy, suggesting that young infants would benefit most from paternity concealment, whereas adults and subadults would benefit from the detection of all types of kin, including half-siblings. In our experiment, we conducted an online study with human participants, in which they had to assess the relatedness of chimpanzees based on facial similarity. To address previous methodological constraints, we used chimpanzee images across all ages, as well as maternal and paternal half-siblings. We found that kin status was detected above chance across all relatedness categories, with easier kin detection of father-offspring pairs, females, and older chimpanzees. Together, these findings support the existence of paternity confusion in infant chimpanzees and provide a possible mechanism for incest avoidance and kin-based social alliances in older individuals. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA, all rights reserved).

与亲缘关系提供了个人利益,但要求这些关系是可检测的。在人类中,面部表型匹配可能有助于评估亲子关系;然而,证据是混杂的。在黑猩猩中,隐藏父权的视觉线索可能是有益的,因为它们的交配系统是滥交的,而且雄性杀婴的风险相当大。另一方面,发现亲缘关系也可以帮助黑猩猩避免近亲繁殖,形成联盟,提高亲缘关系介导的适应性。虽然以前的研究是基于黑猩猩的面部相似性来评估亲缘关系的,但他们使用的是圈养种群的图像,在这些种群中,选择压力和繁殖机会受到控制,并且只评估了成年后代的母性或父性。在自然种群中,杀婴的几率在婴儿早期是最高的,这表明年幼的婴儿将从父权隐瞒中获益最多,而成年人和亚成年人将从所有类型的亲属(包括同父异母的兄弟姐妹)的检测中受益。在我们的实验中,我们对人类参与者进行了一项在线研究,在这项研究中,他们必须根据面部相似性来评估黑猩猩的亲缘关系。为了解决之前的方法限制,我们使用了所有年龄段的黑猩猩图像,以及母亲和父亲的同父异母兄弟姐妹。我们发现,在所有亲缘关系类别中,亲缘关系状态的检测高于偶然,父系后代对、雌性黑猩猩和老年黑猩猩的亲缘关系检测更容易。总之,这些发现支持了幼年黑猩猩中父权混淆的存在,并提供了一种可能的机制,以避免乱伦和在老年个体中建立基于亲属的社会联盟。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA,版权所有)。
{"title":"Recognition of visual kinship signals in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) by humans (Homo sapiens).","authors":"Hella Péter,&nbsp;Marion Laporte,&nbsp;Nicholas E Newton-Fisher,&nbsp;Vernon Reynolds,&nbsp;Liran Samuni,&nbsp;Adrian Soldati,&nbsp;Linda Vigilant,&nbsp;Jakob Villioth,&nbsp;Kirsty E Graham,&nbsp;Klaus Zuberbühler,&nbsp;Catherine Hobaiter","doi":"10.1037/com0000327","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/com0000327","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Associating with kin provides individual benefits but requires that these relationships be detectable. In humans, facial phenotype matching might help assess paternity; however, evidence for it is mixed. In chimpanzees, concealing visual cues of paternity may be beneficial due to their promiscuous mating system and the considerable risk of infanticide by males. On the other hand, detecting kin can also aid chimpanzees in avoiding inbreeding and in forming alliances that improve kin-mediated fitness. Although previous studies assessing relatedness based on facial resemblance in chimpanzees exist, they used images of captive populations in whom selection pressures and reproductive opportunities are controlled and only assessed maternity or paternity of adult offspring. In natural populations, the chances of infanticide are highest during early infancy, suggesting that young infants would benefit most from paternity concealment, whereas adults and subadults would benefit from the detection of all types of kin, including half-siblings. In our experiment, we conducted an online study with human participants, in which they had to assess the relatedness of chimpanzees based on facial similarity. To address previous methodological constraints, we used chimpanzee images across all ages, as well as maternal and paternal half-siblings. We found that kin status was detected above chance across all relatedness categories, with easier kin detection of father-offspring pairs, females, and older chimpanzees. Together, these findings support the existence of paternity confusion in infant chimpanzees and provide a possible mechanism for incest avoidance and kin-based social alliances in older individuals. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":54861,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Comparative Psychology","volume":"136 4","pages":"255-269"},"PeriodicalIF":1.4,"publicationDate":"2022-11-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10824147","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Children (Homo sapiens), but not rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta), perceive the one-is-more illusion. 儿童(智人),而不是恒河猴(猕猴),能感知“一比多”的错觉。
IF 1.4 4区 心理学 Q4 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2022-11-01 DOI: 10.1037/com0000316
Emma J McKeon, Michael J Beran, Audrey E Parrish

Visual illusions are of particular interest to cognitive researchers because they reflect the active role of the brain in processing the world around us. Yousif and Scholl (2019) recently described a new visual experience, the one-is-more illusion, in which adult humans perceived continuous objects as longer than sets of discrete objects of equal length. In the current study, we investigated this phenomenon in human children (Homo sapiens) and rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta). Children were presented with a computerized 2-choice discrimination task and successfully selected the longer of 2 images for control trials. On trials in which 2 versions of the same image were presented (identical in length), and one was of a continuous form and the other consisted of 2 or more distinct units, children showed a bias for the continuous object. Monkeys were given the same computerized task and learned to choose the longer of 2 otherwise identical stimuli. However, monkeys did not show a bias to choose the continuous probe images as longer than the discrete images in the critical test trials with equal-length stimuli. These results are discussed in light of developmental and comparative research on related illusory experiences and perceptual mechanisms. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA, all rights reserved).

认知研究者对视觉错觉特别感兴趣,因为它们反映了大脑在处理我们周围世界时的活跃作用。Yousif和Scholl(2019)最近描述了一种新的视觉体验,即一比多的错觉,在这种错觉中,成年人认为连续的物体比一组长度相等的离散物体更长。在本研究中,我们调查了人类儿童(智人)和恒河猴(Macaca mulatta)的这一现象。孩子们被提供了一个计算机化的两项选择辨别任务,并成功地选择了两幅图像中较长的一幅作为对照试验。在相同图像的两个版本(长度相同),一个是连续的形式,另一个由2个或更多不同的单元组成的试验中,儿童表现出对连续物体的偏见。给猴子们同样的电脑任务,让他们学会在两个完全相同的刺激中选择时间较长的那个。然而,在等长度刺激的关键测试试验中,猴子没有表现出选择连续探针图像比选择离散图像更长的偏见。本文结合相关幻觉体验和知觉机制的发展和比较研究对这些结果进行了讨论。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA,版权所有)。
{"title":"Children (Homo sapiens), but not rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta), perceive the one-is-more illusion.","authors":"Emma J McKeon,&nbsp;Michael J Beran,&nbsp;Audrey E Parrish","doi":"10.1037/com0000316","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/com0000316","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Visual illusions are of particular interest to cognitive researchers because they reflect the active role of the brain in processing the world around us. Yousif and Scholl (2019) recently described a new visual experience, the one-is-more illusion, in which adult humans perceived continuous objects as longer than sets of discrete objects of equal length. In the current study, we investigated this phenomenon in human children (<i>Homo</i> <i>sapiens</i>) and rhesus macaques (<i>Macaca mulatta</i>). Children were presented with a computerized 2-choice discrimination task and successfully selected the longer of 2 images for control trials. On trials in which 2 versions of the same image were presented (identical in length), and one was of a continuous form and the other consisted of 2 or more distinct units, children showed a bias for the continuous object. Monkeys were given the same computerized task and learned to choose the longer of 2 otherwise identical stimuli. However, monkeys did not show a bias to choose the continuous probe images as longer than the discrete images in the critical test trials with equal-length stimuli. These results are discussed in light of developmental and comparative research on related illusory experiences and perceptual mechanisms. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":54861,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Comparative Psychology","volume":"136 4","pages":"270-278"},"PeriodicalIF":1.4,"publicationDate":"2022-11-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10471708","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
Consistent second-order motor planning by cotton-top tamarins (Saguinus oedipus): Evidence from a dowel task. 棉顶绢毛猴(俄狄浦斯绢毛猴)一致的二级运动规划:来自一个固定任务的证据。
IF 1.4 4区 心理学 Q4 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2022-11-01 DOI: 10.1037/com0000331
Natalie Schwob, Ricky Groner, Amy L Lebkuecher, Sylvia Rudnicki, Daniel J Weiss

One of the hallmarks of complex motor planning in humans involves grasping objects in preparation for future actions, termed second-order motor planning. This ability has an extended developmental trajectory in humans and is also shared with nonhuman primates. Here, we presented seven cotton-top tamarins with a dowel task that has prompted variable grasping behaviors for some primate species. Tamarins could use either an efficient grasp to bring food stuck onto the end of a dowel to their mouth (radial grasp) or an inefficient grasp that required repositioning (ulnar grasp). The tamarins were very consistent in their use of radial grasps. These data support the morphological constraint theory suggesting that species with limited dexterity (inability to perform precision grasps) may demonstrate more consistent second-order motor planning due to the increased cost of inefficient grasping postures. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA, all rights reserved).

人类复杂运动规划的特征之一是为未来的行动准备抓握物体,称为二阶运动规划。这种能力在人类中有很长的发展轨迹,非人类灵长类动物也有这种能力。在这里,我们展示了七只棉顶绢毛猴的一个定位任务,这个任务已经提示了一些灵长类物种的不同抓取行为。绢毛猴可以用一种有效的抓握来把食物粘在木桩的末端送到嘴里(径向抓握),也可以用一种低效的抓握来重新定位(尺骨抓握)。绢毛猴使用径向抓握的方式非常一致。这些数据支持形态约束理论,表明灵巧度有限的物种(无法进行精确抓取)可能由于低效抓取姿势的成本增加而表现出更一致的二阶运动规划。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA,版权所有)。
{"title":"Consistent second-order motor planning by cotton-top tamarins (Saguinus oedipus): Evidence from a dowel task.","authors":"Natalie Schwob,&nbsp;Ricky Groner,&nbsp;Amy L Lebkuecher,&nbsp;Sylvia Rudnicki,&nbsp;Daniel J Weiss","doi":"10.1037/com0000331","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/com0000331","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>One of the hallmarks of complex motor planning in humans involves grasping objects in preparation for future actions, termed second-order motor planning. This ability has an extended developmental trajectory in humans and is also shared with nonhuman primates. Here, we presented seven cotton-top tamarins with a dowel task that has prompted variable grasping behaviors for some primate species. Tamarins could use either an efficient grasp to bring food stuck onto the end of a dowel to their mouth (radial grasp) or an inefficient grasp that required repositioning (ulnar grasp). The tamarins were very consistent in their use of radial grasps. These data support the morphological constraint theory suggesting that species with limited dexterity (inability to perform precision grasps) may demonstrate more consistent second-order motor planning due to the increased cost of inefficient grasping postures. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":54861,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Comparative Psychology","volume":"136 4","pages":"279-284"},"PeriodicalIF":1.4,"publicationDate":"2022-11-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10474441","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Mother-infant relationships and infant independence in wild Geoffroy's spider monkeys (Ateles geoffroyi). 野生Geoffroy蜘蛛猴的母子关系和婴儿独立性。
IF 1.4 4区 心理学 Q4 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2022-11-01 DOI: 10.1037/com0000329
Ana Lucía Arbaiza-Bayona, Colleen M Schaffner, Germán Gutiérrez, Filippo Aureli

We studied mother-infant relationships and infant independence in wild Geoffroy's spider monkeys (Ateles geoffroyi) during the first 3 years of infant life. We used 15-min focal sampling to collect data on mother-infant interactions and infant behavior in 12 mother-infant dyads in the Yucatan peninsula, Mexico. Data were analyzed using generalized linear mixed models. Newborns spent almost all their time in proximity and in contact with their mothers. The time infants spent within one-arm reach from the mother decreased with age, and the infant was primarily responsible for maintaining proximity. The time infants spent farther than 8 m from the mother, in independent locomotion, exploration, and proximity with group members other than the mother increased with age. We identified two developmental periods associated with critical milestones of infant independence: the first at 8 to 10 months when independent locomotion and exploration began, and the proportion of time in proximity with group members other than the mother increased and the second at 19 to 21 months when the mother's rejection started, maternal carrying ended and mother's help, in the form of bridging canopy gaps, peaked. Compared with other primate species of similar size, Ateles geoffroyi have an extended dependence period, which could be related to the cognitive and developmental challenges imposed by their socioecological characteristics. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA, all rights reserved).

我们研究了野生Geoffroy蜘蛛猴(Ateles geoffroyi)在婴儿生命的前3年里的母子关系和婴儿独立性。我们采用15分钟的焦点抽样方法收集了墨西哥尤卡坦半岛12对母子的母婴互动和婴儿行为数据。数据分析采用广义线性混合模型。新生儿几乎所有的时间都和母亲在一起。婴儿在母亲单臂可及范围内的时间随着年龄的增长而减少,婴儿主要负责保持与母亲的接近。随着年龄的增长,婴儿在距离母亲超过8米的地方进行独立运动、探索和接近母亲以外的群体成员的时间增加。我们确定了两个与婴儿独立的关键里程碑相关的发育时期:第一个是在8到10个月时,当母亲开始独立运动和探索时,与母亲以外的群体成员接近的时间比例增加;第二个是在19到21个月时,母亲开始排斥,母亲的携带结束,母亲的帮助(以弥合树冠间隙的形式)达到顶峰。与其他类似大小的灵长类物种相比,geoffroyi的依赖期较长,这可能与其社会生态特征带来的认知和发育挑战有关。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA,版权所有)。
{"title":"Mother-infant relationships and infant independence in wild Geoffroy's spider monkeys (Ateles geoffroyi).","authors":"Ana Lucía Arbaiza-Bayona,&nbsp;Colleen M Schaffner,&nbsp;Germán Gutiérrez,&nbsp;Filippo Aureli","doi":"10.1037/com0000329","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/com0000329","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>We studied mother-infant relationships and infant independence in wild Geoffroy's spider monkeys (<i>Ateles geoffroyi</i>) during the first 3 years of infant life. We used 15-min focal sampling to collect data on mother-infant interactions and infant behavior in 12 mother-infant dyads in the Yucatan peninsula, Mexico. Data were analyzed using generalized linear mixed models. Newborns spent almost all their time in proximity and in contact with their mothers. The time infants spent within one-arm reach from the mother decreased with age, and the infant was primarily responsible for maintaining proximity. The time infants spent farther than 8 m from the mother, in independent locomotion, exploration, and proximity with group members other than the mother increased with age. We identified two developmental periods associated with critical milestones of infant independence: the first at 8 to 10 months when independent locomotion and exploration began, and the proportion of time in proximity with group members other than the mother increased and the second at 19 to 21 months when the mother's rejection started, maternal carrying ended and mother's help, in the form of bridging canopy gaps, peaked. Compared with other primate species of similar size, <i>Ateles geoffroyi</i> have an extended dependence period, which could be related to the cognitive and developmental challenges imposed by their socioecological characteristics. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":54861,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Comparative Psychology","volume":"136 4","pages":"221-235"},"PeriodicalIF":1.4,"publicationDate":"2022-11-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10474444","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
Dominance in human (Homo sapiens) personality space and in hominoid phylogeny. 人类(智人)人格空间和类人猿系统发育中的支配地位。
IF 1.4 4区 心理学 Q4 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2022-11-01 DOI: 10.1037/com0000322
Alexander Weiss

Unlike nonhuman primates, individual differences between humans in dominance do not appear as broad personality factors. This may be attributable to differences between the questionnaires used to study human and nonhuman primate personality. Alternatively, this may reflect differences in the organization of personality in humans and nonhuman primates. To determine which of these possibilities was most likely, 1,147 participants were recruited and asked to rate their personality and/or that of somebody else on the Hominoid Personality Questionnaire (HPQ), which has been used to study nonhuman primate personality. A large subset of these participants (~80%) also completed self- and/or rater reports of one of three questionnaires used to measure human personality. Exploratory factor analyses of HPQ rater report data yielded five factors. These factors correlated mostly in expected ways with scales from questionnaires used to study human personality. Exploratory factor analyses of HPQ self-report data yielded no clear number of factors and no consistent evidence with respect to the presence of a dominance factor. Subsequent analyses compared HPQ scales that represented dominance factors in chimpanzees, bonobos, mountain gorillas, and orangutans to scales derived from the Revised NEO Personality Inventory, including Fearless Dominance, which combined Neuroticism, Agreeableness, Conscientiousness, and Extraversion facets, Emotional Stability (the inverse of Neuroticism), and Extraversion's Assertiveness facet. Fearless Dominance and Assertiveness were most like the great ape dominance factors. The absence of human dominance factors, therefore, appears to reflect present or past social conditions of our species. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA, all rights reserved).

与非人类灵长类动物不同,人类在支配地位方面的个体差异并不表现为广泛的人格因素。这可能是由于用于研究人类和非人类灵长类动物性格的问卷存在差异。或者,这可能反映了人类和非人类灵长类动物在人格组织上的差异。为了确定哪一种可能性最大,研究人员招募了1147名参与者,并要求他们在类人猿人格问卷(HPQ)上对自己和/或其他人的人格进行打分,该问卷已被用于研究非人类灵长类动物的人格。这些参与者中的很大一部分(约80%)还完成了用于测量人类性格的三份问卷中的一份自我和/或评分报告。对HPQ评分报告数据进行探索性因素分析,得出五个因素。这些因素与用于研究人类个性的问卷中的尺度主要以预期的方式相关。对HPQ自述数据的探索性因素分析没有得出明确的因素数量,也没有一致的证据表明存在优势因素。随后的分析将代表黑猩猩、倭黑猩猩、山地大猩猩和猩猩的优势因素的HPQ量表与来自修订NEO人格量表的量表进行了比较,其中包括无畏的优势,它结合了神经质、随和性、尽责性和外向性方面、情绪稳定性(与神经质相反)和外向性的自信方面。无所畏惧的优势和自信最接近类人猿的优势因素。因此,人类主导因素的缺失似乎反映了我们这个物种现在或过去的社会状况。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA,版权所有)。
{"title":"Dominance in human (Homo sapiens) personality space and in hominoid phylogeny.","authors":"Alexander Weiss","doi":"10.1037/com0000322","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/com0000322","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Unlike nonhuman primates, individual differences between humans in dominance do not appear as broad personality factors. This may be attributable to differences between the questionnaires used to study human and nonhuman primate personality. Alternatively, this may reflect differences in the organization of personality in humans and nonhuman primates. To determine which of these possibilities was most likely, 1,147 participants were recruited and asked to rate their personality and/or that of somebody else on the Hominoid Personality Questionnaire (HPQ), which has been used to study nonhuman primate personality. A large subset of these participants (~80%) also completed self- and/or rater reports of one of three questionnaires used to measure human personality. Exploratory factor analyses of HPQ rater report data yielded five factors. These factors correlated mostly in expected ways with scales from questionnaires used to study human personality. Exploratory factor analyses of HPQ self-report data yielded no clear number of factors and no consistent evidence with respect to the presence of a dominance factor. Subsequent analyses compared HPQ scales that represented dominance factors in chimpanzees, bonobos, mountain gorillas, and orangutans to scales derived from the Revised NEO Personality Inventory, including Fearless Dominance, which combined Neuroticism, Agreeableness, Conscientiousness, and Extraversion facets, Emotional Stability (the inverse of Neuroticism), and Extraversion's Assertiveness facet. Fearless Dominance and Assertiveness were most like the great ape dominance factors. The absence of human dominance factors, therefore, appears to reflect present or past social conditions of our species. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":54861,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Comparative Psychology","volume":"136 4","pages":"236-254"},"PeriodicalIF":1.4,"publicationDate":"2022-11-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10531690","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 2
What is simple is actually quite complex: A critical note on terminology in the domain of language and communication. 简单的东西实际上很复杂:语言和交流领域术语的关键注释。
IF 1.4 4区 心理学 Q4 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2022-11-01 DOI: 10.1037/com0000328
Limor Raviv, Louise R Peckre, Cedric Boeckx

On the surface, the fields of animal communication and human linguistics have arrived at conflicting theories and conclusions with respect to the effect of social complexity on communicative complexity. For example, an increase in group size is argued to have opposite consequences on human versus animal communication systems: although an increase in human community size leads to some types of language simplification, an increase in animal group size leads to an increase in signal complexity. But do human and animal communication systems really show such a fundamental discrepancy? Our key message is that the tension between these two adjacent fields is the result of (a) a focus on different levels of analysis (namely, signal variation or grammar-like rules) and (b) an inconsistent use of terminology (namely, the terms "simple" and "complex"). By disentangling and clarifying these terms with respect to different measures of communicative complexity, we show that although animal and human communication systems indeed show some contradictory effects with respect to signal variability, they actually display essentially the same patterns with respect to grammar-like structure. This is despite the fact that the definitions of complexity and simplicity are actually aligned for signal variability, but diverge for grammatical structure. We conclude by advocating for the use of more objective and descriptive terms instead of terms such as "complexity," which can be applied uniformly for human and animal communication systems-leading to comparable descriptions of findings across species and promoting a more productive dialogue between fields. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA, all rights reserved).

从表面上看,关于社会复杂性对交际复杂性的影响,动物交际领域和人类语言学领域已经得出了相互矛盾的理论和结论。例如,人们认为群体规模的增加会对人类和动物的交流系统产生相反的影响:尽管人类群体规模的增加会导致某些类型的语言简化,但动物群体规模的增加会导致信号复杂性的增加。但是,人类和动物的交流系统真的表现出如此根本的差异吗?我们的关键信息是,这两个相邻领域之间的紧张关系是(a)关注不同层次的分析(即信号变化或类似语法的规则)和(b)术语使用不一致(即术语“简单”和“复杂”)的结果。通过将这些术语从不同的交流复杂性度量中分离出来并加以澄清,我们表明,尽管动物和人类的交流系统在信号变异性方面确实表现出一些相互矛盾的影响,但它们实际上在类似语法的结构方面表现出本质上相同的模式。尽管事实上,复杂性和简单性的定义实际上是一致的信号变异性,但分歧的语法结构。最后,我们提倡使用更客观和描述性的术语,而不是像“复杂性”这样的术语,因为“复杂性”可以统一应用于人类和动物的交流系统,从而导致跨物种发现的可比描述,并促进领域之间更有成效的对话。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA,版权所有)。
{"title":"What is simple is actually quite complex: A critical note on terminology in the domain of language and communication.","authors":"Limor Raviv,&nbsp;Louise R Peckre,&nbsp;Cedric Boeckx","doi":"10.1037/com0000328","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/com0000328","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>On the surface, the fields of animal communication and human linguistics have arrived at conflicting theories and conclusions with respect to the effect of social complexity on communicative complexity. For example, an increase in group size is argued to have opposite consequences on human versus animal communication systems: although an increase in human community size leads to some types of language simplification, an increase in animal group size leads to an increase in signal complexity. But do human and animal communication systems really show such a fundamental discrepancy? Our key message is that the tension between these two adjacent fields is the result of (a) a focus on different levels of analysis (namely, signal variation or grammar-like rules) and (b) an inconsistent use of terminology (namely, the terms \"simple\" and \"complex\"). By disentangling and clarifying these terms with respect to different measures of communicative complexity, we show that although animal and human communication systems indeed show some contradictory effects with respect to signal variability, they actually display essentially the same patterns with respect to grammar-like structure. This is despite the fact that the definitions of complexity and simplicity are actually aligned for signal variability, but diverge for grammatical structure. We conclude by advocating for the use of more objective and descriptive terms instead of terms such as \"complexity,\" which can be applied uniformly for human and animal communication systems-leading to comparable descriptions of findings across species and promoting a more productive dialogue between fields. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":54861,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Comparative Psychology","volume":"136 4","pages":"215-220"},"PeriodicalIF":1.4,"publicationDate":"2022-11-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10839707","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 6
Supplemental Material for Same-Different Conceptualization in Dogs (Canis familiaris) 犬类同异概念化补充材料
IF 1.4 4区 心理学 Q4 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2022-10-24 DOI: 10.1037/com0000332.supp
{"title":"Supplemental Material for Same-Different Conceptualization in Dogs (Canis familiaris)","authors":"","doi":"10.1037/com0000332.supp","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/com0000332.supp","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":54861,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Comparative Psychology","volume":"10 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":1.4,"publicationDate":"2022-10-24","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"85108016","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Supplemental Material for Investigation of Mirror-Self Recognition in Ravens (Corvus corax) 乌鸦(Corvus corax)镜像自我识别研究补充材料
IF 1.4 4区 心理学 Q4 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2022-06-27 DOI: 10.1037/com0000319.supp
{"title":"Supplemental Material for Investigation of Mirror-Self Recognition in Ravens (Corvus corax)","authors":"","doi":"10.1037/com0000319.supp","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/com0000319.supp","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":54861,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Comparative Psychology","volume":"22 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":1.4,"publicationDate":"2022-06-27","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"87550901","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
Sensitivity to line-of-sight in tolerant versus despotic macaques (Macaca sylvanus and Macaca mulatta). 宽容猕猴与专制猕猴(猕猴和猕猴)对视线的敏感性。
IF 1.4 4区 心理学 Q4 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2022-05-01 DOI: 10.1037/com0000309
Rosemary Bettle, Alexandra G Rosati
Complex social life is considered important to the evolution of cognition in primates. One key aspect of primate social interactions concerns the degree of competition that individuals face in their social group. To examine how social tolerance versus competition shapes social cognition, we experimentally assessed capacities for flexible gaze-following in more tolerant Barbary macaques (Macaca sylvanus) and compared to previous data from despotic rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta). Monkeys experienced one of two possible conditions. In the barrier condition, they observed an actor look upwards into an overheard barrier, so they could not directly see the target of the actor's gaze without reorienting. In the no barrier condition, they observed an actor look upwards without a barrier blocking her line-of-sight, so they could observe the target of the actor's gaze by also looking upwards. Both species (N = 58 Barbary macaques, 64 rhesus macaques) could flexibly modulate their gaze responses to account for the demonstrator's line of sight, looking up more often when no barrier was present, and this flexible modulation declined with age in both species. However, neither species preferentially approached to look inside the barrier when their view of the target location was obscured, although rhesus macaques approached more overall. This pattern suggests that both tolerant and despotic macaques exhibit similar capacities to track other's line of sight and do not preferentially reorient their bodies to observe what an actor looks at in this situation. This contrasts with other work indicating that competitive primates are especially adept at some aspects of theory of mind. Thus, it is important to understand both the similarities and differences in the social-cognitive abilities of primates with different social styles. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA, all rights reserved).
复杂的社会生活被认为对灵长类动物的认知进化很重要。灵长类动物社会互动的一个关键方面是个体在其社会群体中面临的竞争程度。为了研究社会宽容与竞争是如何塑造社会认知的,我们通过实验评估了更具宽容的巴巴里猕猴(Macaca sylvanus)灵活注视跟随的能力,并与之前来自专制恒河猕猴(Macaca mulatta)的数据进行了比较。猴子经历了两种可能的情况之一。在屏障条件下,他们观察到一个演员向上看一个偷听的屏障,所以他们不能直接看到演员的目光的目标,而不改变方向。在无障碍条件下,他们观察到演员向上看,没有障碍挡住她的视线,所以他们可以通过向上看来观察演员凝视的目标。这两个物种(N = 58只巴巴里猕猴,64只恒河猴)都可以灵活地调节它们的凝视反应,以解释演示者的视线,在没有障碍物的情况下更频繁地抬头,这种灵活的调节在这两个物种中都随着年龄的增长而下降。然而,当目标位置被遮挡时,这两个物种都不会优先接近障碍物内部,尽管恒河猴更全面地接近。这种模式表明,宽容和专制的猕猴都表现出类似的追踪他人视线的能力,在这种情况下,它们不会优先调整自己的身体来观察演员在看什么。这与其他表明竞争性灵长类动物特别擅长心理理论某些方面的研究形成了对比。因此,了解不同社会类型的灵长类动物社会认知能力的异同是十分重要的。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA,版权所有)。
{"title":"Sensitivity to line-of-sight in tolerant versus despotic macaques (Macaca sylvanus and Macaca mulatta).","authors":"Rosemary Bettle,&nbsp;Alexandra G Rosati","doi":"10.1037/com0000309","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/com0000309","url":null,"abstract":"Complex social life is considered important to the evolution of cognition in primates. One key aspect of primate social interactions concerns the degree of competition that individuals face in their social group. To examine how social tolerance versus competition shapes social cognition, we experimentally assessed capacities for flexible gaze-following in more tolerant Barbary macaques (Macaca sylvanus) and compared to previous data from despotic rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta). Monkeys experienced one of two possible conditions. In the barrier condition, they observed an actor look upwards into an overheard barrier, so they could not directly see the target of the actor's gaze without reorienting. In the no barrier condition, they observed an actor look upwards without a barrier blocking her line-of-sight, so they could observe the target of the actor's gaze by also looking upwards. Both species (N = 58 Barbary macaques, 64 rhesus macaques) could flexibly modulate their gaze responses to account for the demonstrator's line of sight, looking up more often when no barrier was present, and this flexible modulation declined with age in both species. However, neither species preferentially approached to look inside the barrier when their view of the target location was obscured, although rhesus macaques approached more overall. This pattern suggests that both tolerant and despotic macaques exhibit similar capacities to track other's line of sight and do not preferentially reorient their bodies to observe what an actor looks at in this situation. This contrasts with other work indicating that competitive primates are especially adept at some aspects of theory of mind. Thus, it is important to understand both the similarities and differences in the social-cognitive abilities of primates with different social styles. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA, all rights reserved).","PeriodicalId":54861,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Comparative Psychology","volume":"136 2","pages":"93-104"},"PeriodicalIF":1.4,"publicationDate":"2022-05-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC9851130/pdf/nihms-1862451.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9110397","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Journal of Comparative Psychology
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1