首页 > 最新文献

acta ethologica最新文献

英文 中文
Feeding behavior and prey of three migratory shorebirds (Aves: Charadriiformes) during the nonbreeding season in southern Brazil 巴西南部三种候鸟(鸟类:Charadriiformes)在非繁殖季节的捕食行为和猎物
IF 1.1 4区 生物学 Q4 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2023-10-16 DOI: 10.1007/s10211-023-00427-3
Jenny A. Angarita-Báez, Caio J. Carlos

Migratory shorebirds such as Calidris alba, C. fuscicollis, and Charadrius semipalmatus congregate in stopover areas like coastal wetlands to rest and feed, building up sufficient energy for their ongoing migration. To investigate the feeding ecology of these three shorebird species during their stopover, we conducted a comparative analysis of their feeding behavior and prey in Lagoa do Peixe National Park. We examined the feeding behavior using video recordings of 594 actively foraging individuals. Additionally, we determined the shorebirds' diet by analyzing 106 droppings collected from two areas within the park: the beach's intertidal zone and the lagoon's mudflats. The results highlighted that shorebird species showed marked differences in feeding strategies and prey captured during foraging. C. semipalmatus employed a visual-run-stop strategy with surface pecking, with no significant variations observed between the beach and the lagoon. The two Calidris species utilized a tactile-continuous hunting strategy, involving pecking and probing, with some variations observed between the beach and the lagoon. Multiple probing was mostly used on the beach, and single probing in the lagoon. The variation in probing behavior between the Calidris species appeared to be associated with differences in substrate type and food availability in the respective habitats they frequented. Additionally, our findings indicated that visual searching led to a more diverse prey selection, particularly in the lagoon. These differences in foraging strategies suggest that shorebird species can exploit the park's intertidal plains and trophic resources differently, emphasizing the importance of considering spatial and dietary variability in studying their foraging behavior.

迁徙的海岸鸟类(如白杓鹬(Calidris alba)、红杓鹬(C. fuscicollis)和半杓鹬(Charadrius semipalmatus))会聚集在海岸湿地等中途停留区休息和觅食,为继续迁徙积累足够的能量。为了研究这三种岸鸟在停留期间的觅食生态,我们对它们在裴斯湖国家公园的觅食行为和猎物进行了比较分析。我们通过对 594 只积极觅食的个体进行录像,研究了它们的觅食行为。此外,我们还通过分析从公园内两个区域(海滩潮间带和泻湖泥滩)收集到的 106 个粪便,确定了岸鸟的食性。结果表明,岸鸟物种在觅食策略和觅食过程中捕获的猎物方面存在明显差异。C.semipalmatus采用目视-奔跑-停止的策略,在水面啄食,在海滩和泻湖之间没有观察到明显的差异。两种杓鹬采用触觉连续捕食策略,包括啄食和探测,在海滩和泻湖之间观察到一些差异。在海滩上主要使用多次探测,而在泻湖中则使用单次探测。杓鹬物种之间探测行为的差异似乎与它们各自常去的栖息地的底质类型和食物供应情况的不同有关。此外,我们的研究结果表明,视觉搜索导致猎物选择更加多样化,尤其是在泻湖中。这些觅食策略的差异表明,岸鸟物种可以以不同的方式利用公园的潮间带平原和营养资源,强调了在研究岸鸟觅食行为时考虑空间和食物变化的重要性。
{"title":"Feeding behavior and prey of three migratory shorebirds (Aves: Charadriiformes) during the nonbreeding season in southern Brazil","authors":"Jenny A. Angarita-Báez,&nbsp;Caio J. Carlos","doi":"10.1007/s10211-023-00427-3","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s10211-023-00427-3","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Migratory shorebirds such as <i>Calidris alba</i>, <i>C. fuscicollis</i>, and <i>Charadrius semipalmatus</i> congregate in stopover areas like coastal wetlands to rest and feed, building up sufficient energy for their ongoing migration. To investigate the feeding ecology of these three shorebird species during their stopover, we conducted a comparative analysis of their feeding behavior and prey in Lagoa do Peixe National Park. We examined the feeding behavior using video recordings of 594 actively foraging individuals. Additionally, we determined the shorebirds' diet by analyzing 106 droppings collected from two areas within the park: the beach's intertidal zone and the lagoon's mudflats. The results highlighted that shorebird species showed marked differences in feeding strategies and prey captured during foraging. <i>C. semipalmatus</i> employed a visual-run-stop strategy with surface pecking, with no significant variations observed between the beach and the lagoon. The two <i>Calidris</i> species utilized a tactile-continuous hunting strategy, involving pecking and probing, with some variations observed between the beach and the lagoon. Multiple probing was mostly used on the beach, and single probing in the lagoon. The variation in probing behavior between the <i>Calidris</i> species appeared to be associated with differences in substrate type and food availability in the respective habitats they frequented. Additionally, our findings indicated that visual searching led to a more diverse prey selection, particularly in the lagoon. These differences in foraging strategies suggest that shorebird species can exploit the park's intertidal plains and trophic resources differently, emphasizing the importance of considering spatial and dietary variability in studying their foraging behavior.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":6879,"journal":{"name":"acta ethologica","volume":"27 1","pages":"27 - 38"},"PeriodicalIF":1.1,"publicationDate":"2023-10-16","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"136078272","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
A possibility of tool use in a Japanese marten, Martes melampus 日本貂使用工具的可能性
IF 1.1 4区 生物学 Q4 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2023-10-12 DOI: 10.1007/s10211-023-00429-1
Kei K Suzuki

Tool use by animals, traditionally studied mainly in captive primates due to its relevance to human evolution, has been investigated in both free-ranging and captive mammals. Here, I present a report on tree bark and branch use behaviours by a free-ranging Japanese marten (Martes melampus). The marten put its face in a water-filled container, looking for something, and then it proceeded to make a scooping motion with a bark held in its mouth. Subsequently, it picked up a branch with its mouth and tried to scoop something from the water with it. Upon inspecting the container approximately 2 weeks later, I found a larva belonging to the family Syrphidae. While it is unclear if the marten was trying to catch this larva, its behaviour resembled reach extension behaviour seen in chimpanzees. The use of bark or branches may provide an advantage by allowing access to objects in hard-to-reach positions.

动物的工具使用传统上主要是在圈养灵长类动物中进行研究,因为这与人类进化有关。在此,我报告了一只自由放养的日本貂(Martes melampus)使用树皮和树枝的行为。这只日本貂把脸伸进一个装满水的容器中寻找东西,然后用嘴叼起树皮做舀水的动作。随后,它又用嘴叼起一根树枝,试图用树枝从水中舀东西。大约两周后,我在检查容器时发现了一只属于蚜蝇科的幼虫。虽然不清楚这只貂是否试图捕捉这只幼虫,但它的行为很像黑猩猩的伸手行为。利用树皮或树枝可能是一种优势,因为这样可以接触到难以触及位置的物体。
{"title":"A possibility of tool use in a Japanese marten, Martes melampus","authors":"Kei K Suzuki","doi":"10.1007/s10211-023-00429-1","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s10211-023-00429-1","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Tool use by animals, traditionally studied mainly in captive primates due to its relevance to human evolution, has been investigated in both free-ranging and captive mammals. Here, I present a report on tree bark and branch use behaviours by a free-ranging Japanese marten (<i>Martes melampus</i>). The marten put its face in a water-filled container, looking for something, and then it proceeded to make a scooping motion with a bark held in its mouth. Subsequently, it picked up a branch with its mouth and tried to scoop something from the water with it. Upon inspecting the container approximately 2 weeks later, I found a larva belonging to the family Syrphidae. While it is unclear if the marten was trying to catch this larva, its behaviour resembled reach extension behaviour seen in chimpanzees. The use of bark or branches may provide an advantage by allowing access to objects in hard-to-reach positions.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":6879,"journal":{"name":"acta ethologica","volume":"27 1","pages":"81 - 84"},"PeriodicalIF":1.1,"publicationDate":"2023-10-12","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"135968181","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Thanatological behavior in striped hyena (Hyaena hyaena) 条纹鬣狗的排泄行为
IF 1.1 4区 生物学 Q4 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2023-10-04 DOI: 10.1007/s10211-023-00428-2
Einat Shteckler, Reuven Yosef

In recent decades, reports on responses to death and dying individuals in non-human animals have been increasing. Here, we report the case of a striped hyena who refrained from eating a juvenile conspecific carcass in its territory. We inferred that the juvenile was an offspring of the individual. Although the hyena discovered the carcass several days before any other conspecific, other than maintaining vigilance over it and sniffing it repeatedly, it did not consume it; only an unfamiliar hyena did so. This is the first documentation of thanatological behavior in striped hyenas.

近几十年来,有关非人类动物对死亡和垂死个体的反应的报道越来越多。在这里,我们报告了一只条纹鬣狗在其领地内不吃同类幼体尸体的案例。我们推断该幼体是该个体的后代。虽然这只鬣狗发现尸体的时间比其他同类早了几天,但除了对尸体保持警惕并反复嗅闻之外,它并没有吃掉尸体,只有一只陌生的鬣狗吃掉了尸体。这是首次记录条纹鬣狗的嗜食行为。
{"title":"Thanatological behavior in striped hyena (Hyaena hyaena)","authors":"Einat Shteckler,&nbsp;Reuven Yosef","doi":"10.1007/s10211-023-00428-2","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s10211-023-00428-2","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>In recent decades, reports on responses to death and dying individuals in non-human animals have been increasing. Here, we report the case of a striped hyena who refrained from eating a juvenile conspecific carcass in its territory. We inferred that the juvenile was an offspring of the individual. Although the hyena discovered the carcass several days before any other conspecific, other than maintaining vigilance over it and sniffing it repeatedly, it did not consume it; only an unfamiliar hyena did so. This is the first documentation of thanatological behavior in striped hyenas.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":6879,"journal":{"name":"acta ethologica","volume":"27 1","pages":"75 - 79"},"PeriodicalIF":1.1,"publicationDate":"2023-10-04","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"135591697","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Reproductive behavior analysis in the annual killifish Austrolebias nigripinnis (Regan, 1912) (Cyprinodontiformes: Rivulidae) 年生黑斑南鳉鱼(Regan, 1912)的繁殖行为分析(鲤形目:鲤科)
IF 1.1 4区 生物学 Q2 Agricultural and Biological Sciences Pub Date : 2023-07-20 DOI: 10.1007/s10211-023-00426-4
Juan Carlos Segovia, Marcelo Loureiro, Daniel García

Annual killifish have an annual life cycle unique among vertebrates. Adults lay desiccation-resistant eggs in the soil of the temporary pools they inhabit. Courtship behavior of some Neotropical killifish consists of a series of displays performed by males and females that ends in the total or partial burial of the couple. However, little information exists on their reproductive isolation mechanisms, including a detailed analysis of courtship signals. In this study, we analyzed the courtship behavior of a Neotropical annual killifish (Austrolebias nigripinnis) within a comparative frame. The most frequent units in males were lateral (44%) and sigmoid displays (26%), where males exhibit morphological and color patterns to females. Females showed a high frequency (44%) of quiescence, suggesting that this unit may have an evaluative role during courtship. Behavioral units recognized in this species show some differences with other related species (i.e., Austrolebias affinis), mainly in frequency and duration, but also in sequence. Although the results of this work cannot determine if these differences constitute reproductive isolation mechanisms, it provides fundamental elements to try to establish kinship relationships and solve the complexity implicit in the definition of species.

一年生鳉鱼在脊椎动物中有独特的一年生周期。成虫在它们栖息的临时池塘的土壤中产下抗干燥的卵。一些新热带鳉鱼的求偶行为由雄性和雌性的一系列表演组成,最终以夫妻全部或部分埋葬而告终。然而,关于其生殖隔离机制的信息很少,包括对求爱信号的详细分析。在这项研究中,我们在比较框架内分析了新热带一年生鳉鱼(Austrolebias nigripinnis)的求偶行为。在男性中最常见的单位是横向显示(44%)和乙状体显示(26%),其中男性表现出对女性的形态和颜色模式。雌性表现出较高的沉默频率(44%),这表明这个单位可能在求偶期间起着评估作用。该物种的行为单元与其他亲缘物种(如Austrolebias affinis)有一定的差异,主要表现在频率和持续时间上,但也表现在顺序上。虽然这项工作的结果不能确定这些差异是否构成生殖隔离机制,但它为试图建立亲属关系和解决物种定义中隐含的复杂性提供了基本要素。
{"title":"Reproductive behavior analysis in the annual killifish Austrolebias nigripinnis (Regan, 1912) (Cyprinodontiformes: Rivulidae)","authors":"Juan Carlos Segovia,&nbsp;Marcelo Loureiro,&nbsp;Daniel García","doi":"10.1007/s10211-023-00426-4","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s10211-023-00426-4","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Annual killifish have an annual life cycle unique among vertebrates. Adults lay desiccation-resistant eggs in the soil of the temporary pools they inhabit. Courtship behavior of some Neotropical killifish consists of a series of displays performed by males and females that ends in the total or partial burial of the couple. However, little information exists on their reproductive isolation mechanisms, including a detailed analysis of courtship signals. In this study, we analyzed the courtship behavior of a Neotropical annual killifish (<i>Austrolebias nigripinnis</i>) within a comparative frame. The most frequent units in males were lateral (44%) and sigmoid displays (26%), where males exhibit morphological and color patterns to females. Females showed a high frequency (44%) of quiescence, suggesting that this unit may have an evaluative role during courtship. Behavioral units recognized in this species show some differences with other related species (i.e., <i>Austrolebias affinis</i>), mainly in frequency and duration, but also in sequence. Although the results of this work cannot determine if these differences constitute reproductive isolation mechanisms, it provides fundamental elements to try to establish kinship relationships and solve the complexity implicit in the definition of species.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":6879,"journal":{"name":"acta ethologica","volume":"26 3","pages":"239 - 246"},"PeriodicalIF":1.1,"publicationDate":"2023-07-20","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"48906553","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Safe in the heights: trees as safer sites for female jaguars and their cubs in the Pantanal 高处安全:在潘塔纳尔,树木是雌性美洲虎和幼崽更安全的地方
IF 1.1 4区 生物学 Q2 Agricultural and Biological Sciences Pub Date : 2023-06-15 DOI: 10.1007/s10211-023-00425-5
Carlos Eduardo Fragoso, Lilian Elaine Rampim, Mario Haberfeld, Henrique Villas Boas Concone, Joares Adenilson May-Júnior

While the use of trees has been widely documented for most of the big cats, this information is scant for the jaguar (Panthera onca). In this study, we investigated the use of trees by jaguars, evaluating different underlying reasons as well as the frequency of tree use based on sex, age, and reproductive status. Data were obtained from 2013 to 2020 through a combination of direct observations during fieldwork and camera trapping focused on this behavior in the Brazilian Pantanal. We documented 252 climbing events (176 direct observations, 76 camera captures). Using only camera trapping data to avoid observers’ influence on jaguar behavior, we fitted generalized linear mixed models (GLMM) with jaguar identity as a random factor to test the effects of age, sex, and presence of conspecifics in the odds of animals climbing trees. We also used a generalized linear model (GLM) to test the effect of adult body weight on jaguar tree climbing probability. Age and presence of conspecifics were the most important drivers of jaguar climbing behavior in the study area, with cubs being more likely to climb trees than adults of either sex (odds ratio = 7.91, p < 0.001), and the presence of conspecifics, irrespective of age and sex, accentuated that behavior (odds ratio = 3.26, p < 0.005). Adult females (especially the lighter ones) and cubs were commonly recorded through direct observation on trees, a similar trend to our GLMM that showed a marginally negative effect of body weight on jaguar tree climbing probability. Only a few adult male jaguars ascended trees, and in all cases, these males were following females in heat. We suggest that trees are vertical extensions inside jaguar home ranges and may be safe refuges for resting and for protecting offspring against potential threats, including the harassment of large adult males.

虽然大多数大型猫科动物都有利用树木的记录,但关于美洲虎(Panthera onca)的信息却很少。在这项研究中,我们调查了美洲豹对树木的使用,评估了不同的潜在原因以及基于性别、年龄和生殖状态的树木使用频率。数据是在2013年至2020年期间通过实地工作期间的直接观察和相机捕获的结合获得的,这些数据集中在巴西潘塔纳尔的这种行为上。我们记录了252次攀登事件(176次直接观察,76次相机捕捉)。为了避免观察者对美洲虎行为的影响,我们仅使用相机捕获数据,将美洲虎身份作为随机因素拟合广义线性混合模型(GLMM),以测试年龄、性别和同种动物的存在对动物爬树几率的影响。我们还利用广义线性模型(GLM)检验了成虫体重对美洲虎爬树概率的影响。年龄和同种个体的存在是研究区域内美洲虎攀爬行为的最重要驱动因素,幼崽比任何性别的成年美洲虎更有可能爬树(优势比= 7.91,p < 0.001),而同种个体的存在,无论年龄和性别,都加剧了这种行为(优势比= 3.26,p < 0.005)。成年雌虎(尤其是较轻的雌虎)和幼虎通常通过在树上的直接观察来记录,这与我们的GLMM相似,表明体重对美洲虎爬树概率有轻微的负影响。只有少数成年雄性美洲虎爬上了树,而且在所有情况下,这些雄性都是在发情期跟随雌性。我们认为树木是美洲虎活动范围内的垂直延伸,可能是休息和保护后代免受潜在威胁的安全避难所,包括大型成年雄性的骚扰。
{"title":"Safe in the heights: trees as safer sites for female jaguars and their cubs in the Pantanal","authors":"Carlos Eduardo Fragoso,&nbsp;Lilian Elaine Rampim,&nbsp;Mario Haberfeld,&nbsp;Henrique Villas Boas Concone,&nbsp;Joares Adenilson May-Júnior","doi":"10.1007/s10211-023-00425-5","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s10211-023-00425-5","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>While the use of trees has been widely documented for most of the big cats, this information is scant for the jaguar (<i>Panthera onca</i>). In this study, we investigated the use of trees by jaguars, evaluating different underlying reasons as well as the frequency of tree use based on sex, age, and reproductive status. Data were obtained from 2013 to 2020 through a combination of direct observations during fieldwork and camera trapping focused on this behavior in the Brazilian Pantanal. We documented 252 climbing events (176 direct observations, 76 camera captures). Using only camera trapping data to avoid observers’ influence on jaguar behavior, we fitted generalized linear mixed models (GLMM) with jaguar identity as a random factor to test the effects of age, sex, and presence of conspecifics in the odds of animals climbing trees. We also used a generalized linear model (GLM) to test the effect of adult body weight on jaguar tree climbing probability. Age and presence of conspecifics were the most important drivers of jaguar climbing behavior in the study area, with cubs being more likely to climb trees than adults of either sex (odds ratio = 7.91, <i>p</i> &lt; 0.001), and the presence of conspecifics, irrespective of age and sex, accentuated that behavior (odds ratio = 3.26, <i>p</i> &lt; 0.005). Adult females (especially the lighter ones) and cubs were commonly recorded through direct observation on trees, a similar trend to our GLMM that showed a marginally negative effect of body weight on jaguar tree climbing probability. Only a few adult male jaguars ascended trees, and in all cases, these males were following females in heat. We suggest that trees are vertical extensions inside jaguar home ranges and may be safe refuges for resting and for protecting offspring against potential threats, including the harassment of large adult males.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":6879,"journal":{"name":"acta ethologica","volume":"26 3","pages":"231 - 238"},"PeriodicalIF":1.1,"publicationDate":"2023-06-15","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"49110053","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Do it like the otter—data on the amphibian skinning behaviour in the Eurasian badger (Meles meles L., 1758) 它像水獭一样吗——欧亚獾两栖动物剥皮行为的数据(Meles Meles L.,1758)
IF 1.1 4区 生物学 Q2 Agricultural and Biological Sciences Pub Date : 2023-05-16 DOI: 10.1007/s10211-023-00424-6
Teodora Koynova, Radoslav Tsvetkov, Nikolay Natchev

Abstract

The European badger Meles meles (Carnivora: Mustelidae) is a widely distributed mammal across Europe. This is a highly adaptive species, which is capable of exploiting a wide variety of habitats and food resources. The European badger is a threatened species, and knowledge on all aspects of its ecology and behaviour may provide important information concerning the environmental factors driving its distribution and population density. In the present study, we report on a specific predatory behaviour in M. meles. At Nature Park “Shumensko plato” (NE Bulgaria), the badgers were detected to feed on common toads (Bufo bufo) and fire salamanders (Salamandra salamandra). Both amphibians are known to produce highly toxic secrets which cover the surface of the skin and are generally avoided as prey by the majority of mammal predators. During the prey manipulation phase of the feeding process, the European badger removed the poisonous skin of the captured amphibians and in most cases consumed the rest of the body (except the eggs of the toads). This skinning behaviour appears to be rather similar to that described in another mustelid—the European otter (Lutra lutra). This behaviour could potentially benefit the badgers in using broader spectrum of food resources, especially in case of limited food availability.

摘要欧洲獾(Meles Meles)是一种广泛分布于欧洲的哺乳动物。这是一个高度适应的物种,能够利用各种各样的栖息地和食物资源。欧洲獾是一种濒危物种,对其生态和行为的各个方面的了解可以提供有关驱动其分布和人口密度的环境因素的重要信息。在本研究中,我们报告了一种特定的捕食行为。在“Shumensko plato”自然公园(保加利亚东北部),人们发现獾以普通蟾蜍(Bufo Bufo)和火蜥蜴(Salamandra Salamandra)为食。众所周知,这两种两栖动物都能产生覆盖在皮肤表面的剧毒分泌物,大多数哺乳动物捕食者通常不会将其作为猎物。在捕食过程中的操纵猎物阶段,欧洲獾将捕获的两栖动物的有毒皮肤去除,在大多数情况下,吃掉身体的其余部分(除了蟾蜍的卵)。这种剥皮行为似乎与另一种鼬科动物——欧洲水獭(Lutra Lutra)的行为相当相似。这种行为可能有利于獾使用更广泛的食物资源,特别是在食物供应有限的情况下。
{"title":"Do it like the otter—data on the amphibian skinning behaviour in the Eurasian badger (Meles meles L., 1758)","authors":"Teodora Koynova,&nbsp;Radoslav Tsvetkov,&nbsp;Nikolay Natchev","doi":"10.1007/s10211-023-00424-6","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s10211-023-00424-6","url":null,"abstract":"<div><h2>Abstract\u0000</h2><div><p>The European badger <i>Meles meles</i> (Carnivora: Mustelidae) is a widely distributed mammal across Europe. This is a highly adaptive species, which is capable of exploiting a wide variety of habitats and food resources. The European badger is a threatened species, and knowledge on all aspects of its ecology and behaviour may provide important information concerning the environmental factors driving its distribution and population density. In the present study, we report on a specific predatory behaviour in <i>M. meles</i>. At Nature Park “Shumensko plato” (NE Bulgaria), the badgers were detected to feed on common toads (<i>Bufo bufo</i>) and fire salamanders (<i>Salamandra salamandra</i>). Both amphibians are known to produce highly toxic secrets which cover the surface of the skin and are generally avoided as prey by the majority of mammal predators. During the prey manipulation phase of the feeding process, the European badger removed the poisonous skin of the captured amphibians and in most cases consumed the rest of the body (except the eggs of the toads). This skinning behaviour appears to be rather similar to that described in another mustelid—the European otter (<i>Lutra lutra</i>). This behaviour could potentially benefit the badgers in using broader spectrum of food resources, especially in case of limited food availability.</p></div></div>","PeriodicalId":6879,"journal":{"name":"acta ethologica","volume":"26 3","pages":"225 - 229"},"PeriodicalIF":1.1,"publicationDate":"2023-05-16","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"46092818","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Novel observation of play behaviour between a harem holder and a bachelor group of Przewalski’s horses in the wild 对一个后宫主人和一群野外普氏野马的单身群之间的游戏行为的新观察
IF 1.1 4区 生物学 Q2 Agricultural and Biological Sciences Pub Date : 2023-05-10 DOI: 10.1007/s10211-023-00421-9
Anna Bernátková, Francisco Ceacero, Ganbaatar Oyunsaikhan, Dalaitseren Sukhbaatar, Jaroslav Šimek, Martina Komárková

Abstract

Przewalski’s horses live in stable nonterritorial families (harem) of one or more harem stallions, several mares, and their offspring. The harem stallion typically behaves aggressively towards any male intruder approaching the harem. Play behaviour is frequently observed among the group members in horses. For stallions, the most common, well-known cases of play behaviour are found between members of bachelor groups (groups of young stallions) and between the harem stallion and his offspring. The play between the harem stallion and members of the bachelor groups is, on the other hand, seemingly much rarer as a description of such events in the literature is anecdotal. In this note, we present our observation conducted in the Great Gobi B Strictly Protected Area (Mongolia) and describe the play behaviour between a Przewalski’s horse harem stallion and members of a bachelor group. The observation was done as a part of broader research during which selected Przewalski’s horses’ groups were located by binoculars in daily monitoring routines and filmed from a close distance (from 150 to 800 m). Behavioural Observation Research Interactive Software (BORIS) was used to extract data from the video recordings. The observation described in this note lasted for ∼ 180 min, during which the horses engaged in three separate bouts of play and repeated inter-group association. During the whole study (241.5 h of video recordings), this was the only observation including inter-group interactions. We observed two other events during which two harems approached each other (but never merged). Our observation is the first video recording of such an event and raises the question of how prevalent this behaviour is in the Przewalski’s horses’ restored natural populations. We conclude that this behaviour is unusual or has not been sufficiently studied in wild or feral harems. Further technological advances may help reveal more information on this topic.

普氏野马生活在由一匹或多匹后宫种马、几匹母马及其后代组成的稳定的无领地家庭(后宫)中。后宫种马通常会对任何接近后宫的男性入侵者表现出攻击性。在马的群体成员中经常观察到游戏行为。对于种马来说,最常见的、众所周知的游戏行为发生在单身群体(年轻的种马群体)的成员之间,以及后宫种马和他的后代之间。另一方面,后宫种马和单身群体成员之间的游戏似乎更罕见,因为文学中对此类事件的描述是轶事。在这篇文章中,我们介绍了我们在大戈壁B严格保护区(蒙古)进行的观察,并描述了普氏野马后宫种马和单身群体成员之间的游戏行为。该观察是更广泛研究的一部分,在此期间,通过双筒望远镜在日常监测程序中选定普氏野马群,并近距离拍摄(150至800米)。使用行为观察研究互动软件(BORIS)从视频记录中提取数据。本笔记中描述的观察持续了约180分钟,在此期间,马进行了三次单独的游戏和重复的组间联系。在整个研究(241.5小时的视频记录)中,这是唯一包括组间互动的观察。我们还观察到另外两个事件,两个后宫彼此接近(但从未合并)。我们的观察是这一事件的第一个视频记录,并提出了一个问题,即这种行为在普氏野马恢复的自然种群中有多普遍。我们的结论是,这种行为是不寻常的,或者在野生或野生的后宫没有得到充分的研究。进一步的技术进步可能有助于揭示有关这一主题的更多信息。
{"title":"Novel observation of play behaviour between a harem holder and a bachelor group of Przewalski’s horses in the wild","authors":"Anna Bernátková,&nbsp;Francisco Ceacero,&nbsp;Ganbaatar Oyunsaikhan,&nbsp;Dalaitseren Sukhbaatar,&nbsp;Jaroslav Šimek,&nbsp;Martina Komárková","doi":"10.1007/s10211-023-00421-9","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s10211-023-00421-9","url":null,"abstract":"<div><h2>Abstract\u0000</h2><div><p>Przewalski’s horses live in stable nonterritorial families (harem) of one or more harem stallions, several mares, and their offspring. The harem stallion typically behaves aggressively towards any male intruder approaching the harem. Play behaviour is frequently observed among the group members in horses. For stallions, the most common, well-known cases of play behaviour are found between members of bachelor groups (groups of young stallions) and between the harem stallion and his offspring. The play between the harem stallion and members of the bachelor groups is, on the other hand, seemingly much rarer as a description of such events in the literature is anecdotal. In this note, we present our observation conducted in the Great Gobi B Strictly Protected Area (Mongolia) and describe the play behaviour between a Przewalski’s horse harem stallion and members of a bachelor group. The observation was done as a part of broader research during which selected Przewalski’s horses’ groups were located by binoculars in daily monitoring routines and filmed from a close distance (from 150 to 800 m). Behavioural Observation Research Interactive Software (BORIS) was used to extract data from the video recordings. The observation described in this note lasted for ∼ 180 min, during which the horses engaged in three separate bouts of play and repeated inter-group association. During the whole study (241.5 h of video recordings), this was the only observation including inter-group interactions. We observed two other events during which two harems approached each other (but never merged). Our observation is the first video recording of such an event and raises the question of how prevalent this behaviour is in the Przewalski’s horses’ restored natural populations. We conclude that this behaviour is unusual or has not been sufficiently studied in wild or feral harems. Further technological advances may help reveal more information on this topic.</p></div></div>","PeriodicalId":6879,"journal":{"name":"acta ethologica","volume":"26 2","pages":"145 - 150"},"PeriodicalIF":1.1,"publicationDate":"2023-05-10","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://link.springer.com/content/pdf/10.1007/s10211-023-00421-9.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"48429975","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Correction to: The environment matters: season and female contact affect the behavior of captive Addax nasomaculatus male antelope 更正:环境问题:季节和雌性接触会影响圈养鼻斑Addax雄性羚羊的行为
IF 1.1 4区 生物学 Q2 Agricultural and Biological Sciences Pub Date : 2023-05-09 DOI: 10.1007/s10211-023-00423-7
Matías Villagrán, Mariana Ceva, Ariane Machiñena, Mariana Perdomo, Lucas Berro, Cesar Echaides, Juan Pablo Damián
{"title":"Correction to: The environment matters: season and female contact affect the behavior of captive Addax nasomaculatus male antelope","authors":"Matías Villagrán,&nbsp;Mariana Ceva,&nbsp;Ariane Machiñena,&nbsp;Mariana Perdomo,&nbsp;Lucas Berro,&nbsp;Cesar Echaides,&nbsp;Juan Pablo Damián","doi":"10.1007/s10211-023-00423-7","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s10211-023-00423-7","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":6879,"journal":{"name":"acta ethologica","volume":"26 2","pages":"119 - 120"},"PeriodicalIF":1.1,"publicationDate":"2023-05-09","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"43029389","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Differences in locomotor performances between two sympatric species of freshwater turtles 两种同域淡水龟运动性能的差异
IF 1.1 4区 生物学 Q2 Agricultural and Biological Sciences Pub Date : 2023-05-06 DOI: 10.1007/s10211-023-00420-w
Ana Golubović, Vukašin Bjelica, Ana Štih Koren

Animals whose ecological niches largely overlap within a habitat can get into intra-specific and/or inter-specific competition for some of the limited resources available. We hypothesise that differences in locomotor abilities might be a key component deciding which individual will gain the resource, especially in animals with limited agility, such as turtles. To address this hypothesis, we tested self-righting ability and releasing from a vegetation alike rope in two sympatric populations of turtles: Emys orbicularis and Mauremys rivulata. All individuals managed to complete the tests promptly (≤ 103 s). When overturned on their carapace, E. orbicularis had shorter inspecting time, as well as overturning time, indicating slight advantage in self-righting. On the other hand, both species were equally efficient to release from a rope attached to the front of their plastron. The inter-specific morphological differences indicate that muscular strength might be a good predictor of competitive relations among syntopic turtle species.

如果动物的生态位在一个栖息地内有很大的重叠,它们就会为争夺一些有限的可用资源而进入种内和/或种间竞争。我们假设,运动能力的差异可能是决定哪一个个体将获得资源的关键因素,尤其是在敏捷性有限的动物中,比如乌龟。为了验证这一假设,我们测试了两种同域的海龟种群:圆尾龟和河尾龟的自我矫正能力和从类似植被的绳子上释放的能力。所有个体都能迅速完成测试(≤103 s)。当被翻倒在甲壳上时,圆轮蝶的检查时间更短,翻倒时间也更短,在自扶正方面略有优势。另一方面,这两个物种从绑在它们板前面的绳子上释放的效率是一样的。种间形态差异表明,肌肉力量可能是合鳖种间竞争关系的一个很好的预测指标。
{"title":"Differences in locomotor performances between two sympatric species of freshwater turtles","authors":"Ana Golubović,&nbsp;Vukašin Bjelica,&nbsp;Ana Štih Koren","doi":"10.1007/s10211-023-00420-w","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s10211-023-00420-w","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Animals whose ecological niches largely overlap within a habitat can get into intra-specific and/or inter-specific competition for some of the limited resources available. We hypothesise that differences in locomotor abilities might be a key component deciding which individual will gain the resource, especially in animals with limited agility, such as turtles. To address this hypothesis, we tested self-righting ability and releasing from a vegetation alike rope in two sympatric populations of turtles: <i>Emys orbicularis</i> and <i>Mauremys rivulata</i>. All individuals managed to complete the tests promptly (≤ 103 s). When overturned on their carapace, <i>E. orbicularis</i> had shorter inspecting time, as well as overturning time, indicating slight advantage in self-righting. On the other hand, both species were equally efficient to release from a rope attached to the front of their plastron. The inter-specific morphological differences indicate that muscular strength might be a good predictor of competitive relations among syntopic turtle species.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":6879,"journal":{"name":"acta ethologica","volume":"26 2","pages":"133 - 137"},"PeriodicalIF":1.1,"publicationDate":"2023-05-06","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"43115216","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Experimental evidence that toe-tapping behavior in the green-and-black poison frog (Dendrobates auratus) is related to prey detection 实验证据表明,绿黑毒蛙(dendroates auratus)的脚趾敲击行为与猎物探测有关
IF 1.1 4区 生物学 Q2 Agricultural and Biological Sciences Pub Date : 2023-05-06 DOI: 10.1007/s10211-023-00422-8
Lisa M. Schulte, Yannis König

Toe-tapping, the quick movement of the middle toes of the hind legs, has been observed in many frogs and toads, and is usually associated with feeding, calling, or courtship behaviors. While plenty of observations of toe-tapping exist for different species, experimental evidence regarding the stimuli triggering this behavior is almost non-existent. Here, we systematically tested the influence of different stimuli on the toe-tapping behavior in the green-and-black poison frog (Dendrobates auratus, Dendrobatidae) from a captive colony in the Zoo Frankfurt. We found that, compared to a control, both big and small prey animals (crickets and fruit flies) elicited much more toe-tapping behavior, and that toe-tapping was positively correlated to feeding events. Playback advertisement calls in contrast did not trigger toe-tapping. We further showed that also juvenile frogs already toe-tap, but less frequently than adults. Our results support the observation-based data that toe-tapping is associated with hunting behaviors. While the auditory part of courtship does not seem to trigger toe-tapping, experimental evidence regarding visual and/or tactile courtship stimuli is still lacking.

在许多青蛙和蟾蜍身上都可以观察到后腿中间脚趾的快速运动,通常与进食、呼唤或求爱行为有关。虽然对不同物种进行了大量的观察,但关于触发这种行为的刺激的实验证据几乎不存在。在这里,我们系统地测试了不同刺激对法兰克福动物园圈养的绿黑毒蛙(dendroates auratus, dendroatidae)敲脚行为的影响。我们发现,与对照组相比,大型和小型猎物(蟋蟀和果蝇)都引发了更多的跺脚行为,而且跺脚与进食事件呈正相关。相比之下,播放广告调用不会触发敲脚。我们进一步发现,幼年蛙也会踢腿,但频率低于成年蛙。我们的研究结果支持基于观察的数据,即敲击脚趾与狩猎行为有关。虽然求爱的听觉部分似乎不会触发敲脚,但关于视觉和/或触觉求爱刺激的实验证据仍然缺乏。
{"title":"Experimental evidence that toe-tapping behavior in the green-and-black poison frog (Dendrobates auratus) is related to prey detection","authors":"Lisa M. Schulte,&nbsp;Yannis König","doi":"10.1007/s10211-023-00422-8","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s10211-023-00422-8","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Toe-tapping, the quick movement of the middle toes of the hind legs, has been observed in many frogs and toads, and is usually associated with feeding, calling, or courtship behaviors. While plenty of observations of toe-tapping exist for different species, experimental evidence regarding the stimuli triggering this behavior is almost non-existent. Here, we systematically tested the influence of different stimuli on the toe-tapping behavior in the green-and-black poison frog (<i>Dendrobates auratus</i>, Dendrobatidae) from a captive colony in the Zoo Frankfurt. We found that, compared to a control, both big and small prey animals (crickets and fruit flies) elicited much more toe-tapping behavior, and that toe-tapping was positively correlated to feeding events. Playback advertisement calls in contrast did not trigger toe-tapping. We further showed that also juvenile frogs already toe-tap, but less frequently than adults. Our results support the observation-based data that toe-tapping is associated with hunting behaviors. While the auditory part of courtship does not seem to trigger toe-tapping, experimental evidence regarding visual and/or tactile courtship stimuli is still lacking.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":6879,"journal":{"name":"acta ethologica","volume":"26 2","pages":"139 - 143"},"PeriodicalIF":1.1,"publicationDate":"2023-05-06","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://link.springer.com/content/pdf/10.1007/s10211-023-00422-8.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"48675450","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
期刊
acta ethologica
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1