首页 > 最新文献

Behavioural Processes最新文献

英文 中文
Ambient light spectrum affects larval Mexican jumping bean moth (Cydia saltitans) behavior despite light obstruction from host seed 尽管寄主种子阻挡了光线,但环境光谱仍会影响幼虫墨西哥跳豆蛾(Cydia saltitans)的行为。
IF 1.3 4区 生物学 Q4 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-09-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.beproc.2024.105093

Spectral differences in ambient light can affect animal behavior and convey crucial information about an individual’s environment. The ability to perceive and respond to differences in ambient light varies widely by taxa and is shaped by a species’ ecology. Mexican jumping bean moths, Cydia saltitans, spend their entire larval period encased in fallen host seeds and contend with potentially lethal environmental temperatures when host seeds are in direct sunlight. We investigate if and how C. saltitans larvae in host seeds respond to lighting conditions associated with these thermal risks. In a temperature-controlled experiment, we identified that larvae demonstrated distinct behavioral (“jumping”) responses corresponding to four lighting treatments (white, red, green, and purple), despite extremely minimal light penetration through host seed walls. Red light induced the greatest larval activity (measured by probability of movement and by displacement from origin), suggesting that larvae have mechanisms to perceive low levels of red light and/or to detect subtle increases in heat produced by red/near infrared-biased light spectra, possibly providing them with an early-warning mechanism against thermal stress. Our findings highlight the interplay of environmental lighting, behavior, and potential thermosensory adaptations in a species with a visually constrained environment.

环境光的光谱差异会影响动物的行为,并传递有关个体所处环境的重要信息。不同类群对环境光线差异的感知和反应能力差异很大,并且受物种生态学的影响。墨西哥跳豆天蛾(Cydia saltitans)的整个幼虫期都被包裹在落下的寄主种子中,当寄主种子被阳光直射时,它们要与可能致命的环境温度作斗争。我们研究了寄主种子中的沙丁夜蛾幼虫在自然环境中是否以及如何应对与这些热风险相关的光照条件。在一项温控实验中,我们发现尽管寄主种子壁的透光率极低,但幼虫在四种光照(白光、红光、绿光和紫光)条件下会表现出不同的行为("跳跃")反应。红光诱导的幼虫活动最高(以移动概率和离开原点的位移来衡量),这表明幼虫具有感知低水平红光和/或检测红光/近红外偏振光光谱产生的微弱热量增加的机制,这可能为它们提供了一种对抗热应力的预警机制。我们的研究结果突显了在视觉受限的环境中,环境光照、行为和潜在热感适应的相互作用。
{"title":"Ambient light spectrum affects larval Mexican jumping bean moth (Cydia saltitans) behavior despite light obstruction from host seed","authors":"","doi":"10.1016/j.beproc.2024.105093","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.beproc.2024.105093","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Spectral differences in ambient light can affect animal behavior and convey crucial information about an individual’s environment. The ability to perceive and respond to differences in ambient light varies widely by taxa and is shaped by a species’ ecology. Mexican jumping bean moths, <em>Cydia saltitans</em>, spend their entire larval period encased in fallen host seeds and contend with potentially lethal environmental temperatures when host seeds are in direct sunlight. We investigate if and how <em>C. saltitans</em> larvae in host seeds respond to lighting conditions associated with these thermal risks. In a temperature-controlled experiment, we identified that larvae demonstrated distinct behavioral (“jumping”) responses corresponding to four lighting treatments (white, red, green, and purple), despite extremely minimal light penetration through host seed walls. Red light induced the greatest larval activity (measured by probability of movement and by displacement from origin), suggesting that larvae have mechanisms to perceive low levels of red light and/or to detect subtle increases in heat produced by red/near infrared-biased light spectra, possibly providing them with an early-warning mechanism against thermal stress. Our findings highlight the interplay of environmental lighting, behavior, and potential thermosensory adaptations in a species with a visually constrained environment.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":8746,"journal":{"name":"Behavioural Processes","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.3,"publicationDate":"2024-09-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142078951","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Play contagion in African elephants: The closest, the better 非洲象的游戏传染:越接近越好。
IF 1.3 4区 生物学 Q4 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-09-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.beproc.2024.105092

Behavioural contagion is an automatic process through which a behaviour performed by an individual (trigger) is reproduced by an observer (responder) without necessarily replicating the exact motor sequence. It has been linked to inter-individual synchronisation and possibly emotional contagion. Play can convey emotions and enhance social bonding, although its contagiousness is understudied. To verify social play contagion presence and modulating factors, we gathered audio-video data on social play, distance and affiliation on a group of savannah African elephants (15 individuals) at Parque de la Naturaleza de Cabarceno (Cantabria, Spain). Social play was contagious as it was more likely started by uninvolved elephants (within 3-min) in Post-Play Condition (PP) - after that other elephants had started playing - than in Matched-control Condition (MC; no previous play). Social play contagion mostly occurred within 30 m – probably due to elephants’ limited visual acuity – and it was highest between individuals that affiliated the most, with the distance-affiliation interaction having no effect. The most prominent individuals in the social play network were also the most influential in the play contagion network (Eigenvector-centrality measure). Play contagion was socially modulated, thus suggesting it may extend from motor replication to the replication of the underlying affective state.

行为传染是一个自动过程,通过这个过程,个体(触发者)的行为会被观察者(响应者)复制,但不一定要复制确切的运动序列。它与个体间的同步以及可能的情绪传染有关。游戏可以传递积极的情绪,增强社会联系,但其传染性还没有得到充分研究。为了验证社交游戏的传染性和调节因素,我们在卡巴塞诺自然公园(西班牙坎塔布里亚)收集了一组非洲草原象(15 头)的社交游戏、距离和隶属关系的音频视频数据。社交游戏具有传染性,因为在游戏后条件(PP)下,即在其他大象开始游戏后,未参与游戏的大象(3分钟内)比在匹配对照条件(MC;之前没有游戏)下更有可能开始社交游戏。社交游戏传染大多发生在 30 米以内(这可能是由于大象的视觉敏锐度有限),而且在关系最密切的个体之间传染率最高,距离与关系的交互作用没有影响。社会游戏网络中最突出的个体在游戏传染网络中也最具影响力(特征向量-中心性测量)。游戏传染是受社会调节的,因此表明它可能从运动复制扩展到基本情感状态的复制。
{"title":"Play contagion in African elephants: The closest, the better","authors":"","doi":"10.1016/j.beproc.2024.105092","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.beproc.2024.105092","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Behavioural contagion is an automatic process through which a behaviour performed by an individual (trigger) is reproduced by an observer (responder) without necessarily replicating the exact motor sequence. It has been linked to inter-individual synchronisation and possibly emotional contagion. Play can convey emotions and enhance social bonding, although its contagiousness is understudied. To verify social play contagion presence and modulating factors, we gathered audio-video data on social play, distance and affiliation on a group of savannah African elephants (15 individuals) at Parque de la Naturaleza de Cabarceno (Cantabria, Spain). Social play was contagious as it was more likely started by uninvolved elephants (within 3-min) in Post-Play Condition (PP) - after that other elephants had started playing - than in Matched-control Condition (MC; no previous play). Social play contagion mostly occurred within 30 m – probably due to elephants’ limited visual acuity – and it was highest between individuals that affiliated the most, with the distance-affiliation interaction having no effect. The most prominent individuals in the social play network were also the most influential in the play contagion network (Eigenvector-centrality measure). Play contagion was socially modulated, thus suggesting it may extend from motor replication to the replication of the underlying affective state.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":8746,"journal":{"name":"Behavioural Processes","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.3,"publicationDate":"2024-09-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0376635724001074/pdfft?md5=737d97f49b60a912f7e67d4d5b5f4a0e&pid=1-s2.0-S0376635724001074-main.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142103917","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Sounds in common: Time-frequency as the classification parameters for pulsed sounds produced by Delphinus delphis 共同的声音:时间-频率作为海豚发出的脉冲声的分类参数。
IF 1.3 4区 生物学 Q4 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-08-21 DOI: 10.1016/j.beproc.2024.105091

Sounds produced by dolphins can be grouped into tonal (whistles) and pulsed sounds (e.g., echolocation clicks and burst sounds). Clicks are broadband pulses temporarily spaced to allow echo processing between the sound source and the object. Echolocation is related mainly to prey detection and environmental recognition. Echolocation click trains tend to present a decreasing inter-click interval due to a continuous changing of the target's location when the animal approaches to capture the prey. In addition to foraging and feeding contexts, burst pulsed sounds have been associated with short social communication. Although echolocation clicks are relatively well documented, there is no consensus regarding the broad variety of the burst pulsed signals. The present study analyzed time-frequency characteristics by conducting a clustering and discrimination analysis to classify pulsed sounds. A total of 64 click trains were analyzed from short-beaked common dolphins recorded in the slope region of the western South Atlantic Ocean. Three analyses (time; frequency; and combined time-frequency parameters) were compared through k-means clustering and posterior cluster validation using Random forest analysis. The k-means clustering resulted in four clusters for all groups of analysis. The time parameters were the most accurate among the comparisons, with the first two-dimensional axis corresponding to 87 % (Dim1 = 70.2 % and Dim2 = 17.2 %). The random forest analysis showed that the time-frequency dataset was the best classification of pulsed sounds in D. delphis (Accuracy = 84.6 %; confidence interval CI = 65.1 %-95.6 %; p < 0.01). This result considers the animal an acoustical identity, emphasizing the importance of certain parameters that influence this identity and thus reflecting the energy-cost optimization for sound production.

海豚发出的声音可分为音调声(哨声)和脉冲声(如回声定位点击声和爆裂声)。咔嗒声是宽带脉冲,暂时间隔开来,以便在声源和物体之间进行回声处理。回声定位主要与猎物探测和环境识别有关。当动物接近捕捉猎物时,目标的位置会不断变化,因此回声定位的点击序列往往会出现点击间隔不断缩短的现象。除了觅食和捕食环境外,突发脉冲声音还与短时间的社会交流有关。虽然回声定位的点击声有相对较多的文献记载,但对于脉冲信号的广泛种类还没有达成共识。本研究通过聚类和辨别分析来分析脉冲声的时间频率特性,从而对脉冲声进行分类。本研究分析了在南大西洋西部斜坡地区记录到的短喙普通海豚的 64 个点击音序列。通过 k-means 聚类和使用随机森林分析的后聚类验证,对三种分析(时间、频率和时间频率组合参数)进行了比较。k-means 聚类的结果是所有分析组都有四个聚类。在比较中,时间参数的准确率最高,第一个二维轴的准确率为 87%(Dim1 = 70.2%,Dim2 = 17.2%)。随机森林分析表明,时间-频率数据集是海豚脉冲声的最佳分类方法(准确率 = 84.6%;置信区间 CI = 65.1%-95.6%;p < 0.01)。这一结果认为动物具有声学特征,强调了影响这一特征的某些参数的重要性,从而反映了声音产生的能量成本优化。
{"title":"Sounds in common: Time-frequency as the classification parameters for pulsed sounds produced by Delphinus delphis","authors":"","doi":"10.1016/j.beproc.2024.105091","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.beproc.2024.105091","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Sounds produced by dolphins can be grouped into tonal (whistles) and pulsed sounds (e.g., echolocation clicks and burst sounds). Clicks are broadband pulses temporarily spaced to allow echo processing between the sound source and the object. Echolocation is related mainly to prey detection and environmental recognition. Echolocation click trains tend to present a decreasing inter-click interval due to a continuous changing of the target's location when the animal approaches to capture the prey. In addition to foraging and feeding contexts, burst pulsed sounds have been associated with short social communication. Although echolocation clicks are relatively well documented, there is no consensus regarding the broad variety of the burst pulsed signals. The present study analyzed time-frequency characteristics by conducting a clustering and discrimination analysis to classify pulsed sounds. A total of 64 click trains were analyzed from short-beaked common dolphins recorded in the slope region of the western South Atlantic Ocean. Three analyses (time; frequency; and combined time-frequency parameters) were compared through k-means clustering and posterior cluster validation using Random forest analysis. The k-means clustering resulted in four clusters for all groups of analysis. The time parameters were the most accurate among the comparisons, with the first two-dimensional axis corresponding to 87 % (Dim1 = 70.2 % and Dim2 = 17.2 %). The random forest analysis showed that the time-frequency dataset was the best classification of pulsed sounds in <em>D. delphis</em> (Accuracy = 84.6 %; confidence interval CI = 65.1 %-95.6 %; p &lt; 0.01). This result considers the animal an acoustical identity, emphasizing the importance of certain parameters that influence this identity and thus reflecting the energy-cost optimization for sound production.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":8746,"journal":{"name":"Behavioural Processes","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.3,"publicationDate":"2024-08-21","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142035116","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Expression of exploratory activity of rat offspring depends on the expression of exploratory activity of their parents at the moment of mating 大鼠后代的探索活动表现取决于交配时父母的探索活动表现
IF 1.3 4区 生物学 Q4 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-08-02 DOI: 10.1016/j.beproc.2024.105090

The aim of this work was to study the, so far, unexplored possibility that non-genetic inheritance of animal behavioral characteristics could depend on the state of the parents at the time of conception. In this study, we measured the levels of motor and exploratory activity in rats at the ages of 2 and 5 months. Male and female rats were mated at the age of 5 months. The following groups were used: male and female rats with high motor activity at ages of 2 and 5 months (ACT+); male and female rats with high activity at the age of 2 months, but low activity at the age of 5 months (ACT–); male and female rats with low activity at the ages of 2 and 5 months (PAS–); male and female rats with low activity at the age of 2 months, but high activity at the age of 5 months (PAS+). It was found that both males and females ACT+ had significantly higher motor activity, which was observed in the first 10 minutes, in the next 20–60 minutes, in the center of the cage and more rearings as compared with PAS– rats. Significant differences in the severity of exploratory activity were found between the male offspring of ACT+ and ACT– rats. Differences between the offspring of PAS+ and PAS– rats were observed in both the male and female rats. The motor activity of animals in the period from 20 minutes after the start of registration did not differ between groups. Thus, it can be considered that individual characteristics of general motor activity are due to genetically inherited factors, while differences in the level of exploratory activity, apparently, are formed due to non-genetic influences from parents during mating.

这项工作的目的是研究迄今为止尚未探索过的一种可能性,即动物行为特征的非遗传性可能取决于受孕时父母的状态。在这项研究中,我们测量了 2 个月和 5 个月大鼠的运动和探索活动水平。雌雄大鼠在 5 个月大时交配。我们将大鼠分为以下几组:2 个月和 5 个月大时运动活动水平高的雌雄大鼠(ACT+);2 个月大时活动水平高但 5 个月大时活动水平低的雌雄大鼠(ACT-);2 个月和 5 个月大时活动水平低的雌雄大鼠(PAS-);2 个月大时活动水平低但 5 个月大时活动水平高的雌雄大鼠(PAS+)。研究发现,与 PAS 大鼠相比,ACT+ 雄性和雌性大鼠的运动活动明显较高,在前 10 分钟、接下来的 20-60 分钟、笼子中央和更多的后部都能观察到运动活动。ACT+和ACT-大鼠的雄性后代在探索活动的严重程度上存在显著差异。PAS+和PAS-大鼠的后代在雄性和雌性大鼠中均存在差异。登记开始后 20 分钟内动物的运动活动在组间没有差异。因此,可以认为一般运动活动的个体特征是由遗传因素造成的,而探索活动水平的差异显然是在交配过程中受父母的非遗传影响而形成的。
{"title":"Expression of exploratory activity of rat offspring depends on the expression of exploratory activity of their parents at the moment of mating","authors":"","doi":"10.1016/j.beproc.2024.105090","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.beproc.2024.105090","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>The aim of this work was to study the, so far, unexplored possibility that non-genetic inheritance of animal behavioral characteristics could depend on the state of the parents at the time of conception. In this study, we measured the levels of motor and exploratory activity in rats at the ages of 2 and 5 months. Male and female rats were mated at the age of 5 months. The following groups were used: male and female rats with high motor activity at ages of 2 and 5 months (ACT+); male and female rats with high activity at the age of 2 months, but low activity at the age of 5 months (ACT–); male and female rats with low activity at the ages of 2 and 5 months (PAS–); male and female rats with low activity at the age of 2 months, but high activity at the age of 5 months (PAS+). It was found that both males and females ACT+ had significantly higher motor activity, which was observed in the first 10 minutes, in the next 20–60 minutes, in the center of the cage and more rearings as compared with PAS– rats. Significant differences in the severity of exploratory activity were found between the male offspring of ACT+ and ACT– rats. Differences between the offspring of PAS+ and PAS– rats were observed in both the male and female rats. The motor activity of animals in the period from 20 minutes after the start of registration did not differ between groups. Thus, it can be considered that individual characteristics of general motor activity are due to genetically inherited factors, while differences in the level of exploratory activity, apparently, are formed due to non-genetic influences from parents during mating.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":8746,"journal":{"name":"Behavioural Processes","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.3,"publicationDate":"2024-08-02","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141888331","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Horses can learn to identify joy and sadness against other basic emotions from human facial expressions 马可以从人类的面部表情中学会识别喜悦和悲伤,以及其他基本情绪。
IF 1.3 4区 生物学 Q4 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-08-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.beproc.2024.105081

Recently, horses and other domestic mammals have been shown to perceive and react to human emotional signals, with most studies focusing on joy and anger. In this study, we tested whether horses can learn to identify human joyful and sad expressions against other emotions. We used a touchscreen-based automated device that presented pairs of human portraits and distributed pellets when the horse touched the rewarded face. Six horses were trained to touch the sad face and 5 the joyful face. By the end of training, horses’ performances at the group level were significantly higher than chance level, with higher scores for horses trained with the sad face. At the individual level, evidence of task learning varied among horses, which could be explained by individual variations in horses’ ability to identify different human facial expressions or attention issues during the tests. In a generalization test, we introduced portraits of different humans than those presented during training. Horses trained with the joyful face performed better than chance, demonstrating generalization. Conversely, horses trained with the sad face did not. Horses also showed differences in learning performance according to the non-rewarded emotion, providing insights into horses’ cognitive processing of facial expressions.

最近,有研究表明马和其他家养哺乳动物能够感知人类的情绪信号并做出反应,其中大多数研究集中在喜悦和愤怒方面。在这项研究中,我们测试了马匹能否学会识别人类喜悦和悲伤的表情与其他情绪。我们使用了一种基于触摸屏的自动装置,该装置可显示成对的人类肖像,当马触摸到受奖励的脸部时就会分发颗粒。6 匹马接受了触摸悲伤表情的训练,5 匹马接受了触摸欢乐表情的训练。训练结束时,马匹在群体水平上的表现明显高于偶然水平,接受悲伤脸部训练的马匹得分更高。在个体水平上,任务学习的证据在马匹之间存在差异,这可能是由于马匹识别不同人类面部表情的能力存在个体差异或在测试过程中存在注意力问题。在泛化测试中,我们引入了与训练时不同的人类肖像。接受欢乐表情训练的马匹的表现好于其他马匹,这表明它们的泛化能力很强。相反,接受悲伤表情训练的马匹则没有这种表现。马匹的学习成绩还因非奖励情绪而异,这为马匹对面部表情的认知处理提供了启示。
{"title":"Horses can learn to identify joy and sadness against other basic emotions from human facial expressions","authors":"","doi":"10.1016/j.beproc.2024.105081","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.beproc.2024.105081","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Recently, horses and other domestic mammals have been shown to perceive and react to human emotional signals, with most studies focusing on joy and anger. In this study, we tested whether horses can learn to identify human joyful and sad expressions against other emotions. We used a touchscreen-based automated device that presented pairs of human portraits and distributed pellets when the horse touched the rewarded face. Six horses were trained to touch the sad face and 5 the joyful face. By the end of training, horses’ performances at the group level were significantly higher than chance level, with higher scores for horses trained with the sad face. At the individual level, evidence of task learning varied among horses, which could be explained by individual variations in horses’ ability to identify different human facial expressions or attention issues during the tests. In a generalization test, we introduced portraits of different humans than those presented during training. Horses trained with the joyful face performed better than chance, demonstrating generalization. Conversely, horses trained with the sad face did not. Horses also showed differences in learning performance according to the non-rewarded emotion, providing insights into horses’ cognitive processing of facial expressions.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":8746,"journal":{"name":"Behavioural Processes","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.3,"publicationDate":"2024-08-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0376635724000962/pdfft?md5=4869edee356afb15282e228aff4f0f06&pid=1-s2.0-S0376635724000962-main.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141787190","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Resurgence mitigation across extended extinction following four and eight cycles of on/off alternative reinforcement 在四次和八次循环的开/关替代性强化后,经过延长的消亡期缓解了复发。
IF 1.3 4区 生物学 Q4 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-08-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.beproc.2024.105082

Resurgence is an increase in an extinguished operant response resulting from a worsening of conditions (e.g., extinction) for a more recently reinforced alternative behavior. Previous research has shown that exposure to cycles of alternative reinforcement available versus unavailable (i.e., on/off alternative reinforcement) across sessions can reduce subsequent resurgence. Most previous assessments of the procedure have examined target operant responding during only single-session resurgence tests, and it remains unclear if exposure to relatively few cycles of on/off alternative reinforcement can maintain low rates of target behavior across extended exposure to extinction. This experiment with rats examined the effects of 4 or 8 cycles of on/off alternative reinforcement on subsequent resurgence during a 10-session extinction test. The results show that exposure to 4 cycles of on/off alternative reinforcement is as effective as 8 cycles in producing low rates of target behavior during treatment and across extended extinction. This result is consistent with extant theories of resurgence and suggests that on/off alternative reinforcement could have translational utility following relatively few cycles of exposure.

卷土重来是指由于最近强化的替代行为的条件恶化(如消退)而导致被熄灭的操作反应增加。以往的研究表明,在不同的训练阶段中暴露于可用与不可用的替代性强化循环(即开/关替代性强化)中,可以减少随后的卷土重来。之前对该程序的评估大多只考察了单次复归测试中的目标操作反应,至于暴露于相对较少的开/关替代性强化循环是否能在长时间暴露于消退过程中保持较低的目标行为率,目前仍不清楚。本实验以大鼠为研究对象,考察了在10次消退测试中,4或8个周期的开/关替代性强化对后续复发的影响。结果表明,在治疗过程中以及在长时间的消退过程中,暴露于 4 个周期的开/关替代性强化与 8 个周期的开/关替代性强化在产生低目标行为率方面同样有效。这一结果与现有的卷土重来理论是一致的,并表明在接触相对较少的周期后,开/关替代性强化可能具有转化效用。
{"title":"Resurgence mitigation across extended extinction following four and eight cycles of on/off alternative reinforcement","authors":"","doi":"10.1016/j.beproc.2024.105082","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.beproc.2024.105082","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Resurgence is an increase in an extinguished operant response resulting from a worsening of conditions (e.g., extinction) for a more recently reinforced alternative behavior. Previous research has shown that exposure to cycles of alternative reinforcement available versus unavailable (i.e., on/off alternative reinforcement) across sessions can reduce subsequent resurgence. Most previous assessments of the procedure have examined target operant responding during only single-session resurgence tests, and it remains unclear if exposure to relatively few cycles of on/off alternative reinforcement can maintain low rates of target behavior across extended exposure to extinction. This experiment with rats examined the effects of 4 or 8 cycles of on/off alternative reinforcement on subsequent resurgence during a 10-session extinction test. The results show that exposure to 4 cycles of on/off alternative reinforcement is as effective as 8 cycles in producing low rates of target behavior during treatment and across extended extinction. This result is consistent with extant theories of resurgence and suggests that on/off alternative reinforcement could have translational utility following relatively few cycles of exposure.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":8746,"journal":{"name":"Behavioural Processes","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.3,"publicationDate":"2024-08-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141787191","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The role of collective behaviour in fish response to visual cues 集体行为在鱼类对视觉线索的反应中的作用。
IF 1.3 4区 生物学 Q4 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-08-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.beproc.2024.105079

This study investigated the influence of group size (individual, groups of five, and 20) on the response of common minnow to visual cues created by vertical black and white stripes over time. The stripes were displayed on a monitor either at one end of an experimental tank, while the other was uniform white, or both ends simultaneously. Reponses were compared with a control (stripes absent). Visual cues were pseudo-randomly presented every 15-minutes over six-hours. Three predictions were made: first, due to more efficient flow of information, larger groups would respond more rapidly (Rate of response) to the visual cues. Second, assuming visual cues provide a proxy for structure and larger groups experience greater benefits of group membership due to reduced predatory risk, there will be stronger association (Strength of association and Final association) with stripes for individuals and smaller groups compared with larger groups. Consequently, the association with visual cues exhibited by larger groups would diminish over time compared to smaller, more risk averse groups. As expected, larger groups exhibited a faster Rate of response to visual cues, and individual fish a greater Strength of association compared with the largest group size. Final association, however, was more common for larger groups compared to both smaller groups and individuals. Contrary to the final prediction, responses to visual cues did not decrease over time for any group size, suggesting innate behaviour or an experimental duration insufficient to observe habituation.

本研究调查了群体大小(个体、5 人一组和 20 人一组)对普通鲦鱼对垂直黑白条纹所产生的视觉线索的反应随时间变化的影响。条纹显示在显示器上,要么显示在实验水箱的一端,而另一端则是统一的白色,要么两端同时显示。实验鼠的反应与对照组(无条纹)进行比较。在 6 个小时内,每隔 15 分钟伪随机显示一次视觉提示。我们做出了三项预测:第一,由于信息流的效率更高,较大的群体会对视觉线索做出更迅速的反应(反应速率)。其次,假设视觉线索提供了结构的替代物,而大群体由于捕食风险降低而从群体成员中获得更大的益处,那么与大群体相比,个体和小群体与条纹的关联(关联强度和最终关联)将更强。因此,随着时间的推移,大群体对视觉线索的联想会减弱,而小群体则更厌恶风险。不出所料,与规模最大的群体相比,规模较大的群体对视觉线索的反应速度更快,个体鱼的联想强度更大。然而,与较小的群体和个体相比,较大群体的最终联想更为普遍。与最终预测相反,任何规模的群体对视觉线索的反应都没有随着时间的推移而减少,这表明鱼类的先天行为或实验持续时间不足以观察到习惯化。
{"title":"The role of collective behaviour in fish response to visual cues","authors":"","doi":"10.1016/j.beproc.2024.105079","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.beproc.2024.105079","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>This study investigated the influence of group size (individual, groups of five, and 20) on the response of common minnow to visual cues created by vertical black and white stripes over time. The stripes were displayed on a monitor either at one end of an experimental tank, while the other was uniform white, or both ends simultaneously. Reponses were compared with a control (stripes absent). Visual cues were pseudo-randomly presented every 15-minutes over six-hours. Three predictions were made: first, due to more efficient flow of information, larger groups would respond more rapidly (<em>Rate of response</em>) to the visual cues. Second, assuming visual cues provide a proxy for structure and larger groups experience greater benefits of group membership due to reduced predatory risk, there will be stronger association (<em>Strength of association</em> and <em>Final association</em>) with stripes for individuals and smaller groups compared with larger groups. Consequently, the association with visual cues exhibited by larger groups would diminish over time compared to smaller, more risk averse groups. As expected, larger groups exhibited a faster <em>Rate of response</em> to visual cues, and individual fish a greater <em>Strength of association</em> compared with the largest group size. <em>Final association</em>, however, was more common for larger groups compared to both smaller groups and individuals. Contrary to the final prediction, responses to visual cues did not decrease over time for any group size, suggesting innate behaviour or an experimental duration insufficient to observe habituation.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":8746,"journal":{"name":"Behavioural Processes","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.3,"publicationDate":"2024-08-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0376635724000949/pdfft?md5=e00587c944c5d6103f238ad35ac15bf7&pid=1-s2.0-S0376635724000949-main.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141722928","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Rapid prey manipulation and bite location preferences in three species of wandering spiders 三种游走蜘蛛的快速猎物操纵和咬合位置偏好。
IF 1.3 4区 生物学 Q4 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-07-31 DOI: 10.1016/j.beproc.2024.105083

Predator-prey interactions are the interspecific relationships of greatest interest in ecology. Spiders are among the most diverse and ubiquitous terrestrial predators on the planet. Their large dietary breadth is often linked with the development of specific predatory behaviors and morphological adaptations. However, studies on the predatory behavior of spiders have mostly focused on specialist species, leaving behind the ethological variability occurring in generalist species that allow them to respond to the different prey types. For three species of generalist wandering spiders, we searched images of predation events on the Internet to determine the most common prey. Subsequently, the focal predator species were then used in behavioral experiments. Using high-speed videos, handling patterns for different prey types (spider and cricket) were analyzed. Our results show a notable difference in handling patterns between prey types. We found that the spider prey was often rotated around the axis allowing the predator to bite in the ventral region of the prey and thus avoid a counterattack. Contrary, crickets were arbitrarily rotated. Our work may be an indication that these three species of generalist spiders have a preference for manipulating prey differently with a preference to rotate spiders, allowing them to exploit prey with various defensive mechanisms.

捕食者与被捕食者之间的相互作用是生态学中最受关注的种间关系。蜘蛛是地球上种类最多、最普遍的陆生捕食者之一。它们的食性广泛,往往与特定捕食行为和形态适应性的发展有关。然而,对蜘蛛捕食行为的研究大多集中在专性物种上,而忽略了普性物种的伦理变异性,而这种变异性使它们能够对不同类型的猎物做出反应。我们在互联网上搜索了三种普通游走蜘蛛捕食事件的图片,以确定最常见的猎物。随后,重点捕食物种被用于行为实验。我们利用高速视频分析了不同类型猎物(蜘蛛和蟋蟀)的捕食模式。结果显示,不同类型猎物的处理模式存在明显差异。我们发现,蜘蛛猎物通常会绕轴旋转,这样捕食者就可以咬住猎物的腹部,从而避免反击。相反,蟋蟀则是任意旋转。我们的研究结果可能表明,这三种通食蜘蛛在操纵猎物时有不同的偏好,它们偏好旋转蜘蛛,从而可以利用猎物的各种防御机制。
{"title":"Rapid prey manipulation and bite location preferences in three species of wandering spiders","authors":"","doi":"10.1016/j.beproc.2024.105083","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.beproc.2024.105083","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Predator-prey interactions are the interspecific relationships of greatest interest in ecology. Spiders are among the most diverse and ubiquitous terrestrial predators on the planet. Their large dietary breadth is often linked with the development of specific predatory behaviors and morphological adaptations. However, studies on the predatory behavior of spiders have mostly focused on specialist species, leaving behind the ethological variability occurring in generalist species that allow them to respond to the different prey types. For three species of generalist wandering spiders, we searched images of predation events on the Internet to determine the most common prey. Subsequently, the focal predator species were then used in behavioral experiments. Using high-speed videos, handling patterns for different prey types (spider and cricket) were analyzed. Our results show a notable difference in handling patterns between prey types. We found that the spider prey was often rotated around the axis allowing the predator to bite in the ventral region of the prey and thus avoid a counterattack. Contrary, crickets were arbitrarily rotated. Our work may be an indication that these three species of generalist spiders have a preference for manipulating prey differently with a preference to rotate spiders, allowing them to exploit prey with various defensive mechanisms.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":8746,"journal":{"name":"Behavioural Processes","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.3,"publicationDate":"2024-07-31","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141878300","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Applying an eye tracking technique to gibbons: First study using scanpath measurements for visual stimuli 将眼动跟踪技术应用于长臂猿:首次使用扫描路径测量视觉刺激的研究。
IF 1.3 4区 生物学 Q4 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-07-23 DOI: 10.1016/j.beproc.2024.105080

Compared to the abundance of research on cognition in various nonhuman primate species, studies of gibbons -- often called “the small apes” -- remain limited, despite the importance of gibbons for understanding evolutionary processes in humans and other apes. Over the past decade, eye tracking techniques have been established in chimpanzees and other nonhuman primates using the free-participation method, which requires no physical restraint of the subjects. We investigated the feasibility of using the same method to record visual scanpaths in gibbons. We attempted to measure the eye movements of three adult gibbons while they spontaneously viewed images, with no prior fixation training. Calibration was successful in all three individuals, with errors of less than one degree. In total, 24 stimuli were used, with landscape and nonhuman primate face photographs presented on one-quarter of the screen, to test the prediction that gibbons would change their viewing time depending on image category. All three gibbons viewed the images for longer than the background, and primate face images for longer than landscapes. These results are consistent with previous findings in other primate species that faces attract more attention than non-face stimuli, suggesting that this effect is common across primates. This study demonstrates the feasibility of using eye tracking with gibbons. Further studies on gibbon visual exploration and cognition may enhance our understanding of the phylogenetic origins of hominid intelligence as well as the unique evolution of gibbons.

与对各种非人灵长类动物认知能力的大量研究相比,对长臂猿(常被称为 "小型类人猿")的研究仍然十分有限,尽管长臂猿对了解人类和其他类人猿的进化过程十分重要。在过去的十年中,人们已经在黑猩猩和其他非人灵长类动物身上建立了眼动跟踪技术,这种技术采用自由参与法,不需要对被试进行身体束缚。我们研究了使用同样方法记录长臂猿视觉扫描路径的可行性。我们尝试测量了三只成年长臂猿自发观看图像时的眼球运动,它们事先都没有接受过定点训练。三只长臂猿的校准都很成功,误差小于一度。总共使用了 24 个刺激物,其中风景和非人灵长类脸部照片占屏幕的四分之一,以测试长臂猿会根据图像类别改变观看时间的预测。三只长臂猿观看图片的时间都长于观看背景的时间,而观看灵长类面部图片的时间则长于观看风景的时间。这些结果与之前在其他灵长类动物身上发现的结果一致,即人脸比非人脸刺激物更能吸引注意力,这表明这种效应在灵长类动物中很普遍。这项研究证明了对长臂猿使用眼动仪的可行性。对长臂猿视觉探索和认知的进一步研究可能会加深我们对类人智能系统发育起源以及长臂猿独特进化的理解。
{"title":"Applying an eye tracking technique to gibbons: First study using scanpath measurements for visual stimuli","authors":"","doi":"10.1016/j.beproc.2024.105080","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.beproc.2024.105080","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Compared to the abundance of research on cognition in various nonhuman primate species, studies of gibbons -- often called “the small apes” -- remain limited, despite the importance of gibbons for understanding evolutionary processes in humans and other apes. Over the past decade, eye tracking techniques have been established in chimpanzees and other nonhuman primates using the free-participation method, which requires no physical restraint of the subjects. We investigated the feasibility of using the same method to record visual scanpaths in gibbons. We attempted to measure the eye movements of three adult gibbons while they spontaneously viewed images, with no prior fixation training. Calibration was successful in all three individuals, with errors of less than one degree. In total, 24 stimuli were used, with landscape and nonhuman primate face photographs presented on one-quarter of the screen, to test the prediction that gibbons would change their viewing time depending on image category. All three gibbons viewed the images for longer than the background, and primate face images for longer than landscapes. These results are consistent with previous findings in other primate species that faces attract more attention than non-face stimuli, suggesting that this effect is common across primates. This study demonstrates the feasibility of using eye tracking with gibbons. Further studies on gibbon visual exploration and cognition may enhance our understanding of the phylogenetic origins of hominid intelligence as well as the unique evolution of gibbons.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":8746,"journal":{"name":"Behavioural Processes","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.3,"publicationDate":"2024-07-23","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141756859","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Distribution of brood of the acorn ant Temnothorax crassispinus in artificial nests after forced migration 橡子蚁 Temnothorax crassispinus 被迫迁徙后人工巢中雏鸟的分布。
IF 1.3 4区 生物学 Q4 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-07-08 DOI: 10.1016/j.beproc.2024.105077
Sławomir Mitrus

Nest sites are important for social insects, as they provide refuge against enemies and ensure optimal conditions for the brood development. In large nests, the different chambers can be used for different reasons; for example, for food storage or as a brood chamber. Acorn ants from the genus Temnothorax dwell in small cavities in acorns and wood; however, even such small chambers can have a high degree of spatial heterogeneity. During this study, the distribution of brood items of the acorn ant Temnothorax crassispinus inside artificial nest cavities composed of three chambers in a linear system was analysed. 29 ant colonies were photographed 13 times during a period of approximately one month: during three consecutive days, and after forced migrations. I found that the distribution of the brood inside the nest cavity was similar during the consecutive days; however, after the forced migration, the distribution typically changed. Almost all the brood items were kept farther from the entrance. Keeping the brood farther from the entrance could be explained as a safer option.

巢穴对于社会性昆虫来说非常重要,因为它们提供了抵御敌人的庇护所,并确保育雏的最佳条件。在大型巢穴中,不同的巢室有不同的用途,例如储存食物或作为育雏室。橡子蚁属的橡子蚁居住在橡子和木头的小洞穴中;然而,即使是这样的小洞穴也会有高度的空间异质性。本研究分析了橡子蚁(Temnothorax crassispinus)在由线性系统中的三个巢室组成的人工巢穴内的育雏物品分布情况。在大约一个月的时间里,对 29 个蚁群进行了 13 次拍摄:连续三天拍摄,以及强迫迁移后拍摄。我发现,巢穴内雏鸟的分布在连续几天内是相似的;然而,在强制迁移后,雏鸟的分布通常会发生变化。几乎所有的雏鸟都离入口较远。把雏鸟放在离入口较远的地方可能是一种更安全的选择。
{"title":"Distribution of brood of the acorn ant Temnothorax crassispinus in artificial nests after forced migration","authors":"Sławomir Mitrus","doi":"10.1016/j.beproc.2024.105077","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.beproc.2024.105077","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Nest sites are important for social insects, as they provide refuge against enemies and ensure optimal conditions for the brood development. In large nests, the different chambers can be used for different reasons; for example, for food storage or as a brood chamber. Acorn ants from the genus <em>Temnothorax</em> dwell in small cavities in acorns and wood; however, even such small chambers can have a high degree of spatial heterogeneity. During this study, the distribution of brood items of the acorn ant <em>Temnothorax crassispinus</em> inside artificial nest cavities composed of three chambers in a linear system was analysed. 29 ant colonies were photographed 13 times during a period of approximately one month: during three consecutive days, and after forced migrations. I found that the distribution of the brood inside the nest cavity was similar during the consecutive days; however, after the forced migration, the distribution typically changed. Almost all the brood items were kept farther from the entrance. Keeping the brood farther from the entrance could be explained as a safer option.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":8746,"journal":{"name":"Behavioural Processes","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.3,"publicationDate":"2024-07-08","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0376635724000925/pdfft?md5=fc44114c5dd58dfb2d42d2e6ae534282&pid=1-s2.0-S0376635724000925-main.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141578890","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Behavioural Processes
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1