首页 > 最新文献

Behavioural Processes最新文献

英文 中文
Incentive-salience attribution is attenuated in spontaneously hypertensive rats, an animal model of ADHD 自发性高血压大鼠(一种多动症动物模型)的激励-忍耐归因能力减弱
IF 1.3 4区 生物学 Q4 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-06-16 DOI: 10.1016/j.beproc.2024.105068
Fernanda González-Barriga, Vladimir Orduña

Spontaneously Hypertensive Rats (SHR) have been extensively studied as an animal model of Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) because they show some of the defining features of that disorder, like some forms of impulsivity and hyperactivity. However, other characteristics of the disorder, like a deficit in motivation, have been scarcely studied in the SHR strain. In the present report, we studied in 45 SHR and 45 Wistar rats as a comparison group, the capacity of attribution of incentive salience to a stimulus predictor of reinforcement, which has become a central concept in the study of motivation. We employed the Pavlovian Conditioned-Approach (PCA) task, in which a lever is presented 8 s before a pellet is delivered. The attribution of incentive salience is indicated by responses to the lever, in contrast to the absence of attribution of incentive salience, which is indicated by entrances to the pellet receptacle. For quantifying the attribution of incentive salience, we employed the PCA index, which integrates three related variables for each type of response, lever presses and entrances to the feeder: 1) the number of responses, 2) the latency to the first response, and 3) the probability that at least one response occurred during the presence of the lever. SHR showed lower levels of PCA, suggesting a deficit in the attribution of incentive salience to the lever. This finding replicates the results reported by previous research that compared SHR’s performance in the PCA task against that of Sprague-Dawley rats.

自发性高血压大鼠(SHR)作为注意力缺陷多动障碍(ADHD)的动物模型已被广泛研究,因为它们表现出该障碍的一些明显特征,如某些形式的冲动和多动。然而,对于注意力缺陷多动障碍的其他特征,如动机缺陷,却很少在SHR品系中进行研究。在本报告中,我们以 45 只 SHR 大鼠和 45 只作为对比组的 Wistar 大鼠为研究对象,研究了它们将激励显著性归因于强化刺激预测因子的能力。我们采用了巴甫洛夫条件反射(PCA)任务,即在投放颗粒之前 8 秒钟投放杠杆。激励显著性归因于对杠杆的反应,而激励不显著性归因于对颗粒容器的进入。为了量化激励显著性的归因,我们采用了 PCA 指数,该指数综合了每种反应类型、按下杠杆和进入喂食器的三个相关变量:1)反应次数;2)第一次反应的潜伏期;3)在杠杆存在期间至少发生一次反应的概率。SHR的PCA水平较低,这表明其对杠杆的激励显著性归因不足。这一发现与之前研究报告的结果相同,之前的研究比较了SHR与Sprague-Dawley大鼠在PCA任务中的表现。
{"title":"Incentive-salience attribution is attenuated in spontaneously hypertensive rats, an animal model of ADHD","authors":"Fernanda González-Barriga,&nbsp;Vladimir Orduña","doi":"10.1016/j.beproc.2024.105068","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.beproc.2024.105068","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Spontaneously Hypertensive Rats (SHR) have been extensively studied as an animal model of Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) because they show some of the defining features of that disorder, like some forms of impulsivity and hyperactivity. However, other characteristics of the disorder, like a deficit in motivation, have been scarcely studied in the SHR strain. In the present report, we studied in 45 SHR and 45 Wistar rats as a comparison group, the capacity of attribution of incentive salience to a stimulus predictor of reinforcement, which has become a central concept in the study of motivation. We employed the Pavlovian Conditioned-Approach (PCA) task, in which a lever is presented 8 s before a pellet is delivered. The attribution of incentive salience is indicated by responses to the lever, in contrast to the absence of attribution of incentive salience, which is indicated by entrances to the pellet receptacle. For quantifying the attribution of incentive salience, we employed the PCA index, which integrates three related variables for each type of response, lever presses and entrances to the feeder: 1) the number of responses, 2) the latency to the first response, and 3) the probability that at least one response occurred during the presence of the lever. SHR showed lower levels of PCA, suggesting a deficit in the attribution of incentive salience to the lever. This finding replicates the results reported by previous research that compared SHR’s performance in the PCA task against that of Sprague-Dawley rats.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":8746,"journal":{"name":"Behavioural Processes","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.3,"publicationDate":"2024-06-16","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0376635724000834/pdfft?md5=e810d2ad29ae6aae17073f5f97300c83&pid=1-s2.0-S0376635724000834-main.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141402227","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Individual and group behavioral responses to nutritional state and context in a social fish 社会性鱼类个体和群体对营养状态和环境的行为反应。
IF 1.3 4区 生物学 Q2 Agricultural and Biological Sciences Pub Date : 2024-06-13 DOI: 10.1016/j.beproc.2024.105059
Ling-Qing Zeng , Hong Ling , Shi-Jian Fu , De-Yong Pu , Shaun S. Killen

Understanding how animal collectives and societies form and function is a fundamental goal in animal biology. To date, research examining fish shoaling behavior has focused mostly on the general principles and ecological relevance of the phenomeon, while the ways in which physiological state (e.g., nutrition) affects collective behavior remain overlooked. Here, we investigated the shoaling behavior of common minnows (Phoxinus phoxinus) with three different nutritional states (control treatment: fasting for 24 h, fasting treatment: fasting for 7 days, and digestion treatment: 1 h after satiation feeding) across two ecological contexts (i.e., without and with food). No effects of either nutritional state or context were found on swimming speed, but the acceleration was greater in the digestion group than in the control group, with that in the fasting group being intermediate. Similar to change tendency in group length and group width of shoals, both interindividual distance and nearest neighbor distance were also greater in the fasting group than in the digestion group, suggesting that fasting and digestion may exert opposite driving forces on group cohesion. However, neither nutritional state nor context influenced the group area, group speed, group percent time moving, or group polarization. Both the foraging efficiency and the percentage of food items consumed by the fish shoals were greater in the fasting and control groups than in the digestion group. Our study suggested that one week of hunger and the energetically demanding stage of food digestion tend to have opposite influences on group shape, while the social foraging context does not influence the individual and group behavior of fish.

了解动物集体和社会如何形成和运作是动物生物学的一个基本目标。迄今为止,对鱼类趋群行为的研究主要集中在该现象的一般原理和生态相关性上,而生理状态(如营养)对集体行为的影响方式仍被忽视。在此,我们研究了三种不同营养状态(对照处理:禁食 24 小时;禁食处理:禁食 7 天;消化处理:饱食后 1 小时)下普通鲦鱼(Phoxinus phoxinus)的趋群行为:消化处理:饱食后 1 小时)。营养状态和环境对游泳速度均无影响,但消化处理组的加速度大于对照组,禁食处理组的加速度介于两者之间。与滩涂群体长度和群体宽度的变化趋势相似,禁食组的个体间距离和近邻距离也大于消化组,这表明禁食和消化可能对群体凝聚力产生相反的驱动力。然而,营养状态和环境都不会影响群体面积、群体速度、群体移动时间百分比或群体极化。与消化组相比,禁食组和对照组鱼群的觅食效率和消耗食物的百分比都更高。我们的研究表明,饥饿一周和能量消耗大的食物消化阶段往往会对群体形状产生相反的影响,而社会觅食环境不会影响鱼类的个体和群体行为。
{"title":"Individual and group behavioral responses to nutritional state and context in a social fish","authors":"Ling-Qing Zeng ,&nbsp;Hong Ling ,&nbsp;Shi-Jian Fu ,&nbsp;De-Yong Pu ,&nbsp;Shaun S. Killen","doi":"10.1016/j.beproc.2024.105059","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.beproc.2024.105059","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Understanding how animal collectives and societies form and function is a fundamental goal in animal biology. To date, research examining fish shoaling behavior has focused mostly on the general principles and ecological relevance of the phenomeon, while the ways in which physiological state (e.g., nutrition) affects collective behavior remain overlooked. Here, we investigated the shoaling behavior of common minnows (<em>Phoxinus phoxinus</em>) with three different nutritional states (control treatment: fasting for 24 h, fasting treatment: fasting for 7 days, and digestion treatment: 1 h after satiation feeding) across two ecological contexts (i.e., without and with food). No effects of either nutritional state or context were found on swimming speed, but the acceleration was greater in the digestion group than in the control group, with that in the fasting group being intermediate. Similar to change tendency in group length and group width of shoals, both interindividual distance and nearest neighbor distance were also greater in the fasting group than in the digestion group, suggesting that fasting and digestion may exert opposite driving forces on group cohesion. However, neither nutritional state nor context influenced the group area, group speed, group percent time moving, or group polarization. Both the foraging efficiency and the percentage of food items consumed by the fish shoals were greater in the fasting and control groups than in the digestion group. Our study suggested that one week of hunger and the energetically demanding stage of food digestion tend to have opposite influences on group shape, while the social foraging context does not influence the individual and group behavior of fish.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":8746,"journal":{"name":"Behavioural Processes","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.3,"publicationDate":"2024-06-13","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141327192","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Punishment-induced resurgence 惩罚引发的复苏
IF 1.3 4区 生物学 Q2 Agricultural and Biological Sciences Pub Date : 2024-06-02 DOI: 10.1016/j.beproc.2024.105058
Ryan Redner , Kathryn M. Kestner , Amin Lotfizadeh , Alan Poling

The phenomenon of extinction-induced resurgence is well established, but there is comparatively little experimental evidence for punishment-induced resurgence. Punishment-induced resurgence can by tested by contingent shocks following the alternative response. The purpose of Experiment 1 was to test whether low-intensity shocks, that do not decrease rate of reinforcement, result in resurgence. Four rats served as subjects. Rats were exposed to three sequential conditions: (a) variable-interval (VI) 30-s food delivery for a lever press (target response); (b) VI 30-s food delivery for a nose poke (alternative response) and extinction of the lever press; (c) VI 30-s reinforcement for a nose poke with superimposed VI 60-s shock delivery. In the final condition, shocks increased gradually from 0.1 to 0.5 mA. Experiment 2 evaluated whether an abrupt introduction of a high-intensity shock would result in resurgence. Three rats served as subjects and were exposed to three sequential conditions: (a) VI 30-s food delivery for a lever press; (b) VI 30-s food delivery for a nose poke and extinction of the lever press; (c) continued VI 30-s reinforcement for a nose poke with superimposed VI 60-s 0.6 mA shock delivery. Resurgence was observed in all subjects, including in situations in which rate of responding, but not rate of reinforcement, decreased. The present study provides additional evidence for punishment-induced resurgence, but future studies are warranted to determine the extent to which punishment can produce resurgence with or without decreases in rates of reinforcement.

灭绝诱发复燃的现象已经得到证实,但关于惩罚诱发复燃的实验证据却相对较少。惩罚诱发的回潮可以通过在替代反应之后的偶然冲击进行测试。实验 1 的目的是测试不降低强化率的低强度冲击是否会导致卷土重来。实验对象为四只大鼠。大鼠被置于三种连续条件下:(a)按压杠杆(目标反应)30 秒钟的可变间隔(VI)食物投放;(b)按压鼻子(替代反应)30 秒钟的 VI 食物投放和按压杠杆的熄灭;(c)按压鼻子 30 秒钟的 VI 强化和 60 秒钟的 VI 电击投放。在最后一种情况下,电击从 0.1 mA 逐渐增加到 0.5 mA。实验 2 评估了突然引入高强度电击是否会导致癫痫复发。三只大鼠作为受试者,接受了三种连续的条件:(a)VI 30 秒食物投放,按压杠杆;(b)VI 30 秒食物投放,戳鼻子,按压杠杆消失;(c)VI 30 秒继续强化戳鼻子,同时叠加 VI 60 秒 0.6 毫安电击。在所有受试者身上都观察到了复燃现象,包括在反应率下降而非强化率下降的情况下。本研究为惩罚引起的卷土重来提供了更多证据,但今后的研究还需要确定在强化率降低或不降低强化率的情况下,惩罚能在多大程度上引起卷土重来。
{"title":"Punishment-induced resurgence","authors":"Ryan Redner ,&nbsp;Kathryn M. Kestner ,&nbsp;Amin Lotfizadeh ,&nbsp;Alan Poling","doi":"10.1016/j.beproc.2024.105058","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.beproc.2024.105058","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>The phenomenon of extinction-induced resurgence is well established, but there is comparatively little experimental evidence for punishment-induced resurgence. Punishment-induced resurgence can by tested by contingent shocks following the alternative response. The purpose of Experiment 1 was to test whether low-intensity shocks, that do not decrease rate of reinforcement, result in resurgence. Four rats served as subjects. Rats were exposed to three sequential conditions: (a) variable-interval (VI) 30-s food delivery for a lever press (target response); (b) VI 30-s food delivery for a nose poke (alternative response) and extinction of the lever press; (c) VI 30-s reinforcement for a nose poke with superimposed VI 60-s shock delivery. In the final condition, shocks increased gradually from 0.1 to 0.5 mA. Experiment 2 evaluated whether an abrupt introduction of a high-intensity shock would result in resurgence. Three rats served as subjects and were exposed to three sequential conditions: (a) VI 30-s food delivery for a lever press; (b) VI 30-s food delivery for a nose poke and extinction of the lever press; (c) continued VI 30-s reinforcement for a nose poke with superimposed VI 60-s 0.6 mA shock delivery. Resurgence was observed in all subjects, including in situations in which rate of responding, but not rate of reinforcement, decreased. The present study provides additional evidence for punishment-induced resurgence, but future studies are warranted to determine the extent to which punishment can produce resurgence with or without decreases in rates of reinforcement.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":8746,"journal":{"name":"Behavioural Processes","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.3,"publicationDate":"2024-06-02","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141228903","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Nearest neighbour distance does not affect escape behaviour in urban hooded crows 近邻距离不会影响城市帽乌鸦的逃跑行为。
IF 1.3 4区 生物学 Q2 Agricultural and Biological Sciences Pub Date : 2024-06-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.beproc.2024.105057
Ivana Novčić , Peter Mikula

The nearest-neighbour distance is an important property of a group, as individuals can obtain environmental information more quickly and easily from nearby individuals. We examined whether distance to the nearest neighbour affected two components of escape behaviour – alert distance (AD) and flight initiation distance (FID) – in an urban population of hooded crows Corvus cornix, while controlling for confounding variables. We did not find evidence that AD and FID were influenced by the nearest neighbour distance. However, both AD and FID were negatively affected by feeding activity of individuals – focal crows alerted later and escaped at shorter distance if they were feeding during our approach. In addition, AD and FID were positively related to starting distance and grass coverage. The lack of evidence for the nearest neighbour effect on escape behaviour of crows may be due to: (1) disturbance by close neighbours that may impede antipredator behaviour of focal birds, (2) variable distribution of familiar, dominant or experienced individuals within a flock, and (3) dynamic change in position of the nearest neighbour during the potential predator approach.

最近邻居距离是群体的一个重要属性,因为个体可以更快、更容易地从附近的个体那里获得环境信息。我们研究了与最近邻居的距离是否会影响乌鸦逃跑行为的两个组成部分--警戒距离(AD)和飞行起始距离(FID)--并同时控制了混杂变量。我们没有发现证据表明警戒距离和起始飞行距离受最近邻近距离的影响。但是,AD 和 FID 都受到个体觅食活动的负面影响--如果在我们接近乌鸦时乌鸦正在觅食,那么焦点乌鸦会更晚发出警报,并在更短的距离内逃离。此外,AD 和 FID 与起始距离和草地覆盖率呈正相关。没有证据表明近邻对乌鸦逃跑行为有影响,原因可能是(1)近邻的干扰可能会阻碍重点鸟类的反捕食行为,(2)鸟群中熟悉的、占优势的或有经验的个体的分布不均,以及(3)在潜在捕食者接近过程中近邻位置的动态变化。
{"title":"Nearest neighbour distance does not affect escape behaviour in urban hooded crows","authors":"Ivana Novčić ,&nbsp;Peter Mikula","doi":"10.1016/j.beproc.2024.105057","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.beproc.2024.105057","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>The nearest-neighbour distance is an important property of a group, as individuals can obtain environmental information more quickly and easily from nearby individuals. We examined whether distance to the nearest neighbour affected two components of escape behaviour – alert distance (AD) and flight initiation distance (FID) – in an urban population of hooded crows <em>Corvus cornix</em>, while controlling for confounding variables. We did not find evidence that AD and FID were influenced by the nearest neighbour distance. However, both AD and FID were negatively affected by feeding activity of individuals – focal crows alerted later and escaped at shorter distance if they were feeding during our approach. In addition, AD and FID were positively related to starting distance and grass coverage. The lack of evidence for the nearest neighbour effect on escape behaviour of crows may be due to: (1) disturbance by close neighbours that may impede antipredator behaviour of focal birds, (2) variable distribution of familiar, dominant or experienced individuals within a flock, and (3) dynamic change in position of the nearest neighbour during the potential predator approach.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":8746,"journal":{"name":"Behavioural Processes","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.3,"publicationDate":"2024-06-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141092591","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Detection of relatedness in chemical alarm cues by a selfing vertebrate depends on population and the life stage producing the alarm cue 自交脊椎动物对化学警报线索中亲缘关系的检测取决于产生警报线索的种群和生命阶段。
IF 1.3 4区 生物学 Q2 Agricultural and Biological Sciences Pub Date : 2024-06-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.beproc.2024.105056
Kaitlyn Nunley, Katie E. McGhee

Aquatic prey have impressive abilities to extract information from a variety of chemical cues. For example, they can use the alarm cues released by wounded individuals during a predator attack to learn about predation risk, and they can also distinguish kin from non-kin individuals during interactions. However, it remains unclear whether animals can combine this information on predation risk with kin recognition of the particular individuals under threat. To examine how the relatedness of the individuals in alarm cue affects behaviour we used the self-fertilizing hermaphroditic mangrove rivulus (Kryptolebias marmoratus), in which lineages produce genetically identical offspring through selfing. We explored this in two populations that differ in their level of outcrossing. We measured activity before and after exposure to alarm cue made from individuals (either adults or embryos) from their own lineage or an unrelated lineage from the same population. Fish responded weakly to embryo alarm cues, but tended to reduce their activity more when the alarm cues were from an unrelated lineage compared to alarm cues from their own lineage, particularly in fish from the outcrossing population. In contrast, there was no effect of cue relatedness on the response to adult alarm cues but there was a strong population effect. Specifically, individuals from the outcrossing population tended to react more strongly to alarm cues compared to individuals from the predominantly selfing population. We discuss the potential roles of the major histocompatibility complex in cue detection, differences between adult vs embryo alarm cues in terms of concentration and information, and underlying differences among populations and genetic lineages in their production and detection of chemical cues. Whether this kin recognition offers adaptive benefits or is simply a consequence of being able to detect relatedness in living individuals would be an exciting area for future research.

水生猎物从各种化学线索中提取信息的能力令人印象深刻。例如,它们可以利用捕食者攻击时受伤个体释放的警报信号来了解捕食风险,还可以在互动过程中区分亲缘和非亲缘个体。然而,动物能否将捕食风险信息与对特定受威胁个体的亲缘识别结合起来,目前仍不清楚。为了研究报警线索中个体的亲缘关系如何影响行为,我们使用了自交雌雄同体的红树林杓鹬(Kryptolebias marmoratus)。我们在两个外交程度不同的种群中对此进行了探索。我们测量了鱼类在接触来自其自身种系或来自同一种群的不相关种系的个体(成鱼或胚胎)发出的警报提示前后的活动。鱼类对胚胎报警线索的反应较弱,但当来自非亲缘种系的报警线索比来自本种系的报警线索时,鱼类的活动量往往会减少,尤其是来自杂交种群的鱼类。相反,线索的亲缘关系对成鱼对警报线索的反应没有影响,但存在强烈的种群效应。具体来说,与主要来自自交种群的个体相比,来自外交种群的个体倾向于对警报信号做出更强烈的反应。我们讨论了主要组织相容性复合体在线索检测中的潜在作用、成体与胚胎报警线索在浓度和信息方面的差异,以及不同种群和遗传系在产生和检测化学线索方面的潜在差异。这种亲属识别是否会带来适应性益处,或者仅仅是能够检测到活体亲缘关系的结果,这将是未来研究的一个令人兴奋的领域。
{"title":"Detection of relatedness in chemical alarm cues by a selfing vertebrate depends on population and the life stage producing the alarm cue","authors":"Kaitlyn Nunley,&nbsp;Katie E. McGhee","doi":"10.1016/j.beproc.2024.105056","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.beproc.2024.105056","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Aquatic prey have impressive abilities to extract information from a variety of chemical cues. For example, they can use the alarm cues released by wounded individuals during a predator attack to learn about predation risk, and they can also distinguish kin from non-kin individuals during interactions. However, it remains unclear whether animals can combine this information on predation risk with kin recognition of the particular individuals under threat. To examine how the relatedness of the individuals in alarm cue affects behaviour we used the self-fertilizing hermaphroditic mangrove rivulus (<em>Kryptolebias marmoratus</em>), in which lineages produce genetically identical offspring through selfing. We explored this in two populations that differ in their level of outcrossing. We measured activity before and after exposure to alarm cue made from individuals (either adults or embryos) from their own lineage or an unrelated lineage from the same population. Fish responded weakly to embryo alarm cues, but tended to reduce their activity more when the alarm cues were from an unrelated lineage compared to alarm cues from their own lineage, particularly in fish from the outcrossing population. In contrast, there was no effect of cue relatedness on the response to adult alarm cues but there was a strong population effect. Specifically, individuals from the outcrossing population tended to react more strongly to alarm cues compared to individuals from the predominantly selfing population. We discuss the potential roles of the major histocompatibility complex in cue detection, differences between adult vs embryo alarm cues in terms of concentration and information, and underlying differences among populations and genetic lineages in their production and detection of chemical cues. Whether this kin recognition offers adaptive benefits or is simply a consequence of being able to detect relatedness in living individuals would be an exciting area for future research.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":8746,"journal":{"name":"Behavioural Processes","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.3,"publicationDate":"2024-06-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141086697","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Acute hypoxia exposure rapidly triggers behavioral changes linked to cutaneous gas exchange in Lake Titicaca frogs 急性缺氧会迅速引发的的喀喀湖蛙与皮肤气体交换有关的行为变化。
IF 1.3 4区 生物学 Q2 Agricultural and Biological Sciences Pub Date : 2024-06-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.beproc.2024.105047
Jordan De Padova , Nigel K. Anderson , Roland Halbauer , Doris Preininger , Matthew J. Fuxjager

Ventilation is critical to animal life—it ensures that individuals move air/water across their respiratory surface, and thus it sustains gas exchange with the environment. Many species have evolved highly specialized (if not unusual) ventilatory mechanisms, including the use of behavior to facilitate different aspects of breathing. However, these behavioral traits are often only described anecdotally, and the ecological conditions that elicit them are typically unclear. We study one such “ventilation behavior” in Lake Titicaca frogs (Telmatobius culeus). These frogs inhabit high-altitude (i.e., low oxygen) lakes in the Andean Mountains of South America, and they have become textbook examples of cutaneous gas exchange, which is essentially breathing that occurs across the skin. Accordingly, this species has evolved large, baggy skin-folds that dangle from the body to increase the surface area for ventilation. We show that individuals exposed to acute hypoxic conditions that mirror what free-living individuals likely encounter quickly (within minutes) decrease their activity levels, and thus become very still. If oxygen levels continue to decline, the frogs soon begin to perform push-up behaviors that presumably break the low-oxygen boundary layer around skin-folds to increase the conductance of the water/skin gas exchange pathway. Altogether, we suspect that individuals rapidly adjust aspects of their behavior in response to seemingly sudden changes to the oxygen environment as a mechanism to fine tune cutaneous respiration.

通气对动物的生命至关重要--它确保个体在呼吸表面移动空气/水,从而维持与环境的气体交换。许多物种已经进化出高度专业化(如果不是不寻常的话)的通气机制,包括利用行为来促进呼吸的不同方面。然而,这些行为特征往往只是传闻,而且引发这些行为的生态条件通常也不明确。我们对的的喀喀湖蛙(Telmatobius culeus)的这种 "换气行为 "进行了研究。这些青蛙栖息在南美洲安第斯山脉的高海拔(即低氧)湖泊中,它们已成为皮肤气体交换的教科书范例,这种交换基本上是通过皮肤进行的呼吸。因此,该物种进化出了大而宽松的皮肤褶皱,这些褶皱从身体垂下,以增加换气的表面积。我们的研究表明,暴露在急性缺氧条件下的个体会迅速(在几分钟内)降低活动水平,从而变得一动不动。如果氧气水平继续下降,蛙类很快就会开始俯卧撑行为,这种行为可能会打破皮肤褶皱周围的低氧边界层,从而增加水/皮肤气体交换途径的传导性。总之,我们怀疑个体会根据氧气环境的看似突然的变化迅速调整其行为的各个方面,以此作为一种微调皮肤呼吸的机制。
{"title":"Acute hypoxia exposure rapidly triggers behavioral changes linked to cutaneous gas exchange in Lake Titicaca frogs","authors":"Jordan De Padova ,&nbsp;Nigel K. Anderson ,&nbsp;Roland Halbauer ,&nbsp;Doris Preininger ,&nbsp;Matthew J. Fuxjager","doi":"10.1016/j.beproc.2024.105047","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.beproc.2024.105047","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Ventilation is critical to animal life—it ensures that individuals move air/water across their respiratory surface, and thus it sustains gas exchange with the environment. Many species have evolved highly specialized (if not unusual) ventilatory mechanisms, including the use of behavior to facilitate different aspects of breathing. However, these behavioral traits are often only described anecdotally, and the ecological conditions that elicit them are typically unclear. We study one such “ventilation behavior” in Lake Titicaca frogs (<em>Telmatobius culeus</em>). These frogs inhabit high-altitude (i.e., low oxygen) lakes in the Andean Mountains of South America, and they have become textbook examples of cutaneous gas exchange, which is essentially breathing that occurs across the skin. Accordingly, this species has evolved large, baggy skin-folds that dangle from the body to increase the surface area for ventilation. We show that individuals exposed to acute hypoxic conditions that mirror what free-living individuals likely encounter quickly (within minutes) decrease their activity levels, and thus become very still. If oxygen levels continue to decline, the frogs soon begin to perform push-up behaviors that presumably break the low-oxygen boundary layer around skin-folds to increase the conductance of the water/skin gas exchange pathway. Altogether, we suspect that individuals rapidly adjust aspects of their behavior in response to seemingly sudden changes to the oxygen environment as a mechanism to fine tune cutaneous respiration.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":8746,"journal":{"name":"Behavioural Processes","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.3,"publicationDate":"2024-06-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140955549","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Observation of female-male mounting in the carrion crow 观察腐尸乌鸦的雌雄交配。
IF 1.3 4区 生物学 Q2 Agricultural and Biological Sciences Pub Date : 2024-06-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.beproc.2024.105055
Claudia A.F. Wascher , Friederike Hillemann

In the biological sciences, sexual behaviours in non-human animals are traditionally investigated in the context of reproduction and direct fitness benefits. While the evolutionary functions of non-conceptive sexual behaviours (‘socio-sexual behaviours’) remain less well explored, these interactions and displays have been suggested to be important for shaping and maintaining social relationships. Here, we report an observation of a captive female carrion crow, Corvus corone corone, mounting her co-housed male partner. We highlight the importance of more systematic research, reporting, and discussions of rarely observed behaviours in social evolution research, including considerations for behaviours that transcend binary or heteronormative frameworks, for a more comprehensive understanding of non-conceptive socio-sexual behaviours.

在生物科学领域,对非人类动物性行为的研究历来都是以繁殖和直接的健康益处为背景的。虽然对非受孕性行为("社会性行为")的进化功能探索较少,但这些互动和展示被认为对塑造和维持社会关系非常重要。在此,我们报告了对圈养的雌性腐食乌鸦(Corvus corone corone)上马其共同栖息的雄性伙伴的观察结果。我们强调了在社会进化研究中对很少观察到的行为进行更系统的研究和讨论的重要性,包括对超越二元或异性恋框架的行为的考虑,以便更全面地了解非感知的社会性行为。
{"title":"Observation of female-male mounting in the carrion crow","authors":"Claudia A.F. Wascher ,&nbsp;Friederike Hillemann","doi":"10.1016/j.beproc.2024.105055","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.beproc.2024.105055","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>In the biological sciences, sexual behaviours in non-human animals are traditionally investigated in the context of reproduction and direct fitness benefits. While the evolutionary functions of non-conceptive sexual behaviours (‘socio-sexual behaviours’) remain less well explored, these interactions and displays have been suggested to be important for shaping and maintaining social relationships. Here, we report an observation of a captive female carrion crow, <em>Corvus corone corone</em>, mounting her co-housed male partner. We highlight the importance of more systematic research, reporting, and discussions of rarely observed behaviours in social evolution research, including considerations for behaviours that transcend binary or heteronormative frameworks, for a more comprehensive understanding of non-conceptive socio-sexual behaviours.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":8746,"journal":{"name":"Behavioural Processes","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.3,"publicationDate":"2024-06-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0376635724000706/pdfft?md5=9c257a24444301e1cb5b49d6bb3d0d56&pid=1-s2.0-S0376635724000706-main.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141080389","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
User behaviors in a community-based multifunctional urban park with dog friendly area 社区多功能城市公园(含爱犬区)的用户行为。
IF 1.3 4区 生物学 Q2 Agricultural and Biological Sciences Pub Date : 2024-05-20 DOI: 10.1016/j.beproc.2024.105048
Yuika Kase, Naoko Koda

Urban parks are essential for community revitalization; for example, they are places to walk dogs and interact with other dog keepers. This study focused on an urban park with a dog-friendly area to be used by both dog keepers and other users as an alternative to off-leash dog parks that completely separate them and clarified aspects of park use through behavioral observation. The behaviors of 7122 visitors in 14 areas in the park and 294 pairs of dogs and their keepers in the dog-friendly area were observed. The results showed that the visitors’ age groups and use behaviors differed by area. The dog-friendly area was in constant demand among dog keepers as a place where they could stay and interact with others and as a destination or relay point when walking their dogs. Visitors used the park in accordance with rules and morals, and the park was well managed. As it can be comfortably used by everyone (with or without dogs), this park can serve as a model for the development and maintenance of community-based multifunctional parks in urban areas.

城市公园对社区振兴至关重要;例如,它们是遛狗和与其他养狗人交流的场所。本研究的重点是一个城市公园,该公园设有一个供养狗者和其他使用者共同使用的爱犬区,作为将两者完全分开的非拴养狗公园的替代方案,并通过行为观察明确公园使用的各个方面。在公园的 14 个区域观察了 7122 名游客的行为,在爱犬区观察了 294 对狗和它们的饲养者的行为。结果显示,不同区域的游客年龄段和使用行为各不相同。爱犬区是养犬人经常需要的地方,他们可以在此逗留并与他人交流,同时也是遛狗时的目的地或中转站。游客在使用公园时遵守规则和道德,公园管理良好。由于每个人(带狗或不带狗)都可以舒适地使用该公园,因此该公园可以作为在城市地区开发和维护以社区为基础的多功能公园的典范。
{"title":"User behaviors in a community-based multifunctional urban park with dog friendly area","authors":"Yuika Kase,&nbsp;Naoko Koda","doi":"10.1016/j.beproc.2024.105048","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.beproc.2024.105048","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Urban parks are essential for community revitalization; for example, they are places to walk dogs and interact with other dog keepers. This study focused on an urban park with a dog-friendly area to be used by both dog keepers and other users as an alternative to off-leash dog parks that completely separate them and clarified aspects of park use through behavioral observation. The behaviors of 7122 visitors in 14 areas in the park and 294 pairs of dogs and their keepers in the dog-friendly area were observed. The results showed that the visitors’ age groups and use behaviors differed by area. The dog-friendly area was in constant demand among dog keepers as a place where they could stay and interact with others and as a destination or relay point when walking their dogs. Visitors used the park in accordance with rules and morals, and the park was well managed. As it can be comfortably used by everyone (with or without dogs), this park can serve as a model for the development and maintenance of community-based multifunctional parks in urban areas.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":8746,"journal":{"name":"Behavioural Processes","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.3,"publicationDate":"2024-05-20","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141080390","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Effects of delay sequence in a delay discounting task 延迟折扣任务中延迟顺序的影响。
IF 1.3 4区 生物学 Q2 Agricultural and Biological Sciences Pub Date : 2024-05-17 DOI: 10.1016/j.beproc.2024.105046
J.R. Macías-Navarrete, C.V. dos Santos

Delay discounting refers to the decrease in subjective value of a reward as the delay until its receipt increases. In the present study we assessed the effects of the sequence of delay blocks (increasing or decreasing) on discounting and the data systematicity using a titrating procedure with human participants. All participants completed the delay discounting task in both an increasing and decreasing sequence of delays. Delays ranged from one day to ten years. We found steeper discounting when the delays were presented in an increasing sequence compared with when they were presented in a decreasing sequence. We also found steeper discounting when participants completed the increasing sequence condition first. Our results agree with other findings reported in the literature and suggest that delay discounting may be affected by prior and subsequent experience.

延迟折现是指随着获得奖励的延迟时间增加,奖励的主观价值也随之降低。在本研究中,我们采用滴定法评估了延迟区块顺序(递增或递减)对折现和数据系统性的影响。所有参与者在延迟一天到十年的情况下,按照递增和递减的顺序完成了延迟折现任务。我们发现,当延迟区块以递增顺序呈现时,与以递减顺序呈现时相比,延迟折现明显更陡峭。我们还发现,当参与者先完成递增序列条件时,他们的折现率明显更高。我们的研究结果与文献中报道的其他研究结果一致,表明延迟折现可能会受到之前和之后经验的影响。
{"title":"Effects of delay sequence in a delay discounting task","authors":"J.R. Macías-Navarrete,&nbsp;C.V. dos Santos","doi":"10.1016/j.beproc.2024.105046","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.beproc.2024.105046","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Delay discounting refers to the decrease in subjective value of a reward as the delay until its receipt increases. In the present study we assessed the effects of the sequence of delay blocks (increasing or decreasing) on discounting and the data systematicity using a titrating procedure with human participants. All participants completed the delay discounting task in both an increasing and decreasing sequence of delays. Delays ranged from one day to ten years. We found steeper discounting when the delays were presented in an increasing sequence compared with when they were presented in a decreasing sequence. We also found steeper discounting when participants completed the increasing sequence condition first. Our results agree with other findings reported in the literature and suggest that delay discounting may be affected by prior and subsequent experience.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":8746,"journal":{"name":"Behavioural Processes","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.3,"publicationDate":"2024-05-17","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140955553","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Autoshaped impulsivity: Some explorations with a neural network model 自形冲动:神经网络模型的一些探索
IF 1.3 4区 生物学 Q2 Agricultural and Biological Sciences Pub Date : 2024-05-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.beproc.2024.105040
Miguel Aguayo-Mendoza, Jonathan Buriticá, José E. Burgos

This study evaluated the effect of delay and magnitude of reinforcement in Pavlovian contingencies, extending the understanding of the phenomenon of autoshaped impulsivity as described in Alcalá's thesis (2017) and Burgos and García-Leal (2015). The effects of adding a trace interval were analyzed on the maintained responses of impulsive choice, seen as the preference of a small and immediate reinforcer over a larger and delayed one, and the role of the contextual unit, as well as the inhibitory units according to the Diffuse Discrepancy Model. In the Simulation, the model with inhibitory units was used, trained in two signals with different delays and reinforcement magnitudes, and subsequently presented concurrently in choice tasks without reinforcement nor learning, using an ABA within-subject design. In general, the DD model successfully simulated the phenomenon of autoshaped impulsivity, consistent with studies from Alcalá's thesis (2017), Burgos and García-Leal (2015), and Picker and Poling (1982). It also predicted the elimination of this effect (autoshaped impulsivity) after introducing a trace interval. The observed results and their implications are discussed, as well as possible future studies with animals and humans.

本研究评估了巴甫洛夫或然条件中强化的延迟和幅度的影响,扩展了对阿尔卡拉论文(2017 年)和布尔戈斯与加西亚-雷尔论文(2015 年)中描述的自形冲动现象的理解。根据弥散差异模型,我们分析了添加追踪间隔对冲动选择的维持反应的影响,冲动选择被视为对小的和即时的强化物的偏好,而不是对大的和延迟的强化物的偏好,并分析了情境单元以及抑制单元的作用。在 "模拟 "中,我们使用了带有抑制单元的模型,在两种具有不同延迟和强化幅度的信号中进行训练,随后在没有强化也没有学习的选择任务中同时呈现,采用 ABA 受试者内设计。总的来说,DD 模型成功地模拟了自形冲动现象,这与 Alcalá 的论文(2017 年)、Burgos 和 García-Leal (2015 年)以及 Picker 和 Poling (1982 年)的研究一致。它还预测了在引入追踪间隔后这种效应(自形冲动)的消除。本文讨论了观察到的结果及其影响,以及未来可能对动物和人类进行的研究。
{"title":"Autoshaped impulsivity: Some explorations with a neural network model","authors":"Miguel Aguayo-Mendoza,&nbsp;Jonathan Buriticá,&nbsp;José E. Burgos","doi":"10.1016/j.beproc.2024.105040","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.beproc.2024.105040","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>This study evaluated the effect of delay and magnitude of reinforcement in Pavlovian contingencies, extending the understanding of the phenomenon of autoshaped impulsivity as described in Alcalá's thesis (2017) and Burgos and García-Leal (2015). The effects of adding a trace interval were analyzed on the maintained responses of impulsive choice, seen as the preference of a small and immediate reinforcer over a larger and delayed one, and the role of the contextual unit, as well as the inhibitory units according to the Diffuse Discrepancy Model. In the Simulation, the model with inhibitory units was used, trained in two signals with different delays and reinforcement magnitudes, and subsequently presented concurrently in choice tasks without reinforcement nor learning, using an ABA within-subject design. In general, the DD model successfully simulated the phenomenon of autoshaped impulsivity, consistent with studies from Alcalá's thesis (2017), Burgos and García-Leal (2015), and Picker and Poling (1982). It also predicted the elimination of this effect (autoshaped impulsivity) after introducing a trace interval. The observed results and their implications are discussed, as well as possible future studies with animals and humans.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":8746,"journal":{"name":"Behavioural Processes","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.3,"publicationDate":"2024-05-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140815681","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Behavioural Processes
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1