Pub Date : 2023-01-23DOI: 10.1163/1568539x-bja10200
Yunmeng Zhu, Yuqian Wang, Yuyu Wang, Yiqin Feng, Qun Wang, Qiao Wang, Yaoyao Tian, F. Tai, Rui Jia
Parent–infant bonds are critical early postnatal environment in mammals. Unstable parent–infant bonds, such as maternal separation, paternal deprivation, and neonatal social isolation have negative effects on emotion, cognition, addiction, and social behaviours in the animal’s whole lifespan. Another unstable parent–infant bond, cross-fostering, in which pups were randomly exchanged to other parents has mostly focused on kin recognition and anxiety-like behavioural changes in adult. But its effects on adolescence, particularly on autism spectrum disorder, are poorly understood. Whole cross-fostering model was used in the present study. The results showed that with cross-fostering, adolescent Kunming mice buried more marbles, expressed higher levels of anxiety and depression. Cross-fostering attenuated prosocial behaviours and reduced the activity of adolescence when encountering another individual in social interaction test. Taken together, our results demonstrate that experiencing whole cross-fostering in early life is more likely to induce autism-, anxiety-, and depression- like behaviours in adolescence.
{"title":"Effects of cross-fostering experience on emotion in adolescent Kunming mice","authors":"Yunmeng Zhu, Yuqian Wang, Yuyu Wang, Yiqin Feng, Qun Wang, Qiao Wang, Yaoyao Tian, F. Tai, Rui Jia","doi":"10.1163/1568539x-bja10200","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1163/1568539x-bja10200","url":null,"abstract":"\u0000Parent–infant bonds are critical early postnatal environment in mammals. Unstable parent–infant bonds, such as maternal separation, paternal deprivation, and neonatal social isolation have negative effects on emotion, cognition, addiction, and social behaviours in the animal’s whole lifespan. Another unstable parent–infant bond, cross-fostering, in which pups were randomly exchanged to other parents has mostly focused on kin recognition and anxiety-like behavioural changes in adult. But its effects on adolescence, particularly on autism spectrum disorder, are poorly understood. Whole cross-fostering model was used in the present study. The results showed that with cross-fostering, adolescent Kunming mice buried more marbles, expressed higher levels of anxiety and depression. Cross-fostering attenuated prosocial behaviours and reduced the activity of adolescence when encountering another individual in social interaction test. Taken together, our results demonstrate that experiencing whole cross-fostering in early life is more likely to induce autism-, anxiety-, and depression- like behaviours in adolescence.","PeriodicalId":8822,"journal":{"name":"Behaviour","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.3,"publicationDate":"2023-01-23","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"44607805","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
Pub Date : 2023-01-23DOI: 10.1163/1568539x-bja10194
A. Klimley
A historical example, case study 2, is one of two cases illustrating the complexity of interactions between sharks in their natural environment. Evidence is presented that the white shark is a selective feeder, consuming seals and sea lions with high fat content yet rejecting a pelican, human, and sea otters with low fat content. After biting seals, these sharks carry them in their jaws for a prolonged time underwater to kill them through blood-loss, i.e., exsanguination. Multiple sharks compete to feed on the remaining seal carcass when it floats to the surface. The sharks perform the Tail Slap, consisting of lifting the caudal fin and splashing water toward the accompanying shark. A combatant is permitted to feed further on a seal only if the vigor and frequency of its tail slaps are greater than those of its opponent. Thus, this is an agonistic display, functioning to ward off potential competitors.
{"title":"A historical approach to describing the complex behaviour of a large species of predatory shark — case study 2: the white shark, Carcharodon carcharias","authors":"A. Klimley","doi":"10.1163/1568539x-bja10194","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1163/1568539x-bja10194","url":null,"abstract":"\u0000A historical example, case study 2, is one of two cases illustrating the complexity of interactions between sharks in their natural environment. Evidence is presented that the white shark is a selective feeder, consuming seals and sea lions with high fat content yet rejecting a pelican, human, and sea otters with low fat content. After biting seals, these sharks carry them in their jaws for a prolonged time underwater to kill them through blood-loss, i.e., exsanguination. Multiple sharks compete to feed on the remaining seal carcass when it floats to the surface. The sharks perform the Tail Slap, consisting of lifting the caudal fin and splashing water toward the accompanying shark. A combatant is permitted to feed further on a seal only if the vigor and frequency of its tail slaps are greater than those of its opponent. Thus, this is an agonistic display, functioning to ward off potential competitors.","PeriodicalId":8822,"journal":{"name":"Behaviour","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.3,"publicationDate":"2023-01-23","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"42307454","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
Pub Date : 2023-01-18DOI: 10.1163/1568539X-bja10202
Lauriane Bégué, Mélissa Peignier, Eva Ringler
Environmental variation plays a key role in the evolution and maintenance of animal personality. Individuals with different personality types might exhibit different habitat preferences. Alternatively, variation in individual behaviour across space could arise as a plastic adaptation to distinct habitats. Our study aims to investigate if habitat choice is influenced by an individual's personality. We assessed individual levels of activity, boldness, and exploration in male poison frogs, and performed a habitat choice test under controlled laboratory conditions. Individuals were consistent in their behaviours, but all tested frogs chose the complex over the simple habitat. Individuals that were characterized as bold and very explorative also showed more movements between the two different habitats in the choice test. These results indicate that personality measured in a highly standardized artificial setup, such as a novel environment test, indeed can reflect boldness and exploration related behaviours measured in a more naturalistic setup.
{"title":"The link between animal personality and habitat selection in males of the Neotropical poison frog <i>Allobates femoralis</i>.","authors":"Lauriane Bégué, Mélissa Peignier, Eva Ringler","doi":"10.1163/1568539X-bja10202","DOIUrl":"10.1163/1568539X-bja10202","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Environmental variation plays a key role in the evolution and maintenance of animal personality. Individuals with different personality types might exhibit different habitat preferences. Alternatively, variation in individual behaviour across space could arise as a plastic adaptation to distinct habitats. Our study aims to investigate if habitat choice is influenced by an individual's personality. We assessed individual levels of activity, boldness, and exploration in male poison frogs, and performed a habitat choice test under controlled laboratory conditions. Individuals were consistent in their behaviours, but all tested frogs chose the complex over the simple habitat. Individuals that were characterized as bold and very explorative also showed more movements between the two different habitats in the choice test. These results indicate that personality measured in a highly standardized artificial setup, such as a novel environment test, indeed can reflect boldness and exploration related behaviours measured in a more naturalistic setup.</p>","PeriodicalId":8822,"journal":{"name":"Behaviour","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.2,"publicationDate":"2023-01-18","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC7616150/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"48698815","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
Pub Date : 2023-01-18DOI: 10.1163/1568539x-bja10201
A. Klimley, Mauricio Hoyos-Padilla
In this short note, we describe the convulsive body shuddering of a white shark as it approached two large metallic shark cages, each with multiple divers standing within them. When animals feel threatened, they experience conflicting instincts — one is to escape and another is to fight. In this situation, they do not always fight but often perform an agonistic, or aggressive display. Having arrived at the source of an olfactory corridor, this white shark was confronted with highly visible cages made with aluminium bars. The divers use hookah air hoses to breathe, and were therefore releasing bubbles, which reflect light and generate sounds as they oscillate toward the surface. The photographers may also have been taking pictures of the shark with their flash-bulb equipped cameras, which produce a sudden disruptive flash of irradiance. The shark’s behaviour is illustrated with a series of video frames as he approaches the cage. The body of the shark shutters convulsively and he opens his mouth, keeping it open for a prolonged period of 2.8 s as he passes close to the cage, while (1) depressing his pectoral fins, (2) hunching his back, (3) keeping his caudal fin held at right angle to the axis of view to increase his apparent size, and (4) shaking his body with spasmodic oscillations. The shark appears frightened, and hence may perform the display to discourage any aggression directed at him by the cage with humans emitting a panoply of frightening stimuli. Alternative explanations of the motivation behind this behaviour are also discussed. We hope that it will lead other scientists to look for this behaviour when observing the behaviour of white sharks from a cage, so they can provide further evidence shedding light upon the shark’s motivation for performing this conspicuous behaviour.
{"title":"Could convulsive body shuddering of a white shark near a shark cage be an element of a threat display?","authors":"A. Klimley, Mauricio Hoyos-Padilla","doi":"10.1163/1568539x-bja10201","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1163/1568539x-bja10201","url":null,"abstract":"\u0000In this short note, we describe the convulsive body shuddering of a white shark as it approached two large metallic shark cages, each with multiple divers standing within them. When animals feel threatened, they experience conflicting instincts — one is to escape and another is to fight. In this situation, they do not always fight but often perform an agonistic, or aggressive display. Having arrived at the source of an olfactory corridor, this white shark was confronted with highly visible cages made with aluminium bars. The divers use hookah air hoses to breathe, and were therefore releasing bubbles, which reflect light and generate sounds as they oscillate toward the surface. The photographers may also have been taking pictures of the shark with their flash-bulb equipped cameras, which produce a sudden disruptive flash of irradiance. The shark’s behaviour is illustrated with a series of video frames as he approaches the cage. The body of the shark shutters convulsively and he opens his mouth, keeping it open for a prolonged period of 2.8 s as he passes close to the cage, while (1) depressing his pectoral fins, (2) hunching his back, (3) keeping his caudal fin held at right angle to the axis of view to increase his apparent size, and (4) shaking his body with spasmodic oscillations. The shark appears frightened, and hence may perform the display to discourage any aggression directed at him by the cage with humans emitting a panoply of frightening stimuli. Alternative explanations of the motivation behind this behaviour are also discussed. We hope that it will lead other scientists to look for this behaviour when observing the behaviour of white sharks from a cage, so they can provide further evidence shedding light upon the shark’s motivation for performing this conspicuous behaviour.","PeriodicalId":8822,"journal":{"name":"Behaviour","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.3,"publicationDate":"2023-01-18","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"44337901","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
Pub Date : 2022-12-14DOI: 10.1163/1568539x-bja10199
Rebecca Umeed, K. Lucchini, P. Santos, F. Attademo, F. Luna, I. Normande, B. Bezerra
Vocal complexity can be expressed through variations in repertoire size, structure, and individual manatee repertoires. Here we aimed to assess the complexity of the vocal behaviour of Antillean manatees living in captivity (i.e., artificial pools) and in reintroduction enclosures (i.e., natural enclosures placed in an estuarine area). Specifically, we evaluated: (i) the structure of vocalisations to assess whether they had variants; (ii) the variation in call production (rate and pattern) between groups with different configurations; (iii) whether individuality occurred in vocalisation structure. We found four categories of vocalisations, of which two had different variants. Not all study groups produced all call categories and variants. Older and younger males in the reintroduction enclosures had the highest call rates compared to captive females and captive males. The vocal and behavioural patterns differed between groups. Squeak call structure differed between individuals. Such vocal complexity may aid manatees in adapting to their dynamic social and structural environment, facilitating communication.
{"title":"Vocal complexity in Antillean manatees (Trichechus manatus manatus)","authors":"Rebecca Umeed, K. Lucchini, P. Santos, F. Attademo, F. Luna, I. Normande, B. Bezerra","doi":"10.1163/1568539x-bja10199","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1163/1568539x-bja10199","url":null,"abstract":"Vocal complexity can be expressed through variations in repertoire size, structure, and individual manatee repertoires. Here we aimed to assess the complexity of the vocal behaviour of Antillean manatees living in captivity (i.e., artificial pools) and in reintroduction enclosures (i.e., natural enclosures placed in an estuarine area). Specifically, we evaluated: (i) the structure of vocalisations to assess whether they had variants; (ii) the variation in call production (rate and pattern) between groups with different configurations; (iii) whether individuality occurred in vocalisation structure. We found four categories of vocalisations, of which two had different variants. Not all study groups produced all call categories and variants. Older and younger males in the reintroduction enclosures had the highest call rates compared to captive females and captive males. The vocal and behavioural patterns differed between groups. Squeak call structure differed between individuals. Such vocal complexity may aid manatees in adapting to their dynamic social and structural environment, facilitating communication.","PeriodicalId":8822,"journal":{"name":"Behaviour","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.3,"publicationDate":"2022-12-14","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"46477511","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
Pub Date : 2022-12-07DOI: 10.1163/1568539x-bja10195
D. Canestrari, Eva Trapote, Marta Vila, V. Baglione
Adult sexual interest in dependent young, such as lactating pups or nestlings, has been reported in a few species, with copulations with related dependent young being, at best, extremely rare. Here we document two events of copulations on a nestling by an adult caregiver in a group of kin-living and cooperatively breeding carrion crows Corvus corone. Copulations, as well as other unusual rough actions toward the nestling, occurred within a short time-span characterized by intense conflict among adult group members, suggesting that stress could trigger such behaviours toward the brood.
{"title":"Copulations with a nestling by an adult care-giver in a kin-living bird","authors":"D. Canestrari, Eva Trapote, Marta Vila, V. Baglione","doi":"10.1163/1568539x-bja10195","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1163/1568539x-bja10195","url":null,"abstract":"\u0000Adult sexual interest in dependent young, such as lactating pups or nestlings, has been reported in a few species, with copulations with related dependent young being, at best, extremely rare. Here we document two events of copulations on a nestling by an adult caregiver in a group of kin-living and cooperatively breeding carrion crows Corvus corone. Copulations, as well as other unusual rough actions toward the nestling, occurred within a short time-span characterized by intense conflict among adult group members, suggesting that stress could trigger such behaviours toward the brood.","PeriodicalId":8822,"journal":{"name":"Behaviour","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.3,"publicationDate":"2022-12-07","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"46370467","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
Pub Date : 2022-12-07DOI: 10.1163/1568539x-bja10193
T. Goerge, D. Miles
Lizards engage in push-up displays to signal dominance and to secure access to important resources. The rate and patterns of push-up displays have been shown to vary based on both biotic and abiotic factors. We investigated push-up display rate in tree lizards, Urosaurus ornatus, to determine contributions from potentially conflicting factors including sex, throat colour, microhabitat usage, social context, and thermal traits. We found that display rate was best described by an interaction between microhabitat and body temperature (Tb). The relationship between display rate and Tb was significantly different between three microhabitats: sunny dead trees, the inner branches of trees, and tree trunks. We suggest that this variation in display rate is driven by shifts in microhabitat temperature over the course of the day and spatial and temporal adjustments being made depending on the probabilities of being detected by both conspecifics and predators.
{"title":"Habitat use and body temperature influence push-up display rate in the tree lizard, Urosaurus ornatus","authors":"T. Goerge, D. Miles","doi":"10.1163/1568539x-bja10193","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1163/1568539x-bja10193","url":null,"abstract":"Lizards engage in push-up displays to signal dominance and to secure access to important resources. The rate and patterns of push-up displays have been shown to vary based on both biotic and abiotic factors. We investigated push-up display rate in tree lizards, Urosaurus ornatus, to determine contributions from potentially conflicting factors including sex, throat colour, microhabitat usage, social context, and thermal traits. We found that display rate was best described by an interaction between microhabitat and body temperature (Tb). The relationship between display rate and Tb was significantly different between three microhabitats: sunny dead trees, the inner branches of trees, and tree trunks. We suggest that this variation in display rate is driven by shifts in microhabitat temperature over the course of the day and spatial and temporal adjustments being made depending on the probabilities of being detected by both conspecifics and predators.","PeriodicalId":8822,"journal":{"name":"Behaviour","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.3,"publicationDate":"2022-12-07","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"46916535","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
Pub Date : 2022-10-24DOI: 10.1163/1568539x-bja10191
Sarah S. Garris, Karl N. Rohrer, M. Ferkin
The risk of predation and food deprivation may alter the degree to which animals associate with conspecifics. We examined if food deprivation, the risk of predation, or simultaneous exposure to both altered meadow voles’ preference for odour cues in a way that adheres to the terminal investment, safety in numbers, or avoidance hypotheses. Satiated or food-deprived meadow voles were given the choice to investigate either opposite-sex conspecific bedding, same-sex conspecific bedding, clean bedding, or self-bedding when exposed to mink urine or olive oil. Mink urine and food deprivation did not impact the amount of time meadow voles spent with each type of bedding, but meadow voles did begin investigating more quickly when experiencing either or both stressors. However, food deprivation and mink urine did not have an additive impact on any measured variable. Further research is needed to determine if the terminal investment hypothesis is the hypothesis that best describes the mating behaviour of meadow voles facing one or multiple stressors.
{"title":"Impact of food availability and predator cues on meadow vole response to social vs. non-social odorants","authors":"Sarah S. Garris, Karl N. Rohrer, M. Ferkin","doi":"10.1163/1568539x-bja10191","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1163/1568539x-bja10191","url":null,"abstract":"\u0000 The risk of predation and food deprivation may alter the degree to which animals associate with conspecifics. We examined if food deprivation, the risk of predation, or simultaneous exposure to both altered meadow voles’ preference for odour cues in a way that adheres to the terminal investment, safety in numbers, or avoidance hypotheses. Satiated or food-deprived meadow voles were given the choice to investigate either opposite-sex conspecific bedding, same-sex conspecific bedding, clean bedding, or self-bedding when exposed to mink urine or olive oil. Mink urine and food deprivation did not impact the amount of time meadow voles spent with each type of bedding, but meadow voles did begin investigating more quickly when experiencing either or both stressors. However, food deprivation and mink urine did not have an additive impact on any measured variable. Further research is needed to determine if the terminal investment hypothesis is the hypothesis that best describes the mating behaviour of meadow voles facing one or multiple stressors.","PeriodicalId":8822,"journal":{"name":"Behaviour","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.3,"publicationDate":"2022-10-24","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"46535999","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
Pub Date : 2022-10-21DOI: 10.1163/1568539x-bja10181
Ignacio Escalante, Jerald R. Kellner, R. L. Rodríguez, C. Desjonquères
Animals vary in the complexity and size of the signal repertoire used in communication. Often, these behavioural repertoires include multiple signal types for the same process, for instance, courtship. In Enchenopa treehoppers (Hemiptera: Membracidae) mate-searching males produce plant-borne vibrational advertisement signals. Receptive females then respond to males with their own signals. Here we describe an additional signal type in the repertoire of these males. We collected nymphs in Wisconsin, USA, and recorded the spontaneous signalling bouts of adult males and duetting signals of females using laser vibrometry. Two-thirds of males produced the additional signal type, which differed in temporal and spectral features from the main male advertisement signals, whilst resembling female duetting signals in placement and acoustic features. Our findings suggest that this might be a female mimic signal. Overall, our findings highlight the diversity in the behavioural repertoire that animals may use for reproduction.
{"title":"A female mimic signal type in the vibrational repertoire of male Enchenopa treehoppers","authors":"Ignacio Escalante, Jerald R. Kellner, R. L. Rodríguez, C. Desjonquères","doi":"10.1163/1568539x-bja10181","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1163/1568539x-bja10181","url":null,"abstract":"\u0000 Animals vary in the complexity and size of the signal repertoire used in communication. Often, these behavioural repertoires include multiple signal types for the same process, for instance, courtship. In Enchenopa treehoppers (Hemiptera: Membracidae) mate-searching males produce plant-borne vibrational advertisement signals. Receptive females then respond to males with their own signals. Here we describe an additional signal type in the repertoire of these males. We collected nymphs in Wisconsin, USA, and recorded the spontaneous signalling bouts of adult males and duetting signals of females using laser vibrometry. Two-thirds of males produced the additional signal type, which differed in temporal and spectral features from the main male advertisement signals, whilst resembling female duetting signals in placement and acoustic features. Our findings suggest that this might be a female mimic signal. Overall, our findings highlight the diversity in the behavioural repertoire that animals may use for reproduction.","PeriodicalId":8822,"journal":{"name":"Behaviour","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.3,"publicationDate":"2022-10-21","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"49161880","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
In some social groups, non-breeding subordinates regulate their growth, relative to the size of their immediate dominants in ways that reduce conflict over dominance rank. We predicted that such strategic growth adjustment should also occur in breeding pairs, if this is beneficial for the more subordinate individual within a pair. Using the cichlid fish, Julidochromis transcriptus, held in a laboratory, we examined whether strategic growth regulation occurs in monogamous pairs. In female-largest pairs, smaller males grew slower than their partner when the initial size ratio of pairs (large/small) was small, but faster when the ratio was large, and the number of pairs with an intermediate size ratio increased over time. However, in male-largest pairs, smaller females had a low growth rate and the size ratio of these pairs increased over time. The most important factors for predicting the growth rate of fish were the initial size ratio of pairs for smaller males in female-largest pairs and the initial body size for larger individuals in both pair types, but no such predictors were found for smaller females in male-largest pairs. Neither feeding rate nor attacking rate of the two individuals in a pair predicted the growth rate of smaller fish in a pair. These results suggest that smaller males strategically adjust their own growth, relative to the size of their partner in female-largest pairs, wherein the growth of larger females unrestrained by social relationship with their partner can increase female fecundity, being beneficial for both sexes. The adaptive significance of a low growth rate of smaller females in male-largest pairs is also discussed.
{"title":"Sex-dependent growth regulation in monogamous pairs of a cichlid fish","authors":"Kazunori Matsumoto, Kazuki Yoshihara, Chiyo Katsura, Tatsunori Ono, Masaki Habara, M. Kohda","doi":"10.1163/1568539x-bja10190","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1163/1568539x-bja10190","url":null,"abstract":"\u0000In some social groups, non-breeding subordinates regulate their growth, relative to the size of their immediate dominants in ways that reduce conflict over dominance rank. We predicted that such strategic growth adjustment should also occur in breeding pairs, if this is beneficial for the more subordinate individual within a pair. Using the cichlid fish, Julidochromis transcriptus, held in a laboratory, we examined whether strategic growth regulation occurs in monogamous pairs. In female-largest pairs, smaller males grew slower than their partner when the initial size ratio of pairs (large/small) was small, but faster when the ratio was large, and the number of pairs with an intermediate size ratio increased over time. However, in male-largest pairs, smaller females had a low growth rate and the size ratio of these pairs increased over time. The most important factors for predicting the growth rate of fish were the initial size ratio of pairs for smaller males in female-largest pairs and the initial body size for larger individuals in both pair types, but no such predictors were found for smaller females in male-largest pairs. Neither feeding rate nor attacking rate of the two individuals in a pair predicted the growth rate of smaller fish in a pair. These results suggest that smaller males strategically adjust their own growth, relative to the size of their partner in female-largest pairs, wherein the growth of larger females unrestrained by social relationship with their partner can increase female fecundity, being beneficial for both sexes. The adaptive significance of a low growth rate of smaller females in male-largest pairs is also discussed.","PeriodicalId":8822,"journal":{"name":"Behaviour","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.3,"publicationDate":"2022-10-18","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"46565701","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}