首页 > 最新文献

Journal of Experimental Psychology: General最新文献

英文 中文
Testing the flexibility of ensemble coding: Limitations in cross-modal ensemble perception. 测试集成编码的灵活性:跨模态集成感知的局限性。
IF 4.1 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-01-01 Epub Date: 2023-09-21 DOI: 10.1037/xge0001470
Greer Gillies, Keisuke Fukuda, Jonathan S Cant

Ensemble coding (the brain's ability to rapidly extract summary statistics from groups of items) has been demonstrated across a range of low-level (e.g., average color) to high-level (e.g., average facial expression) visual features, and even on information that cannot be gleaned solely from retinal input (e.g., object lifelikeness). There is also evidence that ensemble coding can interact with other cognitive systems such as long-term memory (LTM), as observers are able to derive the average cost of items. We extended this line of research to examine if different sensory modalities can interact during ensemble coding. Participants made judgments about the average sweetness of groups of different visually presented foods. We found that, when viewed simultaneously, observers were limited in the number of items they could incorporate into their cross-modal ensemble percepts. We speculate that this capacity limit is caused by the cross-modal translation of visual percepts into taste representations stored in LTM. This was supported by findings that (a) participants could use similar stimuli to form capacity-unlimited ensemble representations of average screen size and (b) participants could extract the average sweetness of displays when items were viewed in sequence, with no capacity limitation (suggesting that spatial attention constrains the number of necessary visual cues an observer can integrate in a given moment to trigger cross-modal retrieval of taste). Together, the results of our study demonstrate that there are limits to the flexibility of ensemble coding, especially when multiple cognitive systems need to interact to compress sensory information into an ensemble representation. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

集合编码(大脑从一组项目中快速提取汇总统计数据的能力)已经在一系列低水平(例如,平均颜色)到高水平(例如平均面部表情)的视觉特征中得到了证明,甚至在不能仅从视网膜输入中收集的信息(例如,物体的生命相似性)上也得到了证明。还有证据表明,集成编码可以与其他认知系统相互作用,如长期记忆(LTM),因为观察者能够得出项目的平均成本。我们扩展了这一研究范围,以检查不同的感觉模式是否可以在集成编码过程中相互作用。参与者对不同视觉呈现的食物组的平均甜味做出判断。我们发现,当同时观察时,观察者可以融入跨模态集合感知的项目数量有限。我们推测,这种容量限制是由视觉感知到LTM中存储的味觉表示的跨模态翻译引起的。这得到了以下发现的支持:(a)参与者可以使用类似的刺激来形成平均屏幕大小的容量无限的集合表示,以及(b)当按顺序观看项目时,参与者可以提取显示的平均甜蜜度,没有容量限制(这表明空间注意力限制了观察者在给定时刻可以整合的必要视觉线索的数量,以触发味觉的跨模态检索)。总之,我们的研究结果表明,集成编码的灵活性是有限的,尤其是当多个认知系统需要相互作用以将感官信息压缩成集成表示时。(PsycInfo数据库记录(c)2023 APA,保留所有权利)。
{"title":"Testing the flexibility of ensemble coding: Limitations in cross-modal ensemble perception.","authors":"Greer Gillies, Keisuke Fukuda, Jonathan S Cant","doi":"10.1037/xge0001470","DOIUrl":"10.1037/xge0001470","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Ensemble coding (the brain's ability to rapidly extract summary statistics from groups of items) has been demonstrated across a range of low-level (e.g., average color) to high-level (e.g., average facial expression) visual features, and even on information that cannot be gleaned solely from retinal input (e.g., object lifelikeness). There is also evidence that ensemble coding can interact with other cognitive systems such as long-term memory (LTM), as observers are able to derive the average cost of items. We extended this line of research to examine if different sensory modalities can interact during ensemble coding. Participants made judgments about the average sweetness of groups of different visually presented foods. We found that, when viewed simultaneously, observers were limited in the number of items they could incorporate into their cross-modal ensemble percepts. We speculate that this capacity limit is caused by the cross-modal translation of visual percepts into taste representations stored in LTM. This was supported by findings that (a) participants could use similar stimuli to form capacity-unlimited ensemble representations of average screen size and (b) participants could extract the average sweetness of displays when items were viewed in sequence, with no capacity limitation (suggesting that spatial attention constrains the number of necessary visual cues an observer can integrate in a given moment to trigger cross-modal retrieval of taste). Together, the results of our study demonstrate that there are limits to the flexibility of ensemble coding, especially when multiple cognitive systems need to interact to compress sensory information into an ensemble representation. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":15698,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology: General","volume":" ","pages":"56-69"},"PeriodicalIF":4.1,"publicationDate":"2024-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"41130926","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Sticky tradition impedes selection of creative ideas. 顽固的传统阻碍了创造性思想的选择。
IF 4.1 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-01-01 Epub Date: 2023-10-05 DOI: 10.1037/xge0001490
Moritz Reis, Anna Foerster, Ingo Zettler, Wilfried Kunde, Roland Pfister

Creativity is a driving force for human development and has fascinated scholars for centuries. Surprisingly little is known about the cognitive underpinnings of putting creative ideas into action, however. To shed light on this part of the creative process, we tracked how hand movements unfolded when choosing between either a traditional or a creative use of a given object. Participants could freely decide between both options (Experiment 1, N = 51 adults) or were prompted to select a specific use (Experiment 2, N = 51 adults). Temporal as well as spatial measures of action unfolding revealed behavior to be strongly biased toward traditional options when choosing an available, more creative option eventually. Creative behavior thus comprises two obstacles: not only coming up with new ideas, but also overcoming a lasting bias toward using old ones. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

创造力是人类发展的驱动力,几个世纪以来一直吸引着学者。然而,令人惊讶的是,人们对将创造性想法付诸行动的认知基础知之甚少。为了阐明创作过程的这一部分,我们跟踪了在传统或创造性使用给定对象之间进行选择时,手部动作是如何展开的。参与者可以在两种选择之间自由选择(实验1,N=51名成年人),也可以被提示选择特定用途(实验2,N=51位成年人)。行动展开的时间和空间测量显示,在最终选择一个可用的、更具创造性的选项时,行为强烈偏向传统选项。因此,创造性行为包括两个障碍:不仅要想出新的想法,还要克服使用旧想法的持久偏见。(PsycInfo数据库记录(c)2023 APA,保留所有权利)。
{"title":"Sticky tradition impedes selection of creative ideas.","authors":"Moritz Reis, Anna Foerster, Ingo Zettler, Wilfried Kunde, Roland Pfister","doi":"10.1037/xge0001490","DOIUrl":"10.1037/xge0001490","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Creativity is a driving force for human development and has fascinated scholars for centuries. Surprisingly little is known about the cognitive underpinnings of putting creative ideas into action, however. To shed light on this part of the creative process, we tracked how hand movements unfolded when choosing between either a traditional or a creative use of a given object. Participants could freely decide between both options (Experiment 1, <i>N</i> = 51 adults) or were prompted to select a specific use (Experiment 2, <i>N</i> = 51 adults). Temporal as well as spatial measures of action unfolding revealed behavior to be strongly biased toward traditional options when choosing an available, more creative option eventually. Creative behavior thus comprises two obstacles: not only coming up with new ideas, but also overcoming a lasting bias toward using old ones. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":15698,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology: General","volume":" ","pages":"268-273"},"PeriodicalIF":4.1,"publicationDate":"2024-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"41154716","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Interaction between top-down decision-driven congruency effect and bottom-up input-driven congruency effect is correlated with conscious awareness. 自上而下的决策驱动一致性效应和自下而上的投入驱动一致性效果之间的相互作用与意识意识相关。
IF 4.1 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-01-01 Epub Date: 2023-10-05 DOI: 10.1037/xge0001483
Ze-Fan Zheng, Shu-Yue Huang, Shena Lu, Yong-Chun Cai

In a conventional (Stroop) priming paradigm, it was well documented that objective prime-target incongruency delays response time (RT) to target compared to prime-target congruent condition. Recent evidence suggests that incongruency between the target and subjectively reported prime identity also delays RT over and above the classic congruency effect. When the prime is rendered invisible, the former effect is fundamentally a bottom-up (BU) stimulus-driven congruency effect and the latter a top-down (TD) guess-driven congruency effect. An influential theory of consciousness, global neuronal workspace theory, postulates that the long-lasting simultaneous and reciprocal interaction between TD decision network and BU input network is preserved during conscious processing and disabled during unconscious processing. Current study is focused on testing this theoretical postulation using two behavioral experiments. Our results showed that indeed TD-congruency and BU-congruency produced additive RT effects on prime-invisible trials, which implies that TD and BU prime representations are activated in independent neuronal populations. Meanwhile, an underadditive interaction effect was observed as prime visibility rose, which is a signature that TD and BU prime representations recruited overlapping neuronal populations during conscious perception. In addition, we suggest that current behavioral paradigm might be a financially friendly alternative to detect the presence of representational overlap in the brain between a wide range of mental representations, such as expectation, prediction, conscious/unconscious perception, and conscious/unconscious working memory. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

在传统的(Stroop)启动范式中,有充分的证据表明,与主要目标一致条件相比,目标主要目标不一致延迟了对目标的响应时间(RT)。最近的证据表明,目标和主观报告的主同一性之间的不一致也会延迟RT,超过经典的一致性效应。当素数变得不可见时,前者从根本上是自下而上(BU)刺激驱动的一致性效应,而后者是自上而下(TD)猜测驱动的一致效应。一个有影响力的意识理论,全局神经元工作空间理论,假设TD决策网络和BU输入网络之间的长期同时和相互作用在有意识的处理过程中被保留,在无意识的处理中被禁用。目前的研究主要是通过两个行为实验来检验这一理论假设。我们的结果表明,事实上,TD一致性和BU一致性在初级不可见试验中产生了加性RT效应,这意味着TD和BU初级表征在独立的神经元群体中被激活。同时,随着主可见性的增加,观察到了加性不足的相互作用效应,这是TD和BU主表征在意识感知过程中招募重叠神经元群体的标志。此外,我们认为,当前的行为范式可能是一种经济上友好的替代方案,可以检测大脑中广泛的心理表征之间的表征重叠,如期望、预测、有意识/无意识感知和有意识/潜意识工作记忆。(PsycInfo数据库记录(c)2023 APA,保留所有权利)。
{"title":"Interaction between top-down decision-driven congruency effect and bottom-up input-driven congruency effect is correlated with conscious awareness.","authors":"Ze-Fan Zheng, Shu-Yue Huang, Shena Lu, Yong-Chun Cai","doi":"10.1037/xge0001483","DOIUrl":"10.1037/xge0001483","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>In a conventional (Stroop) priming paradigm, it was well documented that objective prime-target incongruency delays response time (RT) to target compared to prime-target congruent condition. Recent evidence suggests that incongruency between the target and subjectively reported prime identity also delays RT over and above the classic congruency effect. When the prime is rendered invisible, the former effect is fundamentally a bottom-up (BU) stimulus-driven congruency effect and the latter a top-down (TD) guess-driven congruency effect. An influential theory of consciousness, global neuronal workspace theory, postulates that the long-lasting simultaneous and reciprocal interaction between TD decision network and BU input network is preserved during conscious processing and disabled during unconscious processing. Current study is focused on testing this theoretical postulation using two behavioral experiments. Our results showed that indeed TD-congruency and BU-congruency produced additive RT effects on prime-invisible trials, which implies that TD and BU prime representations are activated in independent neuronal populations. Meanwhile, an underadditive interaction effect was observed as prime visibility rose, which is a signature that TD and BU prime representations recruited overlapping neuronal populations during conscious perception. In addition, we suggest that current behavioral paradigm might be a financially friendly alternative to detect the presence of representational overlap in the brain between a wide range of mental representations, such as expectation, prediction, conscious/unconscious perception, and conscious/unconscious working memory. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":15698,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology: General","volume":" ","pages":"102-121"},"PeriodicalIF":4.1,"publicationDate":"2024-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"41135047","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The format of the cognitive map depends on the structure of the environment. 认知地图的格式取决于环境的结构。
IF 4.1 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-01-01 Epub Date: 2023-10-16 DOI: 10.1037/xge0001498
Michael Peer, Catherine Nadar, Russell A Epstein

Humans and animals form cognitive maps that allow them to navigate through large-scale environments. Here we address a central unresolved question about these maps: whether they exhibit similar characteristics across all environments, or-alternatively-whether different environments yield different types of maps. To investigate this question, we examined spatial learning in three virtual environments: an open courtyard with patios connected by paths (open maze), a set of rooms connected by corridors (closed maze), and a set of isolated rooms connected only by teleporters (teleport maze). All three environments shared the same underlying topological graph structure. Postlearning tests showed that participants formed representations of the three environments that varied in accuracy, format, and individual variability. The open maze was most accurately remembered, followed by the closed maze, and then the teleport maze. In the open maze, most participants developed representations that reflected the Euclidean structure of the space, whereas in the teleport maze, most participants constructed representations that aligned more closely with a mental model of an interconnected graph. In the closed maze, substantial individual variability emerged, with some participants forming Euclidean representations and others forming graph-like representations. These results indicate that an environment's features shape the quality and nature of the spatial representations formed within it, determining whether spatial knowledge takes a Euclidean or graph-like format. Consequently, experimental findings obtained in any single environment may not generalize to others with different features. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

人类和动物形成认知地图,使它们能够在大规模环境中导航。在这里,我们解决了关于这些地图的一个尚未解决的核心问题:它们是否在所有环境中都表现出相似的特征,或者不同的环境是否产生不同类型的地图。为了研究这个问题,我们研究了三个虚拟环境中的空间学习:一个带有由路径连接的天井的开放式庭院(开放迷宫)、一组由走廊连接的房间(封闭迷宫)和一组仅由传送机连接的孤立房间(传送迷宫)。所有三个环境共享相同的底层拓扑图结构。学习后测试表明,参与者形成了三种环境的表征,这三种环境在准确性、形式和个体可变性方面各不相同。开放式迷宫记忆最准确,其次是封闭式迷宫,然后是传送迷宫。在开放迷宫中,大多数参与者构建的表示反映了空间的欧几里得结构,而在传送迷宫中,大部分参与者构建的表达与互连图的心理模型更紧密地一致。在封闭迷宫中,出现了大量的个体变异性,一些参与者形成欧几里得表示,另一些参与者形成图形表示。这些结果表明,环境的特征决定了环境中形成的空间表示的质量和性质,决定了空间知识是采用欧几里得格式还是类似图形的格式。因此,在任何单一环境中获得的实验结果都可能无法推广到其他具有不同特征的环境中。(PsycInfo数据库记录(c)2023 APA,保留所有权利)。
{"title":"The format of the cognitive map depends on the structure of the environment.","authors":"Michael Peer, Catherine Nadar, Russell A Epstein","doi":"10.1037/xge0001498","DOIUrl":"10.1037/xge0001498","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Humans and animals form cognitive maps that allow them to navigate through large-scale environments. Here we address a central unresolved question about these maps: whether they exhibit similar characteristics across all environments, or-alternatively-whether different environments yield different types of maps. To investigate this question, we examined spatial learning in three virtual environments: an open courtyard with patios connected by paths (open maze), a set of rooms connected by corridors (closed maze), and a set of isolated rooms connected only by teleporters (teleport maze). All three environments shared the same underlying topological graph structure. Postlearning tests showed that participants formed representations of the three environments that varied in accuracy, format, and individual variability. The open maze was most accurately remembered, followed by the closed maze, and then the teleport maze. In the open maze, most participants developed representations that reflected the Euclidean structure of the space, whereas in the teleport maze, most participants constructed representations that aligned more closely with a mental model of an interconnected graph. In the closed maze, substantial individual variability emerged, with some participants forming Euclidean representations and others forming graph-like representations. These results indicate that an environment's features shape the quality and nature of the spatial representations formed within it, determining whether spatial knowledge takes a Euclidean or graph-like format. Consequently, experimental findings obtained in any single environment may not generalize to others with different features. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":15698,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology: General","volume":" ","pages":"224-240"},"PeriodicalIF":4.1,"publicationDate":"2024-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC10872840/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"41235827","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Immediate cross-language transfer of novel articulatory plans in bilingual speech. 双语语音中新颖发音计划的即时跨语言转移。
IF 4.1 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-01-01 Epub Date: 2023-08-10 DOI: 10.1037/xge0001456
Douglas M Shiller, Sarah Bobbitt, Daniel R Lametti

Current models of second language (L2) acquisition focus on interactions with a first language (L1) at the level of speech sound targets. In multilinguals, the degree of interaction between the articulatory plans that guide speech in each language remains unclear. Here, we directly address this question in bilingual speakers. We use a sensorimotor adaptation paradigm to drive the acquisition of novel articulatory plans for speech in one language and then measure the extent to which these new motor plans influence articulatory plans in the speaker's other language. Twenty L1-French, L2-English bilinguals adapted their speech production to a real-time alteration of vowel sounds. In one session, the adaptation was acquired during French sentence production; in a second session, the adaptation was acquired during English sentence production. In each session, cross-language transfer of these novel articulatory plans for speech was assessed using a transfer task that involved the production of French and English words with heavily noise-masked auditory feedback. Sensorimotor adaptation that countered the vowel sound alteration was observed in both French and English. Regardless of the linguistic context in which the adaptation was acquired, the adaptation transferred to the production of words in both languages. The amount of transfer did not depend on whether the adaptation was acquired in the participant's L1 or L2. In a second experiment, the result was replicated with 20 L1-English, L2-French speakers. The experiments support the idea that, in bilinguals, the interaction between L1 and L2 articulatory motor plans is rapid and bidirectional. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

目前的第二语言(L2)习得模型侧重于与第一语言(L1)在语音目标层面上的互动。在多语言者中,指导每种语言语音的发音计划之间的互动程度仍不清楚。在这里,我们直接在双语者身上解决这个问题。我们使用传感器运动适应范式来驱动一种语言的新发音计划的习得,然后测量这些新运动计划对说话者另一种语言发音计划的影响程度。20 名第一语言为法语、第二语言为英语的双语者根据元音的实时变化来调整他们的语音。在一次训练中,适应是在法语造句过程中获得的;在第二次训练中,适应是在英语造句过程中获得的。在每次训练中,这些新的语音发音计划的跨语言转换都要通过一项转换任务来评估,该任务包括在重度噪声掩蔽的听觉反馈下制作法语和英语单词。在法语和英语中都观察到了对抗元音改变的感觉运动适应。无论适应是在何种语言环境中获得的,适应都会转移到两种语言的单词发音中。转移的程度并不取决于适应是在被试的第一语言还是第二语言中获得的。在第二项实验中,20 名母语为英语、母语为法语的受试者重复了这一结果。这些实验支持了这样一种观点,即在双语者中,第一语言和第二语言发音运动计划之间的相互作用是快速和双向的。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Immediate cross-language transfer of novel articulatory plans in bilingual speech.","authors":"Douglas M Shiller, Sarah Bobbitt, Daniel R Lametti","doi":"10.1037/xge0001456","DOIUrl":"10.1037/xge0001456","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Current models of second language (L2) acquisition focus on interactions with a first language (L1) at the level of speech sound targets. In multilinguals, the degree of interaction between the articulatory plans that guide speech in each language remains unclear. Here, we directly address this question in bilingual speakers. We use a sensorimotor adaptation paradigm to drive the acquisition of novel articulatory plans for speech in one language and then measure the extent to which these new motor plans influence articulatory plans in the speaker's other language. Twenty L1-French, L2-English bilinguals adapted their speech production to a real-time alteration of vowel sounds. In one session, the adaptation was acquired during French sentence production; in a second session, the adaptation was acquired during English sentence production. In each session, cross-language transfer of these novel articulatory plans for speech was assessed using a transfer task that involved the production of French and English words with heavily noise-masked auditory feedback. Sensorimotor adaptation that countered the vowel sound alteration was observed in both French and English. Regardless of the linguistic context in which the adaptation was acquired, the adaptation transferred to the production of words in both languages. The amount of transfer did not depend on whether the adaptation was acquired in the participant's L1 or L2. In a second experiment, the result was replicated with 20 L1-English, L2-French speakers. The experiments support the idea that, in bilinguals, the interaction between L1 and L2 articulatory motor plans is rapid and bidirectional. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":15698,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology: General","volume":" ","pages":"15-25"},"PeriodicalIF":4.1,"publicationDate":"2024-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10321000","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
A novel electroencephalography-based paradigm to measure intergroup prosociality: An intergenerational study in the aftermath of the genocide against Tutsis in Rwanda. 一种新的基于脑电图的衡量群体间亲社会性的范式:卢旺达针对图西人的种族灭绝后的代际研究。
IF 4.1 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-01-01 Epub Date: 2023-10-23 DOI: 10.1037/xge0001480
Guillaume P Pech, Darius Gishoma, Emilie A Caspar

Studying how intergroup prosociality evolves in war-torn societies is critical for gaining a better understanding of conflict perpetuation. Rwanda provides a unique example of how two groups must reconcile and manage their intergroup biases following a genocidal process. In this study, we employed a novel intended behavior task to measure intergroup prosociality among former genocide perpetrators, genocide survivors, and their children in Rwanda. Participants were required to choose between various individuals representing their own in-group or their out-group as recipients of their prosocial intentions. We measured how frequently they selected in-group or out-group individuals and to what extent choosing each individual induced cognitive conflict, as measured by reaction times (RTs) and midfrontal theta (FMθ) activity. The results indicated that survivors and their children selected former perpetrators and their offspring less frequently. Furthermore, selecting them involved a higher cognitive conflict, as evidenced by longer RT and a higher FMθ, compared to choosing their own in-group. For the group composed of former perpetrators and their children, we observed a dissociation. They selected out-group individuals more frequently, perhaps as a compensatory behavior for their past wrongdoings. Nonetheless, selecting the out-group individuals involved a higher cognitive conflict than selecting their own in-group. Importantly, we observed a similar intergroup prosociality bias in the children of both survivors and former perpetrators, mirroring that of their parents. These results are important for understanding how past conflicts influence intergroup prosociality bias and the extent to which this bias is transmitted to the next generation. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

研究群体间亲社会性在饱受战争蹂躏的社会中是如何演变的,对于更好地理解冲突的持久性至关重要。卢旺达提供了一个独特的例子,说明在种族灭绝过程之后,两个群体必须如何调和和管理他们的群体间偏见。在这项研究中,我们采用了一项新颖的意向行为任务来衡量卢旺达前种族灭绝罪犯、种族灭绝幸存者及其子女的群体间亲社会性。参与者被要求在代表他们自己的组内或组外的不同个人之间进行选择,作为他们亲社会意图的接受者。我们测量了他们选择组内或组外个体的频率,以及选择每个个体在多大程度上引发认知冲突,通过反应时间(RT)和额中部θ(FMθ)活动来测量。结果表明,幸存者及其子女选择前肇事者及其后代的频率较低。此外,与在组中选择自己的人相比,选择他们涉及更高的认知冲突,如更长的RT和更高的FMθ所证明的。对于由前犯罪者及其子女组成的群体,我们观察到一种分离。他们更频繁地挑选群体个体,也许是为了补偿他们过去的错误行为。尽管如此,选择组外个体比选择自己的组内个体涉及更高的认知冲突。重要的是,我们在幸存者和前施暴者的孩子身上观察到了类似的群体间亲社会偏见,反映了他们父母的偏见。这些结果对于理解过去的冲突如何影响群体间的亲社会偏见以及这种偏见在多大程度上传递给下一代很重要。(PsycInfo数据库记录(c)2023 APA,保留所有权利)。
{"title":"A novel electroencephalography-based paradigm to measure intergroup prosociality: An intergenerational study in the aftermath of the genocide against Tutsis in Rwanda.","authors":"Guillaume P Pech, Darius Gishoma, Emilie A Caspar","doi":"10.1037/xge0001480","DOIUrl":"10.1037/xge0001480","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Studying how intergroup prosociality evolves in war-torn societies is critical for gaining a better understanding of conflict perpetuation. Rwanda provides a unique example of how two groups must reconcile and manage their intergroup biases following a genocidal process. In this study, we employed a novel intended behavior task to measure intergroup prosociality among former genocide perpetrators, genocide survivors, and their children in Rwanda. Participants were required to choose between various individuals representing their own in-group or their out-group as recipients of their prosocial intentions. We measured how frequently they selected in-group or out-group individuals and to what extent choosing each individual induced cognitive conflict, as measured by reaction times (RTs) and midfrontal theta (FMθ) activity. The results indicated that survivors and their children selected former perpetrators and their offspring less frequently. Furthermore, selecting them involved a higher cognitive conflict, as evidenced by longer RT and a higher FMθ, compared to choosing their own in-group. For the group composed of former perpetrators and their children, we observed a dissociation. They selected out-group individuals more frequently, perhaps as a compensatory behavior for their past wrongdoings. Nonetheless, selecting the out-group individuals involved a higher cognitive conflict than selecting their own in-group. Importantly, we observed a similar intergroup prosociality bias in the children of both survivors and former perpetrators, mirroring that of their parents. These results are important for understanding how past conflicts influence intergroup prosociality bias and the extent to which this bias is transmitted to the next generation. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":15698,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology: General","volume":" ","pages":"241-254"},"PeriodicalIF":4.1,"publicationDate":"2024-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"49690803","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Associations between executive functions assessed in different contexts in a genetically informative sample. 在基因信息样本中,在不同背景下评估的执行功能之间的相关性。
IF 4.1 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-01-01 Epub Date: 2023-09-04 DOI: 10.1037/xge0001471
Samantha M Freis, Jordan D Alexander, Jacob E Anderson, Robin P Corley, Alejandro I De La Vega, Daniel E Gustavson, Scott I Vrieze, Naomi P Friedman

Executive functions (EFs) are cognitive functions that help direct goal-related behavior. EFs are usually measured via behavioral tasks assessed in highly controlled laboratory settings under the supervision of a research assistant. Online versions of EF tasks are an increasingly popular alternative to in-lab testing. However, researchers do not have the same control over the testing environment during online EF assessments. To assess the extent to which EFs assessed in-lab and online are related, we used data from the Colorado Online Twin Study (CoTwins; 887 individual twins aged 13.98-19.05) and constructed an Lab Common EF factor and an Online Common EF factor from four EF tasks assessed in-lab and online. The Lab Common and Online Common EF factors were genetically identical (rA = 1.00) but phenotypically separable (r = .77, 95% confidence interval [0.59, 0.94]) indicating that these EF factors have the same genetic underpinnings but may be differentially influenced by environmental factors. We examined phenotypic, genetic, and environmental correlations between the EF factors and a general cognitive ability factor (g) assessed in the lab and found similar relationships between Lab Common EF and g and Online Common EF and g. Overall, these results suggest that Common EF factors assessed in different contexts are highly related to each other and similarly related to other cognitive outcomes. These findings indicate that online task-based EF assessments could be a viable strategy for increasing sample sizes in large-scale studies, particularly genetically informed studies. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

执行功能是帮助指导目标相关行为的认知功能。EF通常是在研究助理的监督下,通过在高度受控的实验室环境中评估的行为任务来测量的。EF任务的在线版本是实验室测试的一种越来越流行的替代方案。然而,在在线EF评估过程中,研究人员对测试环境没有同样的控制权。为了评估实验室和在线评估的EF之间的相关性,我们使用了科罗拉多在线双胞胎研究(CoTwins;887对年龄为13.98-19.05的双胞胎)的数据,并从实验室和在线评价的四项EF任务中构建了实验室共同EF因子和在线共同EF因子。实验室常见和在线常见EF因子在基因上相同(rA=1.00),但表型上可分离(r=.77,95%置信区间[0.59,0.94]),表明这些EF因子具有相同的遗传基础,但可能受到环境因素的不同影响。我们检测了EF因素与实验室评估的一般认知能力因素(g)之间的表型、遗传和环境相关性,发现实验室常见EF和g与在线常见EF和g,这些结果表明,在不同背景下评估的常见EF因素彼此高度相关,并且与其他认知结果类似。这些发现表明,在线基于任务的EF评估可能是在大规模研究中增加样本量的可行策略,特别是在基因知情研究中。(PsycInfo数据库记录(c)2023 APA,保留所有权利)。
{"title":"Associations between executive functions assessed in different contexts in a genetically informative sample.","authors":"Samantha M Freis, Jordan D Alexander, Jacob E Anderson, Robin P Corley, Alejandro I De La Vega, Daniel E Gustavson, Scott I Vrieze, Naomi P Friedman","doi":"10.1037/xge0001471","DOIUrl":"10.1037/xge0001471","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Executive functions (EFs) are cognitive functions that help direct goal-related behavior. EFs are usually measured via behavioral tasks assessed in highly controlled laboratory settings under the supervision of a research assistant. Online versions of EF tasks are an increasingly popular alternative to in-lab testing. However, researchers do not have the same control over the testing environment during online EF assessments. To assess the extent to which EFs assessed in-lab and online are related, we used data from the Colorado Online Twin Study (CoTwins; 887 individual twins aged 13.98-19.05) and constructed an Lab Common EF factor and an Online Common EF factor from four EF tasks assessed in-lab and online. The Lab Common and Online Common EF factors were genetically identical (<i>r</i>A = 1.00) but phenotypically separable (<i>r</i> = .77, 95% confidence interval [0.59, 0.94]) indicating that these EF factors have the same genetic underpinnings but may be differentially influenced by environmental factors. We examined phenotypic, genetic, and environmental correlations between the EF factors and a general cognitive ability factor (g) assessed in the lab and found similar relationships between Lab Common EF and g and Online Common EF and g. Overall, these results suggest that Common EF factors assessed in different contexts are highly related to each other and similarly related to other cognitive outcomes. These findings indicate that online task-based EF assessments could be a viable strategy for increasing sample sizes in large-scale studies, particularly genetically informed studies. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":15698,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology: General","volume":" ","pages":"70-85"},"PeriodicalIF":4.1,"publicationDate":"2024-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC10843656/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10157279","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Colorfulness influences perceptions of valence and arousal. 色彩会影响对效价和唤醒的感知。
IF 4.1 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-01-01 Epub Date: 2023-10-23 DOI: 10.1037/xge0001484
Adam D Pazda, Christopher A Thorstenson, Adam K Fetterman

Research on color-emotion associations provides evidence that hue, chroma, and lightness relate to various emotional experiences. Most of this research has assessed these relationships via isolated color swatches while confounding color dimensions. We broadened the medium in which color-emotion associations were made by manipulating color in photographs varying in valence and/or arousal, and we solely focused on the chroma dimension. In Experiment 1, participants perceived neutral and positive-valence photographs to be happier and more arousing when displayed chromatically, relative to achromatically. In Experiment 2, participants increased the chroma content of photographs to make them appear maximally happy, and they decreased the chroma content of photographs to make them appear maximally sad. In Experiment 3, participants altered the chroma content of photographs to their preferred levels, with positive-valence photographs containing the most chroma, followed by neutral, then negative-valence photographs. In Experiment 4, participants increased the chroma content of photographs to make them appear maximally positive or arousing, and they decreased chroma to make photographs appear maximally negative or calming. This pattern was similar regardless of the initial valence/arousal content of the images. These results indicate that chroma may convey emotion-relevant information independent of hue or lightness. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

对色彩情感关联的研究提供了证据,证明色调、色度和亮度与各种情感体验有关。这项研究中的大多数都是通过孤立的颜色样本来评估这些关系,同时混淆了颜色维度。我们通过操纵照片中不同价态和/或唤醒的颜色,拓宽了产生颜色-情感关联的媒介,我们只关注色度维度。在实验1中,与无色照片相比,参与者认为中性和正价照片在彩色显示时更快乐、更令人兴奋。在实验2中,参与者增加照片的色度含量以使其看起来最大程度地快乐,而他们降低照片的色度内容以使其显得最大程度地悲伤。在实验3中,参与者将照片的色度含量改变到他们喜欢的水平,正价照片的色度最高,其次是中性照片,然后是负价照片。在实验4中,参与者增加照片的色度含量,使其看起来最大程度地积极或令人兴奋,而他们降低色度,使照片看起来最大限度地消极或平静。无论图像的初始价态/唤醒内容如何,这种模式都是相似的。这些结果表明,色度可以独立于色调或亮度来传达与情绪相关的信息。(PsycInfo数据库记录(c)2023 APA,保留所有权利)。
{"title":"Colorfulness influences perceptions of valence and arousal.","authors":"Adam D Pazda, Christopher A Thorstenson, Adam K Fetterman","doi":"10.1037/xge0001484","DOIUrl":"10.1037/xge0001484","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Research on color-emotion associations provides evidence that hue, chroma, and lightness relate to various emotional experiences. Most of this research has assessed these relationships via isolated color swatches while confounding color dimensions. We broadened the medium in which color-emotion associations were made by manipulating color in photographs varying in valence and/or arousal, and we solely focused on the chroma dimension. In Experiment 1, participants perceived neutral and positive-valence photographs to be happier and more arousing when displayed chromatically, relative to achromatically. In Experiment 2, participants increased the chroma content of photographs to make them appear maximally happy, and they decreased the chroma content of photographs to make them appear maximally sad. In Experiment 3, participants altered the chroma content of photographs to their preferred levels, with positive-valence photographs containing the most chroma, followed by neutral, then negative-valence photographs. In Experiment 4, participants increased the chroma content of photographs to make them appear maximally positive or arousing, and they decreased chroma to make photographs appear maximally negative or calming. This pattern was similar regardless of the initial valence/arousal content of the images. These results indicate that chroma may convey emotion-relevant information independent of hue or lightness. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":15698,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology: General","volume":" ","pages":"145-158"},"PeriodicalIF":4.1,"publicationDate":"2024-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"49690804","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Changing the culture of peer review for a more inclusive and equitable psychological science. 改变同行评议的文化,使心理科学更加包容和公平。
IF 4.1 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2023-12-01 Epub Date: 2023-09-07 DOI: 10.1037/xge0001461
Mariam Aly, Eliana Colunga, M J Crockett, Matthew Goldrick, Pablo Gomez, Franki Y H Kung, Paul C McKee, Miriam Pérez, Sarah M Stilwell, Amanda B Diekman

Peer review is a core component of scientific practice. Although peer review ideally improves research and promotes rigor, it also has consequences for what types of research are published and cited and who wants to (and is able to) advance in research-focused careers. Despite these consequences, few reviewers or editors receive training or oversight to ensure their feedback is helpful, professional, and culturally sensitive. Here, we critically examine the peer-review system in psychology and neuroscience at multiple levels, from ideas to institutions, interactions, and individuals. We highlight initiatives that aim to change the normative negativity of peer review and provide authors with constructive, actionable feedback that is sensitive to diverse identities, methods, topics, and environments. We conclude with a call to action for how individuals, groups, and organizations can improve the culture of peer review. We provide examples of how changes in the peer-review system can be made with an eye to diversity (increasing the range of identities and experiences constituting the field), equity (fair processes and outcomes across groups), and inclusion (experiences that promote belonging across groups). These changes can improve scientists' experience of peer review, promote diverse perspectives and identities, and enhance the quality and impact of science. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2023 APA, all rights reserved).

同行评议是科学实践的核心组成部分。虽然同行评议在理想上提高了研究水平,提高了研究的严谨性,但它也会影响到什么类型的研究被发表和引用,以及谁想要(并且能够)在以研究为重点的职业生涯中取得进步。尽管有这些后果,很少有审稿人或编辑接受培训或监督,以确保他们的反馈是有用的、专业的和文化敏感的。在这里,我们从多个层面对心理学和神经科学的同行评议系统进行了批判性的研究,从思想到机构、互动和个人。我们强调旨在改变同行评议的规范性消极性的倡议,并为作者提供建设性的、可操作的反馈,这些反馈对不同的身份、方法、主题和环境都很敏感。最后,我们呼吁个人、团体和组织如何改善同行评审的文化。我们提供了一些例子,说明如何在关注多样性(增加构成该领域的身份和经验的范围)、公平(跨群体的公平过程和结果)和包容(促进跨群体归属感的经历)的情况下,对同行评审系统进行改变。这些变化可以改善科学家对同行评议的体验,促进不同的观点和身份,提高科学的质量和影响。(PsycInfo数据库记录(c) 2023 APA,版权所有)。
{"title":"Changing the culture of peer review for a more inclusive and equitable psychological science.","authors":"Mariam Aly, Eliana Colunga, M J Crockett, Matthew Goldrick, Pablo Gomez, Franki Y H Kung, Paul C McKee, Miriam Pérez, Sarah M Stilwell, Amanda B Diekman","doi":"10.1037/xge0001461","DOIUrl":"10.1037/xge0001461","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Peer review is a core component of scientific practice. Although peer review ideally improves research and promotes rigor, it also has consequences for what types of research are published and cited and who wants to (and is able to) advance in research-focused careers. Despite these consequences, few reviewers or editors receive training or oversight to ensure their feedback is helpful, professional, and culturally sensitive. Here, we critically examine the peer-review system in psychology and neuroscience at multiple levels, from ideas to institutions, interactions, and individuals. We highlight initiatives that aim to change the normative negativity of peer review and provide authors with constructive, actionable feedback that is sensitive to diverse identities, methods, topics, and environments. We conclude with a call to action for how individuals, groups, and organizations can improve the culture of peer review. We provide examples of how changes in the peer-review system can be made with an eye to diversity (increasing the range of identities and experiences constituting the field), equity (fair processes and outcomes across groups), and inclusion (experiences that promote belonging across groups). These changes can improve scientists' experience of peer review, promote diverse perspectives and identities, and enhance the quality and impact of science. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2023 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":15698,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology: General","volume":" ","pages":"3546-3565"},"PeriodicalIF":4.1,"publicationDate":"2023-12-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10524534","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
False memories for ending of events. 事件结束的虚假记忆。
IF 3.7 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2023-12-01 Epub Date: 2023-08-31 DOI: 10.1037/xge0001462
Petar P Raykov, Dominika Varga, Chris M Bird

Memories are not perfect recordings of the past and can be subject to systematic biases. Memory distortions are often caused by our experience of what typically happens in a given situation. However, it is unclear whether memory for events is biased by the knowledge that events usually have a predictable structure (a beginning, middle, and an end). Using video clips of everyday situations, we tested how interrupting events at unexpected time points affects memory of how those events ended. In four free recall experiments (1, 2, 4, and 5), we found that interrupting clips just before a salient piece of action was completed, resulted in the false recall of details about how the clip might have ended. We refer to this as "event extension." On the other hand, interrupting clips just after one scene had ended and a new scene started, resulted in omissions of details about the true ending of the clip (Experiments 4 and 5). We found that these effects were present, albeit attenuated, when testing memory shortly after watching the video clips compared to a week later (Experiments 5a and 5b). The event extension effect was not present when memory was tested with a recognition paradigm (Experiment 3). Overall, we conclude that when people watch videos that violate their expectations of typical event structure, they show a bias to later recall the videos as if they had ended at a predictable event boundary, exhibiting event extension or the omission of details depending on where the original video was interrupted. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2023 APA, all rights reserved).

记忆并不是对过去的完美记录,可能会受到系统性偏见的影响。记忆扭曲通常是由我们对特定情况下通常发生的事情的经验引起的。然而,对于事件的记忆是否受到事件通常具有可预测结构(开始、中间和结束)这一知识的影响,目前尚不清楚。使用日常场景的视频片段,我们测试了在意想不到的时间点中断事件会如何影响人们对这些事件如何结束的记忆。在四个自由回忆实验(1、2、4和5)中,我们发现,在一个重要动作完成之前打断片段,会导致对片段可能如何结束的细节的错误回忆。我们将此称为“事件扩展”。另一方面,在一个场景结束和一个新场景开始后打断剪辑,导致剪辑真实结束的细节被遗漏(实验4和5)。我们发现,在观看视频剪辑后不久测试记忆时,与一周后相比,这些影响存在,尽管减弱了(实验5a和5b)。当用识别范式测试记忆时,事件延伸效应不存在(实验3)。总的来说,我们得出结论,当人们观看违背他们对典型事件结构的期望的视频时,他们表现出一种稍后回忆视频的倾向,就好像他们在一个可预测的事件边界结束一样,表现出事件延伸或细节的遗漏,这取决于原始视频被中断的位置。(PsycInfo数据库记录(c) 2023 APA,版权所有)。
{"title":"False memories for ending of events.","authors":"Petar P Raykov, Dominika Varga, Chris M Bird","doi":"10.1037/xge0001462","DOIUrl":"10.1037/xge0001462","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Memories are not perfect recordings of the past and can be subject to systematic biases. Memory distortions are often caused by our experience of what typically happens in a given situation. However, it is unclear whether memory for events is biased by the knowledge that events usually have a predictable structure (a beginning, middle, and an end). Using video clips of everyday situations, we tested how interrupting events at unexpected time points affects memory of how those events ended. In four free recall experiments (1, 2, 4, and 5), we found that interrupting clips just before a salient piece of action was completed, resulted in the false recall of details about how the clip might have ended. We refer to this as \"event extension.\" On the other hand, interrupting clips just after one scene had ended and a new scene started, resulted in omissions of details about the true ending of the clip (Experiments 4 and 5). We found that these effects were present, albeit attenuated, when testing memory shortly after watching the video clips compared to a week later (Experiments 5a and 5b). The event extension effect was not present when memory was tested with a recognition paradigm (Experiment 3). Overall, we conclude that when people watch videos that violate their expectations of typical event structure, they show a bias to later recall the videos as if they had ended at a predictable event boundary, exhibiting event extension or the omission of details depending on where the original video was interrupted. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2023 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":15698,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology: General","volume":" ","pages":"3459-3475"},"PeriodicalIF":3.7,"publicationDate":"2023-12-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC10694998/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10122671","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Journal of Experimental Psychology: General
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1