首页 > 最新文献

Journal of Experimental Psychology: General最新文献

英文 中文
Does governmental corruption aid or hamper early moral development? Insights from the Dominican Republic and United States contexts. 政府腐败会帮助还是阻碍早期道德发展?来自多米尼加共和国和美国的启示。
IF 4.1 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-09-26 DOI: 10.1037/xge0001664
Bolivar Reyes-Jaquez,Melissa A Koenig
We tested whether children growing up in the Dominican Republic (D.R.), a context with relatively high governmental corruption levels, would support versus distance themselves from widespread unethical practices like bribery. In Experiment 1 (moral judgments; n = 106), D.R. elementary schoolers and adults evaluated judges who accepted gifts from contestants before or after selecting contest winners and predicted whether bribe-taking judges would be secretive. Like adults, older-but not younger-D.R. elementary schoolers differentially condemned judges who accepted gifts before versus after picking contest winners. Unlike adults, children often predicted that judges would disclose receiving gifts. In Experiment 2 (moral behaviors; n = 44), D.R. elementary schoolers could secretly accept or reject a bribe in exchange for 1st place while judging a drawing contest. All but two children rejected the bribe. Together, these findings stand in contrast with U.S. bribery-related developmental trends (Reyes-Jaquez & Koenig, 2021, 2022) and support this contention: When growing up in a more morally heterogeneous context like the D.R., children eventually assume a critical and differentiated stance toward-and will resist or subvert-some of their culture's unethical practices. Greater exposure to a wide range of unethical transactions might hinder aspects of bribery-related moral development early on, depending on how these aspects are measured (moral judgment vs. behavior). Nevertheless, over time, such exposure may strengthen children's capacity to resist unethical cultural practices, indicated by children's overwhelming rejection of bribes. We discuss the importance of including diverse response modalities (verbal, behavioral) when measuring psychological constructs in non-Western societies. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).
多米尼加共和国(D.R.)是一个政府腐败程度相对较高的国家,我们测试了在多米尼加共和国长大的儿童是否会支持或远离贿赂等普遍存在的不道德行为。在实验 1(道德判断;n = 106)中,多米尼加共和国的小学生和成年人对在评选优胜者之前或之后收受参赛者礼物的法官进行了评价,并预测受贿法官是否会保密。与成人一样,年龄较大但不太小的 D.R. 小学生对在挑选比赛获胜者之前和之后收受礼物的法官的谴责程度不同。与成人不同的是,儿童经常预测法官会透露收受礼物的情况。在实验 2(道德行为;n = 44)中,D.R. 小学生可以在评判绘画比赛时秘密接受或拒绝贿赂,以换取第一名。除两名儿童外,所有儿童都拒绝了贿赂。这些研究结果与美国与贿赂有关的发展趋向形成鲜明对比(Reyes-Jaquez & Koenig, 2021, 2022),并支持这一论点:当儿童在像印尼这样道德异质性较高的环境中成长时,他们最终会对其文化中的一些不道德做法采取批判和区别对待的立场,并会抵制或颠覆这些做法。更多地接触各种不道德的交易可能会阻碍早期与贿赂有关的道德发展,这取决于如何衡量这些方面(道德判断与行为)。然而,随着时间的推移,这种接触可能会增强儿童抵制不道德文化习俗的能力,儿童对贿赂的强烈排斥就说明了这一点。我们讨论了在测量非西方社会中的心理建构时包括不同反应模式(语言、行为)的重要性。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA,保留所有权利)。
{"title":"Does governmental corruption aid or hamper early moral development? Insights from the Dominican Republic and United States contexts.","authors":"Bolivar Reyes-Jaquez,Melissa A Koenig","doi":"10.1037/xge0001664","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/xge0001664","url":null,"abstract":"We tested whether children growing up in the Dominican Republic (D.R.), a context with relatively high governmental corruption levels, would support versus distance themselves from widespread unethical practices like bribery. In Experiment 1 (moral judgments; n = 106), D.R. elementary schoolers and adults evaluated judges who accepted gifts from contestants before or after selecting contest winners and predicted whether bribe-taking judges would be secretive. Like adults, older-but not younger-D.R. elementary schoolers differentially condemned judges who accepted gifts before versus after picking contest winners. Unlike adults, children often predicted that judges would disclose receiving gifts. In Experiment 2 (moral behaviors; n = 44), D.R. elementary schoolers could secretly accept or reject a bribe in exchange for 1st place while judging a drawing contest. All but two children rejected the bribe. Together, these findings stand in contrast with U.S. bribery-related developmental trends (Reyes-Jaquez & Koenig, 2021, 2022) and support this contention: When growing up in a more morally heterogeneous context like the D.R., children eventually assume a critical and differentiated stance toward-and will resist or subvert-some of their culture's unethical practices. Greater exposure to a wide range of unethical transactions might hinder aspects of bribery-related moral development early on, depending on how these aspects are measured (moral judgment vs. behavior). Nevertheless, over time, such exposure may strengthen children's capacity to resist unethical cultural practices, indicated by children's overwhelming rejection of bribes. We discuss the importance of including diverse response modalities (verbal, behavioral) when measuring psychological constructs in non-Western societies. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).","PeriodicalId":15698,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology: General","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":4.1,"publicationDate":"2024-09-26","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142325064","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Different methods elicit different belief distributions. 不同的方法会产生不同的信念分布。
IF 4.1 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-09-26 DOI: 10.1037/xge0001655
Beidi Hu,Joseph P Simmons
When eliciting people's forecasts or beliefs, you can ask for a point estimate-for example, what is the most likely state of the world?-or you can ask for an entire distribution of beliefs-for example, how likely is every possible state of the world? Eliciting belief distributions potentially yields more information, and researchers have increasingly tried to do so. In this article, we show that different elicitation methods elicit different belief distributions. We compare two popular methods used to elicit belief distributions: Distribution Builder and Sliders. In 10 preregistered studies (N = 14,553), we find that Distribution Builder elicits more accurate belief distributions than Sliders, except when true distributions are right-skewed, for which the results are mixed. This result holds when we assess accuracy (a) relative to a normative benchmark and (b) relative to participants' own beliefs. Our evidence suggests that participants approach these two methods differently: Sliders users are more likely to start with the lowest bins in the interface, which in turn leads them to put excessive mass in those bins. Our research sheds light on the process by which people construct belief distributions while offering a practical recommendation for future research: All else equal, Distribution Builder yields more accurate belief distributions. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).
在征询人们的预测或信念时,您可以询问一个估计点,例如,世界上最有可能出现的状况是什么;您也可以询问整个信念分布,例如,世界上每一种可能出现的状况的可能性有多大?激发信念分布可能会获得更多信息,研究人员也越来越多地尝试这样做。在本文中,我们将展示不同的诱导方法会诱发出不同的信念分布。我们比较了两种常用的信念分布诱导方法:分布生成器和滑块。在 10 项预先登记的研究(N = 14553)中,我们发现分布生成器比滑动器能引出更准确的信念分布,除非真实分布是右偏的,在这种情况下结果不一。当我们评估(a)相对于规范基准和(b)相对于参与者自身信念的准确性时,这一结果是成立的。我们的证据表明,参与者采用这两种方法的方式不同:滑块用户更有可能从界面中最低的箱开始,这反过来又会导致他们在这些箱中放入过多的质量。我们的研究揭示了人们构建信念分布的过程,同时也为未来的研究提供了切实可行的建议:在其他条件相同的情况下,"分布生成器 "能生成更准确的信念分布。(PsycInfo 数据库记录 (c) 2024 APA,保留所有权利)。
{"title":"Different methods elicit different belief distributions.","authors":"Beidi Hu,Joseph P Simmons","doi":"10.1037/xge0001655","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/xge0001655","url":null,"abstract":"When eliciting people's forecasts or beliefs, you can ask for a point estimate-for example, what is the most likely state of the world?-or you can ask for an entire distribution of beliefs-for example, how likely is every possible state of the world? Eliciting belief distributions potentially yields more information, and researchers have increasingly tried to do so. In this article, we show that different elicitation methods elicit different belief distributions. We compare two popular methods used to elicit belief distributions: Distribution Builder and Sliders. In 10 preregistered studies (N = 14,553), we find that Distribution Builder elicits more accurate belief distributions than Sliders, except when true distributions are right-skewed, for which the results are mixed. This result holds when we assess accuracy (a) relative to a normative benchmark and (b) relative to participants' own beliefs. Our evidence suggests that participants approach these two methods differently: Sliders users are more likely to start with the lowest bins in the interface, which in turn leads them to put excessive mass in those bins. Our research sheds light on the process by which people construct belief distributions while offering a practical recommendation for future research: All else equal, Distribution Builder yields more accurate belief distributions. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).","PeriodicalId":15698,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology: General","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":4.1,"publicationDate":"2024-09-26","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142325065","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
An associative-learning account of how infants learn about causal action in animates and inanimates: A critical reexamination of four classic studies. 关于婴儿如何学习动物和非动物因果行动的联想学习理论:对四项经典研究的重新审视。
IF 3.7 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-09-23 DOI: 10.1037/xge0001656
Deon T Benton

Considerable research shows that causal perception emerges between 6 and 10 months of age. Yet, because this research tends to use artificial stimuli, it is unanswered how or through what mechanisms of change human infants learn about the causal properties of real-world categories such as animate entities and inanimate objects. One answer to this question is that this knowledge is innate (i.e., unlearned, evolutionarily ancient, and possibly present at birth) and underpinned by core knowledge and core cognition. An alternative perspective that is tested here through computer simulations is that infants acquire this knowledge via domain-general associative learning. This article demonstrates that associative learning alone-as instantiated in an artificial neural network-is sufficient to explain the data presented in four classic infancy studies: Spelke et al. (1995), Saxe et al. (2005), Saxe et al. (2007), and Markson and Spelke (2006). This work not only advances theoretical perspectives within developmental psychology but also has implications for the design of artificial intelligence systems inspired by human cognitive development. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

大量研究表明,因果感知出现在婴儿 6 到 10 个月大的时候。然而,由于这些研究倾向于使用人工刺激,因此人类婴儿是如何或通过何种变化机制来学习现实世界类别(如有生命的实体和无生命的物体)的因果属性的,尚无定论。对这一问题的一种回答是,这种知识是与生俱来的(即未学过的、进化古老的、可能在出生时就存在的),并以核心知识和核心认知为基础。本文通过计算机模拟测试了另一种观点,即婴儿是通过领域性联想学习获得这些知识的。本文证明,联想学习本身--在人工神经网络中的实例--足以解释四项经典婴儿研究中的数据:Spelke 等人(1995 年)、Saxe 等人(2005 年)、Saxe 等人(2007 年)以及 Markson 和 Spelke(2006 年)。这项工作不仅推进了发展心理学的理论观点,而且对设计受人类认知发展启发的人工智能系统具有重要意义。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"An associative-learning account of how infants learn about causal action in animates and inanimates: A critical reexamination of four classic studies.","authors":"Deon T Benton","doi":"10.1037/xge0001656","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/xge0001656","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Considerable research shows that causal perception emerges between 6 and 10 months of age. Yet, because this research tends to use artificial stimuli, it is unanswered how or through what mechanisms of change human infants learn about the causal properties of real-world categories such as animate entities and inanimate objects. One answer to this question is that this knowledge is innate (i.e., unlearned, evolutionarily ancient, and possibly present at birth) and underpinned by core knowledge and core cognition. An alternative perspective that is tested here through computer simulations is that infants acquire this knowledge via domain-general associative learning. This article demonstrates that associative learning alone-as instantiated in an artificial neural network-is sufficient to explain the data presented in four classic infancy studies: Spelke et al. (1995), Saxe et al. (2005), Saxe et al. (2007), and Markson and Spelke (2006). This work not only advances theoretical perspectives within developmental psychology but also has implications for the design of artificial intelligence systems inspired by human cognitive development. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":15698,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology: General","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.7,"publicationDate":"2024-09-23","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142288950","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Variance (un)explained: Experimental conditions and temporal dependencies explain similarly small proportions of reaction time variability in linear models of perceptual and cognitive tasks. 方差(未)解释:在感知和认知任务的线性模型中,实验条件和时间依赖性对反应时间变异的解释比例同样很小。
IF 3.7 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-09-23 DOI: 10.1037/xge0001630
Marlou Nadine Perquin, Tobias Heed, Christoph Kayser

Any series of sensorimotor actions shows fluctuations in speed and accuracy from repetition to repetition, even when the sensory input and motor output requirements remain identical over time. Such fluctuations are particularly prominent in reaction time (RT) series from laboratory neurocognitive tasks. Despite their omnipresent nature, trial-to-trial fluctuations remain poorly understood. Here, we systematically analyzed RT series from various neurocognitive tasks, quantifying how much of the total trial-to-trial RT variance can be explained with general linear models (GLMs) by three sources of variability that are frequently investigated in behavioral and neuroscientific research: (1) experimental conditions, employed to induce systematic patterns in variability, (2) short-term temporal dependencies such as the autocorrelation between subsequent trials, and (3) long-term temporal trends over experimental blocks and sessions. Furthermore, we examined to what extent the explained variances by these sources are shared or unique. We analyzed 1913 unique RT series from 30 different cognitive control and perception-based tasks. On average, the three sources together explained ∼8%-17% of the total variance. The experimental conditions explained on average ∼2.5%-3.5% but did not share explained variance with temporal dependencies. Thus, the largest part of the trial-to-trial fluctuations in RT remained unexplained by these three sources. Unexplained fluctuations may take on nonlinear forms that are not picked up by GLMs. They may also be partially attributable to observable endogenous factors, such as fluctuations in brain activity and bodily states. Still, some extent of randomness may be a feature of the neurobiological system rather than just nuisance. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

任何一系列的感觉运动动作,即使在感觉输入和运动输出要求在一段时间内保持相同的情况下,其速度和准确性在重复与重复之间都会出现波动。这种波动在实验室神经认知任务的反应时间(RT)系列中尤为突出。尽管这种波动无处不在,但人们对每次试验之间的波动仍然知之甚少。在这里,我们系统地分析了各种神经认知任务的反应时间序列,量化了行为和神经科学研究中经常调查的三个变异性来源(1)实验条件,以诱导变异性的系统模式,(2)短期时间依赖性,如后续试验之间的自相关性,以及(3)实验块和实验阶段的长期时间趋势。此外,我们还研究了这些来源所解释的变异在多大程度上是共享的或独特的。我们分析了来自 30 个不同认知控制和感知任务的 1913 个独特的 RT 序列。平均而言,这三个来源共同解释了总方差的 8%-17%。实验条件平均解释了 2.5%-3.5%,但不与时间依赖性共同解释方差。因此,RT 试验间波动的最大部分仍然是这三个来源无法解释的。无法解释的波动可能以非线性形式出现,而 GLM 无法捕捉到这些波动。它们也可能部分归因于可观察到的内生因素,如大脑活动和身体状态的波动。不过,某种程度的随机性可能是神经生物学系统的一个特征,而不仅仅是一种干扰。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Variance (un)explained: Experimental conditions and temporal dependencies explain similarly small proportions of reaction time variability in linear models of perceptual and cognitive tasks.","authors":"Marlou Nadine Perquin, Tobias Heed, Christoph Kayser","doi":"10.1037/xge0001630","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/xge0001630","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Any series of sensorimotor actions shows fluctuations in speed and accuracy from repetition to repetition, even when the sensory input and motor output requirements remain identical over time. Such fluctuations are particularly prominent in reaction time (RT) series from laboratory neurocognitive tasks. Despite their omnipresent nature, trial-to-trial fluctuations remain poorly understood. Here, we systematically analyzed RT series from various neurocognitive tasks, quantifying how much of the total trial-to-trial RT variance can be explained with general linear models (GLMs) by three sources of variability that are frequently investigated in behavioral and neuroscientific research: (1) experimental conditions, employed to induce systematic patterns in variability, (2) short-term temporal dependencies such as the autocorrelation between subsequent trials, and (3) long-term temporal trends over experimental blocks and sessions. Furthermore, we examined to what extent the explained variances by these sources are shared or unique. We analyzed 1913 unique RT series from 30 different cognitive control and perception-based tasks. On average, the three sources together explained ∼8%-17% of the total variance. The experimental conditions explained on average ∼2.5%-3.5% but did not share explained variance with temporal dependencies. Thus, the largest part of the trial-to-trial fluctuations in RT remained unexplained by these three sources. Unexplained fluctuations may take on nonlinear forms that are not picked up by GLMs. They may also be partially attributable to observable endogenous factors, such as fluctuations in brain activity and bodily states. Still, some extent of randomness may be a feature of the neurobiological system rather than just nuisance. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":15698,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology: General","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.7,"publicationDate":"2024-09-23","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142288956","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The curve of control: Nonmonotonic effects of task difficulty on cognitive control. 控制曲线:任务难度对认知控制的非单调效应
IF 3.7 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-09-19 DOI: 10.1037/xge0001637
Miklos Bognar, Mate Gyurkovics, Balazs Aczel, Henk van Steenbergen

The U-shaped curve has long been recognized as a fundamental concept in psychological science, particularly in theories about motivational accounts and cognitive control. In this study (N = 330), we empirically tested the prediction of a nonmonotonic, curvilinear relationship between task difficulty and control adaptation. Drawing from motivational intensity theory and the expected value of control framework, we hypothesized that control intensity would increase with task difficulty until a maximum tolerable level, after which it would decrease. To examine this hypothesis, we conducted two experiments utilizing Stroop-like conflict tasks, systematically manipulating the number of distractors to vary task difficulty. We assessed control adaptation and measured subjective task difficulty. Our results revealed a curvilinear pattern between perceived task difficulty and adaptation of control. The findings provide empirical support for the theoretical accounts of motivational intensity theory and expected value of control, highlighting the nonlinear nature of the relationship between task difficulty and cognitive control. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

长期以来,U 型曲线一直被认为是心理科学中的一个基本概念,尤其是在动机账户和认知控制理论中。在本研究(N = 330)中,我们对任务难度与控制适应之间非单调曲线关系的预测进行了实证检验。根据动机强度理论和控制的预期价值框架,我们假设控制强度会随着任务难度的增加而增加,直到达到最大可承受水平,之后会降低。为了验证这一假设,我们利用类似于 Stroop 的冲突任务进行了两次实验,通过系统地操纵干扰物的数量来改变任务难度。我们评估了控制适应性并测量了主观任务难度。我们的结果表明,在感知任务难度和控制适应之间存在曲线模式。研究结果为动机强度理论和控制预期值理论提供了经验支持,突出了任务难度与认知控制之间的非线性关系。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"The curve of control: Nonmonotonic effects of task difficulty on cognitive control.","authors":"Miklos Bognar, Mate Gyurkovics, Balazs Aczel, Henk van Steenbergen","doi":"10.1037/xge0001637","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/xge0001637","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The U-shaped curve has long been recognized as a fundamental concept in psychological science, particularly in theories about motivational accounts and cognitive control. In this study (<i>N</i> = 330), we empirically tested the prediction of a nonmonotonic, curvilinear relationship between task difficulty and control adaptation. Drawing from motivational intensity theory and the expected value of control framework, we hypothesized that control intensity would increase with task difficulty until a maximum tolerable level, after which it would decrease. To examine this hypothesis, we conducted two experiments utilizing Stroop-like conflict tasks, systematically manipulating the number of distractors to vary task difficulty. We assessed control adaptation and measured subjective task difficulty. Our results revealed a curvilinear pattern between perceived task difficulty and adaptation of control. The findings provide empirical support for the theoretical accounts of motivational intensity theory and expected value of control, highlighting the nonlinear nature of the relationship between task difficulty and cognitive control. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":15698,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology: General","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.7,"publicationDate":"2024-09-19","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142288955","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Reconciling opposing effects of emotion on relational memory: Behavioral, eye-tracking, and brain imaging investigations. 调和情绪对关系记忆的对立影响:行为、眼动追踪和脑成像研究。
IF 3.7 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-09-19 DOI: 10.1037/xge0001625
Paul C Bogdan, Florin Dolcos, Yuta Katsumi, Margaret O'Brien, Alexandru D Iordan, Samantha Iwinski, Simona Buetti, Alejandro Lleras, Kelly Freeman Bost, Sanda Dolcos

The effects of emotion on memory are wide-ranging and powerful, but they are not uniform. Although there is agreement that emotion enhances memory for individual items, how it influences memory for the associated contextual details (relational memory, RM) remains debated. The prevalent view suggests that emotion impairs RM, but there is also evidence that emotion enhances RM. To reconcile these diverging results, we carried out three studies incorporating the following features: (1) testing RM with increased specificity, distinguishing between subjective (recollection based) and objective (item-context match) RM accuracy, (2) accounting for emotion-attention interactions via eye-tracking and task manipulation, and (3) using stimuli with integrated item-context content. Challenging the prevalent view, we identified both enhancing and impairing effects. First, emotion enhanced subjective RM, separately and when confirmed by accurate objective RM. Second, emotion impaired objective RM through attention capturing, but it enhanced RM accuracy when attentional effects were statistically accounted for using eye-tracking data. Third, emotion also enhanced RM when participants were cued to focus on contextual details during encoding, likely by increasing item-context binding. Finally, functional magnetic resonance imaging data recorded from a subset of participants showed that emotional enhancement of RM was associated with increased activity in the medial temporal lobe (MTL) and ventrolateral prefrontal cortex, along with increased intra-MTL and ventrolateral prefrontal cortex-MTL functional connectivity. Overall, these findings reconcile evidence regarding opposing effects of emotion on RM and point to possible training interventions to increase RM specificity in healthy functioning, posttraumatic stress disorder, and aging, by promoting item-context binding and diminishing memory decontextualization. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

情绪对记忆的影响是广泛而强大的,但并不一致。尽管人们一致认为情绪会增强对单个项目的记忆,但情绪如何影响对相关情境细节的记忆(关系记忆,RM)仍存在争议。流行的观点认为情绪会损害关系记忆,但也有证据表明情绪会增强关系记忆。为了调和这些不同的结果,我们进行了三项包含以下特征的研究:(1)以更高的特异性测试RM,区分主观(基于回忆)和客观(项目-上下文匹配)RM的准确性;(2)通过眼动跟踪和任务操作来考虑情绪-注意力的相互作用;(3)使用具有综合项目-上下文内容的刺激物。与普遍观点不同的是,我们同时发现了增强效应和损害效应。首先,情绪会单独增强主观RM,并通过准确的客观RM加以证实。其次,情绪会通过注意力捕捉损害客观RM,但当使用眼动追踪数据对注意力效应进行统计时,情绪会提高RM的准确性。第三,当被试在编码过程中被提示关注上下文细节时,情绪也会增强RM,这可能是通过增加项目-上下文的结合来实现的。最后,一部分参与者记录的功能磁共振成像数据显示,情绪对RM的增强与内侧颞叶(MTL)和腹外侧前额叶皮层活动的增加有关,同时内侧颞叶(MTL)和腹外侧前额叶皮层与内侧颞叶(MTL)的功能连接也增加了。总之,这些发现调和了情绪对RM的相反影响的证据,并指出了可能的训练干预措施,通过促进项目与上下文的结合和减少记忆的非上下文化,提高健康功能、创伤后应激障碍和衰老的RM特异性。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Reconciling opposing effects of emotion on relational memory: Behavioral, eye-tracking, and brain imaging investigations.","authors":"Paul C Bogdan, Florin Dolcos, Yuta Katsumi, Margaret O'Brien, Alexandru D Iordan, Samantha Iwinski, Simona Buetti, Alejandro Lleras, Kelly Freeman Bost, Sanda Dolcos","doi":"10.1037/xge0001625","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/xge0001625","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The effects of emotion on memory are wide-ranging and powerful, but they are not uniform. Although there is agreement that emotion enhances memory for individual items, how it influences memory for the associated contextual details (relational memory, RM) remains debated. The prevalent view suggests that emotion impairs RM, but there is also evidence that emotion enhances RM. To reconcile these diverging results, we carried out three studies incorporating the following features: (1) testing RM with increased specificity, distinguishing between <i>subjective</i> (recollection based) and <i>objective</i> (item-context match) RM accuracy, (2) accounting for emotion-attention interactions via eye-tracking and task manipulation, and (3) using stimuli with integrated item-context content. Challenging the prevalent view, we identified both enhancing and impairing effects. First, emotion enhanced subjective RM, separately and when confirmed by accurate objective RM. Second, emotion impaired objective RM through attention capturing, but it enhanced RM accuracy when attentional effects were statistically accounted for using eye-tracking data. Third, emotion also enhanced RM when participants were cued to focus on contextual details during encoding, likely by increasing item-context binding. Finally, functional magnetic resonance imaging data recorded from a subset of participants showed that emotional enhancement of RM was associated with increased activity in the medial temporal lobe (MTL) and ventrolateral prefrontal cortex, along with increased intra-MTL and ventrolateral prefrontal cortex-MTL functional connectivity. Overall, these findings reconcile evidence regarding opposing effects of emotion on RM and point to possible training interventions to increase RM specificity in healthy functioning, posttraumatic stress disorder, and aging, by promoting item-context binding and diminishing memory decontextualization. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":15698,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology: General","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.7,"publicationDate":"2024-09-19","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142288953","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Assessing the effects of "native speaker" status on classic findings in speech research. 评估 "母语使用者 "身份对语音研究经典结论的影响。
IF 3.7 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-09-19 DOI: 10.1037/xge0001640
Julia F Strand, Violet A Brown, Katrina Sewell, Yuxin Lin, Emmett Lefkowitz, Caroline G Saksena

It is common practice in speech research to only sample participants who self-report being "native English speakers." Although there is research on differences in language processing between native and nonnative listeners (see Lecumberri et al., 2010, for a review), the majority of speech research that aims to establish general findings (e.g., testing models of spoken word recognition) only includes native speakers in their sample. Not only is the "native English speaker" criterion poorly defined, but it also excludes historically underrepresented groups from speech perception research, often without attention to whether this exclusion is likely to affect study outcomes. The purpose of this study is to empirically test whether and how using different inclusion criteria ("native English speakers" vs. "nonnative English speakers") affects several well-known phenomena in speech perception research. Five hundred participants completed word (N = 200) and sentence (N = 300) identification tasks in quiet and in moderate levels of background noise. Results indicate that multiple classic findings in speech perception research-including the effects of noise level, lexical density, and semantic context on speech intelligibility-persist regardless of "native English" speaking status. However, the magnitude of some of these effects differed across participant groups. Taken together, these results suggest that researchers should carefully consider whether native speaker status is likely to affect outcomes and make decisions about inclusion criteria on a study-by-study basis. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

在语音研究中,通常只对自称 "英语为母语 "的参与者进行抽样调查。尽管对母语听者和非母语听者在语言处理过程中的差异有研究(见 Lecumberri 等人,2010 年,综述),但大多数旨在得出一般性结论的语音研究(如测试口语单词识别模型)都只将母语听者作为样本。英语为母语者 "的标准不仅定义不清,而且还将历史上代表性不足的群体排除在言语感知研究之外,而且往往不关注这种排除是否会影响研究结果。本研究的目的是通过实证检验使用不同的纳入标准("英语为母语者 "与 "非英语为母语者")是否以及如何影响语音感知研究中几个众所周知的现象。五百名参与者在安静和中等程度的背景噪声中完成了单词(200 人)和句子(300 人)识别任务。结果表明,语音感知研究中的多个经典发现--包括噪音水平、词汇密度和语义语境对语音可懂度的影响--与 "英语为母语 "者的身份无关。不过,其中一些影响的程度在不同的参与者群体中有所不同。综上所述,这些结果表明,研究人员应仔细考虑母语为英语的人的身份是否可能影响研究结果,并在逐项研究的基础上决定纳入标准。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Assessing the effects of \"native speaker\" status on classic findings in speech research.","authors":"Julia F Strand, Violet A Brown, Katrina Sewell, Yuxin Lin, Emmett Lefkowitz, Caroline G Saksena","doi":"10.1037/xge0001640","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/xge0001640","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>It is common practice in speech research to only sample participants who self-report being \"native English speakers.\" Although there is research on differences in language processing between native and nonnative listeners (see Lecumberri et al., 2010, for a review), the majority of speech research that aims to establish general findings (e.g., testing models of spoken word recognition) only includes native speakers in their sample. Not only is the \"native English speaker\" criterion poorly defined, but it also excludes historically underrepresented groups from speech perception research, often without attention to whether this exclusion is likely to affect study outcomes. The purpose of this study is to empirically test whether and how using different inclusion criteria (\"native English speakers\" vs. \"nonnative English speakers\") affects several well-known phenomena in speech perception research. Five hundred participants completed word (<i>N</i> = 200) and sentence (N = 300) identification tasks in quiet and in moderate levels of background noise. Results indicate that multiple classic findings in speech perception research-including the effects of noise level, lexical density, and semantic context on speech intelligibility-persist regardless of \"native English\" speaking status. However, the magnitude of some of these effects differed across participant groups. Taken together, these results suggest that researchers should carefully consider whether native speaker status is likely to affect outcomes and make decisions about inclusion criteria on a study-by-study basis. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":15698,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology: General","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.7,"publicationDate":"2024-09-19","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142288951","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
No evidence that sound-shape associations influence temporal resolution in humans: Five nonreplications of Parise and Spence (2009) and meta-analyses. 没有证据表明声形关联会影响人类的时间分辨率:Parise和Spence(2009年)的五项非重复性研究和荟萃分析。
IF 3.7 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-09-19 DOI: 10.1037/xge0001641
Suddha Sourav, Brigitte Röder, Franka Ambsdorf, Andromachi Melissari, Miketa Arvaniti, Argiro Vatakis

Sound-shape associations (e.g., preferentially matching angular shapes with high-pitched sounds and smooth shapes with low-pitched ones) have been almost universally observed in humans. If cross-modally congruent sounds and shapes are more robustly integrated in humans, distinguishing them in time might be hypothetically more challenging compared to incongruent sound-shape pairings. Supporting this premise, a highly cited work by Parise and Spence (2009; n = 12) reported worse temporal order judgement performance for audiovisual stimuli with congruent compared to incongruent sound-shape associations. Here, we report the results of five experiments across two laboratories, including a preregistered replication attempt, all (∑n = 102) failing to replicate the original results. Additionally, frequentist and Bayesian meta-analyses found no evidence against the null hypothesis, revealing a negligible effect size. The combined results indicate that multisensory temporal resolution in humans is unaffected by sound-shape associations, which might arise at a later (or parallel) processing stage compared to cross-modal temporal order judgements. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

在人类身上几乎普遍可以观察到声形关联(例如,有棱角的形状优先与高音调的声音相匹配,光滑的形状优先与低音调的声音相匹配)。如果跨模态一致的声音和形状在人类中的整合能力更强,那么与不一致的声形配对相比,及时区分它们可能更具挑战性。Parise 和 Spence(2009 年;n = 12)的一项被广泛引用的研究报告指出,与不一致的声形关联相比,一致的视听刺激在时间顺序判断上表现更差,这也支持了这一假设。在此,我们报告了两个实验室的五次实验结果,包括一次预先登记的复制尝试,所有实验(∑n = 102)均未能复制原始结果。此外,频数分析和贝叶斯元分析没有发现反对零假设的证据,揭示的效应大小可以忽略不计。综合结果表明,人类的多感官时间分辨率不受声形关联的影响,与跨模态时间顺序判断相比,声形关联可能出现在较晚(或平行)的处理阶段。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"No evidence that sound-shape associations influence temporal resolution in humans: Five nonreplications of Parise and Spence (2009) and meta-analyses.","authors":"Suddha Sourav, Brigitte Röder, Franka Ambsdorf, Andromachi Melissari, Miketa Arvaniti, Argiro Vatakis","doi":"10.1037/xge0001641","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/xge0001641","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Sound-shape associations (e.g., preferentially matching angular shapes with high-pitched sounds and smooth shapes with low-pitched ones) have been almost universally observed in humans. If cross-modally congruent sounds and shapes are more robustly integrated in humans, distinguishing them in time might be hypothetically more challenging compared to incongruent sound-shape pairings. Supporting this premise, a highly cited work by Parise and Spence (2009; <i>n</i> = 12) reported worse temporal order judgement performance for audiovisual stimuli with congruent compared to incongruent sound-shape associations. Here, we report the results of five experiments across two laboratories, including a preregistered replication attempt, all (∑<i>n</i> = 102) failing to replicate the original results. Additionally, frequentist and Bayesian meta-analyses found no evidence against the null hypothesis, revealing a negligible effect size. The combined results indicate that multisensory temporal resolution in humans is unaffected by sound-shape associations, which might arise at a later (or parallel) processing stage compared to cross-modal temporal order judgements. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":15698,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology: General","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.7,"publicationDate":"2024-09-19","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142288952","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
No evidence for unconscious initiation and following of arithmetic rules: A replication study. 没有证据表明算术规则是无意识启动和遵循的:重复研究
IF 4.1 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-09-12 DOI: 10.1037/xge0000622
Amir Tal,Liad Mudrik
The field of consciousness studies has yielded various-sometimes contradicting-accounts regarding the function of consciousness, ranging from denying it has such function to claiming that any high-level cognitive function requires consciousness. Empirical findings supporting both accounts were reported, yet some of them have been recently revisited based on failures to replicate. Here, we aimed at replicating a remarkable finding reported by Ric and Muller (2012); participants were able to follow an unseen instruction, integrate it with a subsequently presented pair of unseen digits, and accordingly either add the digits (resulting in a priming effect), or simply represent them. This finding thus demonstrates unconscious task-switching, temporal integration (involving mental chaining), and arithmetic operation. Finding such high-level processes in the absence of awareness is of pivotal importance to our understanding of consciousness, as it challenges prominent theories in the field (e.g., the global neuronal workspace). Accordingly, in light of the self-correction wave in psychological science in general and in the field of consciousness studies in particular, we report here a preregistered replication aimed at testing the reproducibility of this finding, while also better controlling for subjects' awareness of both the instruction and the digits. Across two highly powered experiments, our results failed to replicate the original effect. We, therefore, conclude that the current evidence does not support the claim that arithmetic operations (specifically, addition) can be flexibly initiated without awareness, in line with the current arguments for a more limited scope of unconscious processing. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).
关于意识的功能,意识研究领域产生了各种说法,有时甚至是相互矛盾的说法,有的否认意识具有这种功能,有的则声称任何高级认知功能都需要意识。支持这两种说法的经验性发现都有报道,但其中一些发现最近因无法复制而被重新审视。在这里,我们的目的是复制里克和穆勒(2012 年)报告的一个显著发现:参与者能够根据一个未见的指令,将其与随后呈现的一对未见的数字整合在一起,并相应地将数字相加(产生引物效应)或简单地表示它们。因此,这一发现证明了无意识的任务切换、时间整合(涉及心理连锁)和算术运算。在没有意识的情况下发现这种高级过程对我们理解意识具有关键意义,因为它挑战了该领域的著名理论(如全局神经元工作空间)。因此,鉴于心理科学尤其是意识研究领域的自我修正浪潮,我们在此报告了一项预先登记的重复实验,旨在测试这一发现的可重复性,同时更好地控制受试者对指令和数字的意识。在两个高能量实验中,我们的结果都未能复制原始效应。因此,我们得出结论,目前的证据并不支持算术运算(特别是加法)可以在无意识的情况下灵活启动的说法,这与目前关于无意识加工范围更有限的论点是一致的。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"No evidence for unconscious initiation and following of arithmetic rules: A replication study.","authors":"Amir Tal,Liad Mudrik","doi":"10.1037/xge0000622","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/xge0000622","url":null,"abstract":"The field of consciousness studies has yielded various-sometimes contradicting-accounts regarding the function of consciousness, ranging from denying it has such function to claiming that any high-level cognitive function requires consciousness. Empirical findings supporting both accounts were reported, yet some of them have been recently revisited based on failures to replicate. Here, we aimed at replicating a remarkable finding reported by Ric and Muller (2012); participants were able to follow an unseen instruction, integrate it with a subsequently presented pair of unseen digits, and accordingly either add the digits (resulting in a priming effect), or simply represent them. This finding thus demonstrates unconscious task-switching, temporal integration (involving mental chaining), and arithmetic operation. Finding such high-level processes in the absence of awareness is of pivotal importance to our understanding of consciousness, as it challenges prominent theories in the field (e.g., the global neuronal workspace). Accordingly, in light of the self-correction wave in psychological science in general and in the field of consciousness studies in particular, we report here a preregistered replication aimed at testing the reproducibility of this finding, while also better controlling for subjects' awareness of both the instruction and the digits. Across two highly powered experiments, our results failed to replicate the original effect. We, therefore, conclude that the current evidence does not support the claim that arithmetic operations (specifically, addition) can be flexibly initiated without awareness, in line with the current arguments for a more limited scope of unconscious processing. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).","PeriodicalId":15698,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology: General","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":4.1,"publicationDate":"2024-09-12","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142174463","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Social contagion of challenge-seeking behavior. 寻求挑战行为的社会传染。
IF 3.7 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-09-09 DOI: 10.1037/xge0001620
Cansu Ogulmus, Ying Lee, Bhismadev Chakrabarti, Kou Murayama

Despite having little economic utility, people are sometimes motivated to seek challenges (i.e., proactively choosing to work on a more difficult task than an easier one). The present study investigated whether just observing others' challenge-seeking behaviors could motivate people to seek more challenging tasks-the social contagion effect of challenge-seeking. The participants were presented with pairs of options, each associated with a math word problem of a certain difficulty level. We examined whether the participants' preference for a more challenging (i.e., more difficult) option changes after observing the decisions of others who hold a challenge-seeking or a challenge-avoiding attitude. Five experiments consistently showed that, while the participants generally avoided challenging word problems, observing challenge-seeking in others increased the probability of participants choosing more challenging options. These results indicate that our motivation to seek challenges may be instilled, in part, through social processes. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

尽管没有什么经济效用,但人们有时还是会主动寻求挑战(即主动选择完成一项比容易的任务更难的任务)。本研究调查了观察他人寻求挑战的行为是否会促使人们寻求更具挑战性的任务--即寻求挑战的社会传染效应。研究人员向参与者展示了一对选项,每个选项都与一个具有一定难度的数学单词问题相关联。我们研究了在观察了其他持有寻求挑战或回避挑战态度的人的决定后,参与者对更具挑战性(即更难)的选项的偏好是否会发生变化。五项实验一致表明,虽然参与者一般都会回避具有挑战性的文字问题,但观察他人寻求挑战的行为会增加参与者选择更具挑战性选项的概率。这些结果表明,我们寻求挑战的动机可能部分是通过社会过程灌输的。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Social contagion of challenge-seeking behavior.","authors":"Cansu Ogulmus, Ying Lee, Bhismadev Chakrabarti, Kou Murayama","doi":"10.1037/xge0001620","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/xge0001620","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Despite having little economic utility, people are sometimes motivated to seek challenges (i.e., proactively choosing to work on a more difficult task than an easier one). The present study investigated whether just observing others' challenge-seeking behaviors could motivate people to seek more challenging tasks-the social contagion effect of challenge-seeking. The participants were presented with pairs of options, each associated with a math word problem of a certain difficulty level. We examined whether the participants' preference for a more challenging (i.e., more difficult) option changes after observing the decisions of others who hold a challenge-seeking or a challenge-avoiding attitude. Five experiments consistently showed that, while the participants generally avoided challenging word problems, observing challenge-seeking in others increased the probability of participants choosing more challenging options. These results indicate that our motivation to seek challenges may be instilled, in part, through social processes. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":15698,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology: General","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.7,"publicationDate":"2024-09-09","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142288954","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Journal of Experimental Psychology: General
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1