首页 > 最新文献

Journal of Experimental Psychology: General最新文献

英文 中文
Bypassing versus correcting misinformation: Efficacy and fundamental processes. 绕过与纠正错误信息:功效和基本过程。
IF 3.7 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-11-18 DOI: 10.1037/xge0001687
Javier A Granados Samayoa, Dolores Albarracín

The standard method for addressing the consequences of misinformation is the provision of a correction in which the misinformation is directly refuted. However, the impact of misinformation may also be successfully addressed by introducing or bolstering alternative beliefs with opposite evaluative implications. Six preregistered experiments clarified important processes influencing the impact of bypassing versus correcting misinformation via negation. First, we find that, following exposure to misinformation, bypassing generally changes people's attitudes and intentions more than correction in the form of a simple negation. Second, this relative advantage is not a function of the depth at which information is processed but rather the degree to which people form attitudes or beliefs when they receive the misinformation. When people form attitudes when they first receive the misinformation, bypassing has no advantage over corrections, likely owing to anchoring. In contrast, when individuals focus on the accuracy of the statements and form beliefs, bypassing is significantly more successful at changing their attitudes because these attitudes are constructed based on expectancy-value principles, while misinformation continues to influence attitudes after correction. Broader implications of this work are discussed. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

处理错误信息后果的标准方法是提供更正,直接驳斥错误信息。然而,通过引入或强化具有相反评价意义的替代信念,也可以成功地消除错误信息的影响。六项预先登记的实验阐明了影响通过否定绕过或纠正错误信息的重要过程。首先,我们发现,在接触到错误信息后,绕过一般比简单否定形式的纠正更能改变人们的态度和意图。其次,这种相对优势并不是信息处理深度的函数,而是人们在接收错误信息时形成态度或信念的程度。当人们第一次接收到错误信息时就形成了态度,那么绕过与更正相比就没有优势,这很可能是由于锚定的缘故。相反,当人们关注陈述的准确性并形成信念时,绕过在改变态度方面明显更成功,因为这些态度是根据期望值原则构建的,而错误信息在纠正后会继续影响人们的态度。本文讨论了这项工作的更广泛意义。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Bypassing versus correcting misinformation: Efficacy and fundamental processes.","authors":"Javier A Granados Samayoa, Dolores Albarracín","doi":"10.1037/xge0001687","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/xge0001687","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The standard method for addressing the consequences of misinformation is the provision of a correction in which the misinformation is directly refuted. However, the impact of misinformation may also be successfully addressed by introducing or bolstering alternative beliefs with opposite evaluative implications. Six preregistered experiments clarified important processes influencing the impact of bypassing versus correcting misinformation via negation. First, we find that, following exposure to misinformation, bypassing generally changes people's attitudes and intentions more than correction in the form of a simple negation. Second, this relative advantage is not a function of the depth at which information is processed but rather the degree to which people form attitudes or beliefs when they receive the misinformation. When people form attitudes when they first receive the misinformation, bypassing has no advantage over corrections, likely owing to anchoring. In contrast, when individuals focus on the accuracy of the statements and form beliefs, bypassing is significantly more successful at changing their attitudes because these attitudes are constructed based on expectancy-value principles, while misinformation continues to influence attitudes after correction. Broader implications of this work are discussed. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":15698,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology: General","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":3.7,"publicationDate":"2024-11-18","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142648221","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Shortcuts to insincerity: Texting abbreviations seem insincere and not worth answering. 不真诚的捷径:短信缩写显得不真诚,不值得回复。
IF 3.7 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-11-14 DOI: 10.1037/xge0001684
David Fang, Yiran Eileen Zhang, Sam J Maglio

As social interactions increasingly move to digital platforms, communicators confront new factors that enhance or diminish virtual interactions. Texting abbreviations, for instance, are now pervasive in digital communication-but do they enhance or diminish interactions? The present study examines the influence of texting abbreviation usage on interpersonal perceptions. We explore how texting abbreviations affect perceived sender sincerity and the subsequent likelihood that recipients respond. Eight preregistered studies (N = 5,306) using mixed methods (e.g., surveys, field and lab experiments, and archival analysis of Tinder conversations) find that abbreviations make senders seem less sincere and recipients less likely to write back. These negative effects arise because abbreviations signal a lower level of effort from the sender. Communicator familiarity and text exchange length do not attenuate these effects, providing evidence for a robust phenomenon. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

随着社交互动越来越多地转移到数字平台上,传播者面临着新的因素,这些因素会增强或削弱虚拟互动。例如,发短信缩写如今在数字交流中十分普遍,但它们会增强还是削弱互动呢?本研究探讨了短信缩写的使用对人际感知的影响。我们探讨了短信缩写如何影响发信人的真诚感以及随后收信人做出回应的可能性。八项预先登记的研究(N = 5306)采用混合方法(如调查、现场和实验室实验以及 Tinder 对话档案分析)发现,缩写会使发信人显得不那么真诚,收信人也不太可能回信。产生这些负面影响的原因是,缩写意味着发送者的努力程度较低。交流者的熟悉程度和文本交流的长度并不能减弱这些影响,从而为这一强大的现象提供了证据。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Shortcuts to insincerity: Texting abbreviations seem insincere and not worth answering.","authors":"David Fang, Yiran Eileen Zhang, Sam J Maglio","doi":"10.1037/xge0001684","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/xge0001684","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>As social interactions increasingly move to digital platforms, communicators confront new factors that enhance or diminish virtual interactions. Texting abbreviations, for instance, are now pervasive in digital communication-but do they enhance or diminish interactions? The present study examines the influence of texting abbreviation usage on interpersonal perceptions. We explore how texting abbreviations affect perceived sender sincerity and the subsequent likelihood that recipients respond. Eight preregistered studies (<i>N</i> = 5,306) using mixed methods (e.g., surveys, field and lab experiments, and archival analysis of Tinder conversations) find that abbreviations make senders seem less sincere and recipients less likely to write back. These negative effects arise because abbreviations signal a lower level of effort from the sender. Communicator familiarity and text exchange length do not attenuate these effects, providing evidence for a robust phenomenon. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":15698,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology: General","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":3.7,"publicationDate":"2024-11-14","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142621743","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Risky hybrid foraging: The impact of risk, reward value, and prevalence on foraging behavior in hybrid visual search. 风险混合觅食:混合视觉搜索中风险、奖励价值和普遍性对觅食行为的影响。
IF 3.7 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-11-14 DOI: 10.1037/xge0001652
Yanjun Liu, Jeremy M Wolfe, Jennifer S Trueblood

In hybrid foraging, foragers search for multiple targets in multiple patches throughout the foraging session, mimicking a range of real-world scenarios. This research examines outcome uncertainty, the prevalence of different target types, and the reward value of targets in human hybrid foraging. Our empirical findings show a consistent tendency toward risk-averse behavior in hybrid foraging. That is, people display a preference for certainty and actively avoid taking risks. While altering the prevalence or reward value of the risky targets does influence people's aversion to risk, the overall effect of risk remains dominant. Additionally, simulation results suggest that the observed risk-averse strategy is suboptimal in the sense that it prevents foragers from maximizing their overall returns. These results underscore the crucial role of outcome uncertainty in shaping hybrid foraging behavior and shed light on potential theoretical developments bridging theories in decision making and hybrid foraging. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

在混合觅食中,觅食者会在整个觅食过程中在多个斑块中寻找多个目标,这模拟了一系列真实世界的场景。这项研究考察了人类混合觅食中结果的不确定性、不同目标类型的普遍性以及目标的奖励价值。我们的实证研究结果表明,在混合觅食中,人们的行为始终倾向于规避风险。也就是说,人们表现出对确定性的偏好,并积极避免承担风险。虽然改变风险目标的普遍性或奖励价值确实会影响人们对风险的厌恶,但风险的总体影响仍然占主导地位。此外,模拟结果表明,观察到的规避风险策略是次优的,因为它阻碍了觅食者获得最大的总体收益。这些结果强调了结果的不确定性在形成混合觅食行为中的关键作用,并揭示了连接决策理论和混合觅食理论的潜在理论发展。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Risky hybrid foraging: The impact of risk, reward value, and prevalence on foraging behavior in hybrid visual search.","authors":"Yanjun Liu, Jeremy M Wolfe, Jennifer S Trueblood","doi":"10.1037/xge0001652","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/xge0001652","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>In hybrid foraging, foragers search for multiple targets in multiple patches throughout the foraging session, mimicking a range of real-world scenarios. This research examines outcome uncertainty, the prevalence of different target types, and the reward value of targets in human hybrid foraging. Our empirical findings show a consistent tendency toward risk-averse behavior in hybrid foraging. That is, people display a preference for certainty and actively avoid taking risks. While altering the prevalence or reward value of the risky targets does influence people's aversion to risk, the overall effect of risk remains dominant. Additionally, simulation results suggest that the observed risk-averse strategy is suboptimal in the sense that it prevents foragers from maximizing their overall returns. These results underscore the crucial role of outcome uncertainty in shaping hybrid foraging behavior and shed light on potential theoretical developments bridging theories in decision making and hybrid foraging. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":15698,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology: General","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":3.7,"publicationDate":"2024-11-14","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142621742","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Confidence regulates feedback processing during human probabilistic learning. 信心调节人类概率学习过程中的反馈处理。
IF 3.7 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-11-11 DOI: 10.1037/xge0001669
Michael Ben Yehuda, Robin A Murphy, Mike E Le Pelley, Danielle J Navarro, Nick Yeung

Uncertainty presents a key challenge when learning how best to act to attain a desired outcome. People can report uncertainty in the form of confidence judgments, but how such judgments contribute to learning and subsequent decisions remains unclear. In a series of three experiments employing an operant learning task, we tested the hypothesis that confidence plays a central role in learning by regulating resource allocation to the seeking and processing of feedback. We predicted that, as participants' confidence in their task knowledge grew, they would discount feedback when it was provided and be correspondingly less willing to pay for it when it was costly. Consistent with these predictions, we found that higher confidence was associated with reduced electrophysiological markers of feedback processing and decreased updating of beliefs following feedback receipt. Bayesian modeling suggests that this decrease in processing was due to a drop in the expected informative value of novel information when participants were highly confident. Thus, when choosing whether to pay a fee to receive further feedback, participants' subjective confidence, rather than the objective accuracy of their decisions, guided their choices. Overall, our results suggest that confidence regulates learning and subsequent decision making. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

在学习如何采取最佳行动以获得理想结果时,不确定性是一个关键挑战。人们可以用信心判断的形式来报告不确定性,但这种判断如何有助于学习和后续决策仍不清楚。在一系列采用操作性学习任务的三个实验中,我们检验了这样一个假设,即信心通过调节寻求和处理反馈的资源分配,在学习中发挥着核心作用。我们预测,随着参与者对自己任务知识的信心增强,他们在获得反馈时会打折扣,相应地,当反馈成本较高时,他们就不太愿意为之付出代价。与这些预测一致的是,我们发现信心越高,反馈处理的电生理指标越低,收到反馈后信念更新的次数也越少。贝叶斯模型表明,这种处理的减少是由于参与者高度自信时,新信息的预期信息价值下降所致。因此,在选择是否支付费用以获得进一步的反馈时,参与者的主观信心,而不是其决策的客观准确性,引导着他们的选择。总之,我们的研究结果表明,信心对学习和后续决策具有调节作用。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Confidence regulates feedback processing during human probabilistic learning.","authors":"Michael Ben Yehuda, Robin A Murphy, Mike E Le Pelley, Danielle J Navarro, Nick Yeung","doi":"10.1037/xge0001669","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/xge0001669","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Uncertainty presents a key challenge when learning how best to act to attain a desired outcome. People can report uncertainty in the form of confidence judgments, but how such judgments contribute to learning and subsequent decisions remains unclear. In a series of three experiments employing an operant learning task, we tested the hypothesis that confidence plays a central role in learning by regulating resource allocation to the seeking and processing of feedback. We predicted that, as participants' confidence in their task knowledge grew, they would discount feedback when it was provided and be correspondingly less willing to pay for it when it was costly. Consistent with these predictions, we found that higher confidence was associated with reduced electrophysiological markers of feedback processing and decreased updating of beliefs following feedback receipt. Bayesian modeling suggests that this decrease in processing was due to a drop in the expected informative value of novel information when participants were highly confident. Thus, when choosing whether to pay a fee to receive further feedback, participants' subjective confidence, rather than the objective accuracy of their decisions, guided their choices. Overall, our results suggest that confidence regulates learning and subsequent decision making. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":15698,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology: General","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":3.7,"publicationDate":"2024-11-11","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142621741","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Recognizing the beauty in diversity: Exposure to body-positive content on social media broadens women's concept of ideal body weight. 认识到多样性的美:社交媒体上积极正面的身体内容扩大了女性对理想体重的概念。
IF 3.7 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-11-01 Epub Date: 2023-05-11 DOI: 10.1037/xge0001397
Jan-Philipp Stein, Sophie Scheufen, Markus Appel

By depicting an unrealistic share of skinny or toned body types, modern mass media have been found to shift users' perception of an ideal body toward narrow and often unattainable standards. In response to this, the "#bodypositivity" (BoPo) movement on social media has set out to challenge restrictive body ideals, advocating for more open-minded views toward the human physique. Matching BoPo's emphasis on diversity, we hypothesized that viewing body-positive online content alters women's concept of an ideal body to encompass a broader range of body shapes (on a spectrum from skinny to obese). The results of two pre-registered experiments (N₁ = 191; N₂ = 266) support our assumption, connecting BoPo not only to a larger mean ideal body shape but also to a diversification of weight-related standards. We discuss our work as a crucial extension of prior research, noting that the range found in people's bodily ideals may be (at least) as relevant as their central tendency. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

现代大众媒体通过描绘不切实际的瘦削或健美体型,使用户对理想体型的认识转向狭隘且往往难以企及的标准。为此,社交媒体上的 "#身体积极性"(BoPo)运动开始挑战限制性的身体理想,倡导以更开放的心态看待人类体型。为了与 BoPo 运动对多样性的强调相匹配,我们提出了这样的假设:观看正面人体的网络内容会改变女性对理想体型的概念,使其涵盖更广泛的体型(从瘦到肥胖)。两个预注册实验(N₁ = 191;N₂ = 266)的结果支持了我们的假设,BoPo 不仅与更大的平均理想体形有关,还与体重相关标准的多样化有关。我们认为我们的研究是对先前研究的重要扩展,并指出人们身体理想的范围可能(至少)与他们的中心倾向一样重要。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Recognizing the beauty in diversity: Exposure to body-positive content on social media broadens women's concept of ideal body weight.","authors":"Jan-Philipp Stein, Sophie Scheufen, Markus Appel","doi":"10.1037/xge0001397","DOIUrl":"10.1037/xge0001397","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>By depicting an unrealistic share of skinny or toned body types, modern mass media have been found to shift users' perception of an ideal body toward narrow and often unattainable standards. In response to this, the \"#bodypositivity\" (BoPo) movement on social media has set out to challenge restrictive body ideals, advocating for more open-minded views toward the human physique. Matching BoPo's emphasis on diversity, we hypothesized that viewing body-positive online content alters women's concept of an ideal body to encompass a broader range of body shapes (on a spectrum from skinny to obese). The results of two pre-registered experiments (<i>N</i>₁ = 191; <i>N</i>₂ = 266) support our assumption, connecting BoPo not only to a larger mean ideal body shape but also to a <i>diversification</i> of weight-related standards. We discuss our work as a crucial extension of prior research, noting that the range found in people's bodily ideals may be (at least) as relevant as their central tendency. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":15698,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology: General","volume":" ","pages":"2642-2656"},"PeriodicalIF":3.7,"publicationDate":"2024-11-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9813842","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The developmental trajectories of children's reorientation to global and local properties of environmental geometry. 儿童对环境几何的整体和局部特性进行重新定位的发展轨迹。
IF 4.1 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-04-01 Epub Date: 2022-08-04 DOI: 10.1037/xge0001265
Matthew G Buckley, Luke J Holden, Alastair D Smith, Mark Haselgrove

The way in which organisms represent the shape of their environments during navigation has been debated in cognitive, comparative, and developmental psychology. While there is evidence that adult humans encode the entire boundary shape of an environment (a global-shape representation), there are also data demonstrating that organisms reorient using only segments of the boundary that signal a goal location (a local-shape representation). Developmental studies offer unique insights into this debate; however, most studies have used designs that cannot dissociate the type of boundary-shape representation that children use to guide reorientation. Thus, we examined the developmental trajectories of children's reorientation according to local and global boundary shape. Participants aged 6-12 years were trained to find a goal hidden in one corner of a virtual arena, after which they were required to reorient in a novel test arena. From 10.5 years, children performed above chance when the test arena permitted reorientation based only on local-shape (Experiment 2), or only global-shape (Experiment 3) information. Moreover, when these responses were placed into conflict, older children reoriented with respect to global-shape information (Experiment 4). These age-related findings were not due to older children being better able to reorient in virtual environments per se: when trained and tested within the same environment (Experiment 1), children performed above chance from 6 years. Together, our results suggest (a) the ability to reorient on the basis of global- and local-shape representations develops in parallel, and (b) shape-based information is weighted to determine which representation informs reorientation. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

在认知、比较和发展心理学中,生物在导航过程中表示环境形状的方式一直备受争议。虽然有证据表明,成年人类会对环境的整个边界形状进行编码(全局形状表征),但也有数据表明,生物体只利用边界中表示目标位置的片段来调整方向(局部形状表征)。发育研究为这一争论提供了独特的见解;然而,大多数研究采用的设计无法区分儿童用于指导调整方向的边界形状表征类型。因此,我们研究了儿童根据局部和全局边界形状调整方向的发展轨迹。我们对 6-12 岁的儿童进行了训练,让他们找到隐藏在虚拟竞技场一角的目标,然后要求他们在新的测试竞技场中调整方向。从 10.5 岁开始,当测试场仅允许根据局部形状信息(实验 2)或仅允许根据全局形状信息(实验 3)调整方向时,儿童的表现均高于正常水平。此外,当这些反应发生冲突时,年龄较大的儿童会根据整体形状信息调整方向(实验 4)。这些与年龄相关的结果并不是因为大龄儿童在虚拟环境中调整方向的能力更强:当在同一环境中进行训练和测试时(实验 1),6 岁以上儿童的表现均高于正常水平。总之,我们的研究结果表明:(a) 根据整体和局部形状表征调整方向的能力是平行发展的;(b) 基于形状的信息是加权的,以决定哪种表征为调整方向提供信息。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved)。
{"title":"The developmental trajectories of children's reorientation to global and local properties of environmental geometry.","authors":"Matthew G Buckley, Luke J Holden, Alastair D Smith, Mark Haselgrove","doi":"10.1037/xge0001265","DOIUrl":"10.1037/xge0001265","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The way in which organisms represent the shape of their environments during navigation has been debated in cognitive, comparative, and developmental psychology. While there is evidence that adult humans encode the entire boundary shape of an environment (a global-shape representation), there are also data demonstrating that organisms reorient using only segments of the boundary that signal a goal location (a local-shape representation). Developmental studies offer unique insights into this debate; however, most studies have used designs that cannot dissociate the type of boundary-shape representation that children use to guide reorientation. Thus, we examined the developmental trajectories of children's reorientation according to local and global boundary shape. Participants aged 6-12 years were trained to find a goal hidden in one corner of a virtual arena, after which they were required to reorient in a novel test arena. From 10.5 years, children performed above chance when the test arena permitted reorientation based only on local-shape (Experiment 2), or only global-shape (Experiment 3) information. Moreover, when these responses were placed into conflict, older children reoriented with respect to global-shape information (Experiment 4). These age-related findings were not due to older children being better able to reorient in virtual environments per se: when trained and tested within the same environment (Experiment 1), children performed above chance from 6 years. Together, our results suggest (a) the ability to reorient on the basis of global- and local-shape representations develops in parallel, and (b) shape-based information is weighted to determine which representation informs reorientation. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":15698,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology: General","volume":" ","pages":"889-912"},"PeriodicalIF":4.1,"publicationDate":"2024-04-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11115358/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9352746","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The adaptive flexibility of rhythmic attentional sampling in attending to multiple targets. 注意多个目标时节奏注意取样的适应灵活性。
IF 4.1 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-01-01 Epub Date: 2023-09-14 DOI: 10.1037/xge0001468
Yong Jiang, Sheng He, Jiedong Zhang

Recent behavioral and neural imaging studies revealed a rhythmic sampling in the theta-band (3-8 Hz) of attention. Such observation indicates that visual attention sequentially visits attended locations rapidly and periodically to cover multiple spatial locations, which is believed driven by a general sampling mechanism with a sampling rate invariant to the number of targets. However, a general sampling mechanism with a fixed rate would lead to the consequence that it would take longer time for attention to revisit the same item when attention needs to cover more items, which could impair perceptual continuity. It is unclear whether and how the attentional sampling mechanism can flexibly adapt to varying task demand to balance between covering more items and maintaining stable perception. Here with five behavioral experiments, we investigated how the sequential sampling mechanism adapts to the need of attending to from one to four locations. With state-of-the-art analysis methods, results show clear evidence of sequential sampling in attending to multiple locations, that both theta-band oscillations and phase-shift among different locations were observed in the behavioral performance. At each location, the oscillation period increased when the attended locations increased from one to three, maintaining a relatively stable attention-dwelling time at each location. Critically, oscillation period remained essentially the same from three to four, suggesting a flexible task-driven acceleration of attentional sampling to keep the revisiting duration within a reasonable range. Thus, our results reveal that the generally stable rhythmic attention mechanism could flexibly adjust its sampling rate to accommodate increased attentional demands. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

最近的行为学和神经成像研究发现,注意力在θ波段(3-8赫兹)有节奏地采样。这种观察结果表明,视觉注意力会快速、周期性地依次访问被注意的位置,以覆盖多个空间位置,这被认为是由采样率与目标数量无关的一般采样机制驱动的。然而,如果采用固定速率的一般取样机制,则会导致当注意力需要覆盖更多项目时,需要更长的时间重新访问同一项目,从而影响知觉的连续性。目前还不清楚注意取样机制是否以及如何灵活适应不同的任务需求,在覆盖更多项目和保持知觉稳定之间取得平衡。在这里,我们通过五项行为实验研究了顺序取样机制如何适应从一个到四个位置的注意需求。通过最先进的分析方法,实验结果表明,在关注多个位置时,顺序采样机制有明确的证据,在行为表现中可以观察到θ波段振荡和不同位置间的相移。在每个地点,当注意的地点从一个增加到三个时,振荡周期会增加,在每个地点保持相对稳定的注意停留时间。重要的是,振荡周期从三个增加到四个时基本保持不变,这表明任务驱动的注意采样会灵活加速,从而将重访时间保持在合理的范围内。因此,我们的研究结果表明,普遍稳定的节律注意机制可以灵活地调整其采样率,以适应增加的注意需求。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"The adaptive flexibility of rhythmic attentional sampling in attending to multiple targets.","authors":"Yong Jiang, Sheng He, Jiedong Zhang","doi":"10.1037/xge0001468","DOIUrl":"10.1037/xge0001468","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Recent behavioral and neural imaging studies revealed a rhythmic sampling in the theta-band (3-8 Hz) of attention. Such observation indicates that visual attention sequentially visits attended locations rapidly and periodically to cover multiple spatial locations, which is believed driven by a general sampling mechanism with a sampling rate invariant to the number of targets. However, a general sampling mechanism with a fixed rate would lead to the consequence that it would take longer time for attention to revisit the same item when attention needs to cover more items, which could impair perceptual continuity. It is unclear whether and how the attentional sampling mechanism can flexibly adapt to varying task demand to balance between covering more items and maintaining stable perception. Here with five behavioral experiments, we investigated how the sequential sampling mechanism adapts to the need of attending to from one to four locations. With state-of-the-art analysis methods, results show clear evidence of sequential sampling in attending to multiple locations, that both theta-band oscillations and phase-shift among different locations were observed in the behavioral performance. At each location, the oscillation period increased when the attended locations increased from one to three, maintaining a relatively stable attention-dwelling time at each location. Critically, oscillation period remained essentially the same from three to four, suggesting a flexible task-driven acceleration of attentional sampling to keep the revisiting duration within a reasonable range. Thus, our results reveal that the generally stable rhythmic attention mechanism could flexibly adjust its sampling rate to accommodate increased attentional demands. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":15698,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology: General","volume":" ","pages":"26-37"},"PeriodicalIF":4.1,"publicationDate":"2024-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10231034","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Judgments during perceptual comparisons predict distinct forms of memory updating. 知觉比较过程中的判断预示着不同形式的记忆更新。
IF 4.1 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-01-01 Epub Date: 2023-08-31 DOI: 10.1037/xge0001469
Joseph M Saito, Gi-Yeul Bae, Keisuke Fukuda

Comparing a visual memory with new visual stimuli can bias memory content, especially when the new stimuli are perceived as similar. Perceptual comparisons of this kind may play a mechanistic role in memory updating and can explain how memories can become erroneous in daily life. To test this possibility, we investigated whether comparisons can produce other types of memory distortion beyond memory bias that are commonly implicated in erroneous memories (e.g., memory misattribution). We hypothesized that the type of memory distortion induced during a comparison depends on the perceived overlap between the memory and incoming stimulus-when the input is perceived as similar, it biases memory content; when perceived as the same, it replaces memory content. Participants completed a delayed estimation task in which they compared their memories of color (Experiment 1) and shape stimuli (Experiment 2) to probe stimuli before reporting memory content. We found systematic errors in participants' memory reports following perceived similarity and sameness that were toward the probes and larger following perceived sameness. Simulations confirmed that these errors were not explained by noisy encoding processes that occurred before comparisons. Instead, computational modeling suggested that these errors were likely explained by the probabilistic replacement of the memory by the probe following perceived sameness and integration between the memory and the probe following perceived similarity. Together, these findings suggest that perceptual comparisons can prompt distinct forms of memory updating that have been described previously and may explain how memories become erroneous during their use in everyday behavior. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

将视觉记忆与新的视觉刺激进行比较会使记忆内容产生偏差,尤其是当新的刺激被认为是相似的时候。这种知觉上的比较可能在记忆更新中起到机制性的作用,并能解释日常生活中记忆为何会出错。为了验证这种可能性,我们研究了比较是否会产生除记忆偏差之外的其他类型的记忆扭曲,而记忆偏差通常与错误记忆(如记忆错误归因)有关。我们假设,在比较过程中引起的记忆失真类型取决于记忆和输入刺激之间的感知重叠--当输入被感知为相似时,它会使记忆内容产生偏差;当输入被感知为相同时,它会取代记忆内容。受试者完成了一项延迟估计任务,在报告记忆内容之前,他们将自己对颜色刺激(实验 1)和形状刺激(实验 2)的记忆与探测刺激进行了比较。我们发现,在感知到相似性和相同性之后,参与者的记忆报告会出现系统误差,这些误差向探针方向发展,而在感知到相同性之后则会更大。模拟证实,这些错误不能用比较前发生的噪声编码过程来解释。相反,计算建模表明,这些错误很可能是由感知相同后探针对记忆的概率替换以及感知相似后记忆与探针之间的整合造成的。这些发现共同表明,知觉比较可以促使记忆更新的不同形式,而这些记忆更新形式之前已经被描述过,并且可以解释记忆在日常行为中是如何变得错误的。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Judgments during perceptual comparisons predict distinct forms of memory updating.","authors":"Joseph M Saito, Gi-Yeul Bae, Keisuke Fukuda","doi":"10.1037/xge0001469","DOIUrl":"10.1037/xge0001469","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Comparing a visual memory with new visual stimuli can bias memory content, especially when the new stimuli are perceived as similar. Perceptual comparisons of this kind may play a mechanistic role in memory updating and can explain how memories can become erroneous in daily life. To test this possibility, we investigated whether comparisons can produce other types of memory distortion beyond memory bias that are commonly implicated in erroneous memories (e.g., memory misattribution). We hypothesized that the type of memory distortion induced during a comparison depends on the perceived overlap between the memory and incoming stimulus-when the input is perceived as similar, it biases memory content; when perceived as the same, it replaces memory content. Participants completed a delayed estimation task in which they compared their memories of color (Experiment 1) and shape stimuli (Experiment 2) to probe stimuli before reporting memory content. We found systematic errors in participants' memory reports following perceived similarity and sameness that were toward the probes and larger following perceived sameness. Simulations confirmed that these errors were not explained by noisy encoding processes that occurred before comparisons. Instead, computational modeling suggested that these errors were likely explained by the probabilistic replacement of the memory by the probe following perceived sameness and integration between the memory and the probe following perceived similarity. Together, these findings suggest that perceptual comparisons can prompt distinct forms of memory updating that have been described previously and may explain how memories become erroneous during their use in everyday behavior. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":15698,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology: General","volume":" ","pages":"38-55"},"PeriodicalIF":4.1,"publicationDate":"2024-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10122670","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Object-based encoding constrains storage in visual working memory. 基于对象的编码限制了视觉工作记忆中的存储。
IF 4.1 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-01-01 Epub Date: 2023-09-11 DOI: 10.1037/xge0001479
William X Q Ngiam, Krystian B Loetscher, Edward Awh

The fundamental unit of visual working memory (WM) has been debated for decades. WM could be object-based, such that capacity is set by the number of individuated objects, or feature-based, such that capacity is determined by the total number of feature values stored. The present work examined whether object- or feature-based models would best explain how multifeature objects (i.e., color/orientation or color/shape) are encoded into visual WM. If maximum capacity is limited by the number of individuated objects, then above-chance performance should be restricted to the same number of items as in a single-feature condition. By contrast, if the capacity is determined by independent storage resources for distinct features-without respect to the objects that contain those features-then successful storage of feature values could be distributed across a larger number of objects than when only a single feature is relevant. We conducted four experiments using a whole-report task in which subjects reported both features from every item in a six-item array. The crucial finding was that above-chance recall-for both single- and multifeatured objects-was restricted to the first three or four responses, while the later responses were best modeled as guesses. Thus, whole-report with multifeature objects reveals a distribution of recalled features that indicates an object-based limit on WM capacity. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

视觉工作记忆的基本单位已经争论了几十年。WM可以是基于对象的,使得容量由单个对象的数量设置,或者基于特征的,使得能力由存储的特征值的总数确定。目前的工作考察了基于对象或特征的模型是否能最好地解释多特征对象(即颜色/方向或颜色/形状)如何被编码到视觉WM中。如果最大容量受到单个对象数量的限制,那么机会性能应限制在与单个特征条件下相同的项目数量。相反,如果容量是由不同特征的独立存储资源决定的,而不考虑包含这些特征的对象,那么与仅单个特征相关时相比,特征值的成功存储可以分布在更多的对象上。我们使用一个完整的报告任务进行了四个实验,受试者报告了六个项目阵列中每个项目的两个特征。关键的发现是,单特征和多特征物体的偶然回忆都仅限于前三个或四个反应,而后面的反应最好被建模为猜测。因此,具有多特征对象的整个报告揭示了召回特征的分布,这表明基于对象的WM容量限制。(PsycInfo数据库记录(c)2023 APA,保留所有权利)。
{"title":"Object-based encoding constrains storage in visual working memory.","authors":"William X Q Ngiam, Krystian B Loetscher, Edward Awh","doi":"10.1037/xge0001479","DOIUrl":"10.1037/xge0001479","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The fundamental unit of visual working memory (WM) has been debated for decades. WM could be object-based, such that capacity is set by the number of individuated objects, or feature-based, such that capacity is determined by the total number of feature values stored. The present work examined whether object- or feature-based models would best explain how multifeature objects (i.e., color/orientation or color/shape) are encoded into visual WM. If maximum capacity is limited by the number of individuated objects, then above-chance performance should be restricted to the same number of items as in a single-feature condition. By contrast, if the capacity is determined by independent storage resources for distinct features-without respect to the objects that contain those features-then successful storage of feature values could be distributed across a larger number of objects than when only a single feature is relevant. We conducted four experiments using a whole-report task in which subjects reported both features from every item in a six-item array. The crucial finding was that above-chance recall-for both single- and multifeatured objects-was restricted to the first three or four responses, while the later responses were best modeled as guesses. Thus, whole-report with multifeature objects reveals a distribution of recalled features that indicates an object-based limit on WM capacity. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":15698,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology: General","volume":" ","pages":"86-101"},"PeriodicalIF":4.1,"publicationDate":"2024-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC10840914/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10554851","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Immediate cross-language transfer of novel articulatory plans in bilingual speech. 双语语音中新颖发音计划的即时跨语言转移。
IF 4.1 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-01-01 Epub Date: 2023-08-10 DOI: 10.1037/xge0001456
Douglas M Shiller, Sarah Bobbitt, Daniel R Lametti

Current models of second language (L2) acquisition focus on interactions with a first language (L1) at the level of speech sound targets. In multilinguals, the degree of interaction between the articulatory plans that guide speech in each language remains unclear. Here, we directly address this question in bilingual speakers. We use a sensorimotor adaptation paradigm to drive the acquisition of novel articulatory plans for speech in one language and then measure the extent to which these new motor plans influence articulatory plans in the speaker's other language. Twenty L1-French, L2-English bilinguals adapted their speech production to a real-time alteration of vowel sounds. In one session, the adaptation was acquired during French sentence production; in a second session, the adaptation was acquired during English sentence production. In each session, cross-language transfer of these novel articulatory plans for speech was assessed using a transfer task that involved the production of French and English words with heavily noise-masked auditory feedback. Sensorimotor adaptation that countered the vowel sound alteration was observed in both French and English. Regardless of the linguistic context in which the adaptation was acquired, the adaptation transferred to the production of words in both languages. The amount of transfer did not depend on whether the adaptation was acquired in the participant's L1 or L2. In a second experiment, the result was replicated with 20 L1-English, L2-French speakers. The experiments support the idea that, in bilinguals, the interaction between L1 and L2 articulatory motor plans is rapid and bidirectional. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

目前的第二语言(L2)习得模型侧重于与第一语言(L1)在语音目标层面上的互动。在多语言者中,指导每种语言语音的发音计划之间的互动程度仍不清楚。在这里,我们直接在双语者身上解决这个问题。我们使用传感器运动适应范式来驱动一种语言的新发音计划的习得,然后测量这些新运动计划对说话者另一种语言发音计划的影响程度。20 名第一语言为法语、第二语言为英语的双语者根据元音的实时变化来调整他们的语音。在一次训练中,适应是在法语造句过程中获得的;在第二次训练中,适应是在英语造句过程中获得的。在每次训练中,这些新的语音发音计划的跨语言转换都要通过一项转换任务来评估,该任务包括在重度噪声掩蔽的听觉反馈下制作法语和英语单词。在法语和英语中都观察到了对抗元音改变的感觉运动适应。无论适应是在何种语言环境中获得的,适应都会转移到两种语言的单词发音中。转移的程度并不取决于适应是在被试的第一语言还是第二语言中获得的。在第二项实验中,20 名母语为英语、母语为法语的受试者重复了这一结果。这些实验支持了这样一种观点,即在双语者中,第一语言和第二语言发音运动计划之间的相互作用是快速和双向的。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Immediate cross-language transfer of novel articulatory plans in bilingual speech.","authors":"Douglas M Shiller, Sarah Bobbitt, Daniel R Lametti","doi":"10.1037/xge0001456","DOIUrl":"10.1037/xge0001456","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Current models of second language (L2) acquisition focus on interactions with a first language (L1) at the level of speech sound targets. In multilinguals, the degree of interaction between the articulatory plans that guide speech in each language remains unclear. Here, we directly address this question in bilingual speakers. We use a sensorimotor adaptation paradigm to drive the acquisition of novel articulatory plans for speech in one language and then measure the extent to which these new motor plans influence articulatory plans in the speaker's other language. Twenty L1-French, L2-English bilinguals adapted their speech production to a real-time alteration of vowel sounds. In one session, the adaptation was acquired during French sentence production; in a second session, the adaptation was acquired during English sentence production. In each session, cross-language transfer of these novel articulatory plans for speech was assessed using a transfer task that involved the production of French and English words with heavily noise-masked auditory feedback. Sensorimotor adaptation that countered the vowel sound alteration was observed in both French and English. Regardless of the linguistic context in which the adaptation was acquired, the adaptation transferred to the production of words in both languages. The amount of transfer did not depend on whether the adaptation was acquired in the participant's L1 or L2. In a second experiment, the result was replicated with 20 L1-English, L2-French speakers. The experiments support the idea that, in bilinguals, the interaction between L1 and L2 articulatory motor plans is rapid and bidirectional. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":15698,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology: General","volume":" ","pages":"15-25"},"PeriodicalIF":4.1,"publicationDate":"2024-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10321000","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Journal of Experimental Psychology: General
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1