首页 > 最新文献

Journal of Experimental Psychology: General最新文献

英文 中文
Reading a graph is like reading a paragraph. 阅读图表就像阅读段落。
IF 3.7 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-07-01 Epub Date: 2024-05-23 DOI: 10.1037/xge0001604
Tal Boger, Steven Franconeri

Vision provides rapid processing for some tasks, but encounters strong constraints from others. Although many tasks encounter a capacity limit of processing four visual objects at once, some evidence suggests far lower limits for processing relationships among objects. What is our capacity limit for relational processing? If it is indeed limited, then people may miss important relationships between data values in a graph. To test this question, we asked people to explore graphs of trivially simple 2 × 2 data sets and found that half of the viewers missed surprising and improbable relationships (e.g., a child's height decreasing over time). These relationships were spotted easily in a control condition, which implicitly directed viewers to prioritize inspecting the key relationships. Thus, a severe limit on relational processing, combined with a cascade of other capacity-limited operations (e.g., linking values to semantic content), makes understanding a graph more like slowly reading a paragraph then immediately recognizing an image. These results also highlight the practical importance of "data storytelling" techniques, where communicators design graphs that help their audience prioritize the most important relationships in data. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

视觉可为某些任务提供快速处理能力,但也会遇到其他任务的强大限制。虽然许多任务都会遇到同时处理四个视觉对象的能力限制,但一些证据表明,处理对象之间关系的能力限制要低得多。我们处理关系的能力极限是多少?如果确实有限,那么人们可能会错过图表中数据值之间的重要关系。为了测试这个问题,我们让人们探索非常简单的 2 × 2 数据集的图表,结果发现有一半的观察者错过了令人吃惊和不可能的关系(例如,一个孩子的身高随着时间的推移而降低)。而在对照条件下,这些关系很容易就被发现,因为对照条件隐含地引导观众优先检查关键关系。因此,对关系处理的严格限制,再加上一连串其他能力受限的操作(例如,将数值与语义内容联系起来),使得对图表的理解更像是慢慢阅读一段文字,而不是立即识别一幅图像。这些结果也凸显了 "数据讲故事 "技术的实际重要性,即传播者通过设计图表来帮助受众优先考虑数据中最重要的关系。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Reading a graph is like reading a paragraph.","authors":"Tal Boger, Steven Franconeri","doi":"10.1037/xge0001604","DOIUrl":"10.1037/xge0001604","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Vision provides rapid processing for some tasks, but encounters strong constraints from others. Although many tasks encounter a capacity limit of processing four visual objects at once, some evidence suggests far lower limits for processing relationships among objects. What is our capacity limit for relational processing? If it is indeed limited, then people may miss important relationships between data values in a graph. To test this question, we asked people to explore graphs of trivially simple 2 × 2 data sets and found that half of the viewers missed surprising and improbable relationships (e.g., a child's height decreasing over time). These relationships were spotted easily in a control condition, which implicitly directed viewers to prioritize inspecting the key relationships. Thus, a severe limit on relational processing, combined with a cascade of other capacity-limited operations (e.g., linking values to semantic content), makes understanding a graph more like slowly reading a paragraph then immediately recognizing an image. These results also highlight the practical importance of \"data storytelling\" techniques, where communicators design graphs that help their audience prioritize the most important relationships in data. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":15698,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology: General","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.7,"publicationDate":"2024-07-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141081722","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Everyday amnesia: Residual memory for high confidence misses and implications for decision models of recognition. 日常健忘症:高置信度遗漏的残余记忆及其对识别决策模型的影响。
IF 3.7 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-07-01 Epub Date: 2024-04-25 DOI: 10.1037/xge0001599
Christopher J Berry, David R Shanks

Despite studying a list of items only minutes earlier, when reencountered in a recognition memory test, undergraduate participants often say with total confidence that they have not studied some of the items before. Such high confidence miss (HCM) responses have been taken as evidence of rapid and complete forgetting and of everyday amnesia (Roediger & Tekin, 2020). We investigated (a) if memory for HCMs is completely lost or whether a residual memory effect exists and (b) whether dominant decision models predict the effect. Participants studied faces (Experiments 1a, 2, and 3) or words (Experiment 1b), then completed a single-item recognition memory task, followed by either (a) a two-alternative forced-choice recognition task, in which the studied and nonstudied alternatives on each trial were matched for their previous old/new decision and confidence rating (Experiments 1 and 2) or (b) a second single-item recognition task in which the targets and foils were HCMs and high confidence correct rejections, respectively (Experiment 3). In each experiment, participants reliably distinguished HCMs from high-confidence correct rejections. The unequal variance signal detection and dual-process signal detection models were fit to the single-item recognition data, and the parameter estimates were used to predict the memory effect for HCMs. The dual-process signal detection model predicted the residual memory effect (as did another popular model, the mixture signal detection theory model). However, the unequal variance signal detection model incorrectly predicted a negative, or no, effect, invalidating this model. The residual memory effect for HCMs demonstrates that everyday amnesia is not associated with complete memory loss and distinguishes between decision models. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

尽管几分钟前才学习过一个项目列表,但当在识别记忆测试中再次遇到这些项目时,大学生参与者往往会非常自信地说,他们以前没有学习过其中的一些项目。这种高置信度遗漏(HCM)反应被认为是快速完全遗忘和日常遗忘的证据(Roediger 和 Tekin,2020 年)。我们研究了:(a) 对 HCM 的记忆是完全遗失还是存在残余记忆效应;(b) 主导决策模型是否能预测这种效应。参与者先学习面孔(实验 1a、2 和 3)或单词(实验 1b),然后完成单项识别记忆任务,接着完成(a)双项强迫选择识别任务,其中每个试验中的学习和非学习备选方案都与之前的新旧决策和置信度评级相匹配(实验 1 和 2),或者(b)第二个单项识别任务,其中目标和衬托物分别是高密度测量和高置信度正确拒绝(实验 3)。在每个实验中,被试都能可靠地将高置信度正确拒绝项与高置信度正确拒绝项区分开来。对单项识别数据拟合了不等方差信号检测模型和双过程信号检测模型,并利用参数估计来预测 HCM 的记忆效应。双过程信号检测模型预测了残余记忆效应(另一个流行模型--混合信号检测理论模型也预测了残余记忆效应)。然而,不等方差信号检测模型错误地预测了负效应或无效应,从而使该模型失效。HCMs 的残余记忆效应表明,日常健忘症与完全失忆无关,并能区分不同的决策模型。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA,保留所有权利)。
{"title":"Everyday amnesia: Residual memory for high confidence misses and implications for decision models of recognition.","authors":"Christopher J Berry, David R Shanks","doi":"10.1037/xge0001599","DOIUrl":"10.1037/xge0001599","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Despite studying a list of items only minutes earlier, when reencountered in a recognition memory test, undergraduate participants often say with total confidence that they have not studied some of the items before. Such high confidence miss (HCM) responses have been taken as evidence of rapid and complete forgetting and of everyday amnesia (Roediger & Tekin, 2020). We investigated (a) if memory for HCMs is completely lost or whether a residual memory effect exists and (b) whether dominant decision models predict the effect. Participants studied faces (Experiments 1a, 2, and 3) or words (Experiment 1b), then completed a single-item recognition memory task, followed by either (a) a two-alternative forced-choice recognition task, in which the studied and nonstudied alternatives on each trial were matched for their previous old/new decision and confidence rating (Experiments 1 and 2) or (b) a second single-item recognition task in which the targets and foils were HCMs and high confidence correct rejections, respectively (Experiment 3). In each experiment, participants reliably distinguished HCMs from high-confidence correct rejections. The unequal variance signal detection and dual-process signal detection models were fit to the single-item recognition data, and the parameter estimates were used to predict the memory effect for HCMs. The dual-process signal detection model predicted the residual memory effect (as did another popular model, the mixture signal detection theory model). However, the unequal variance signal detection model incorrectly predicted a negative, or no, effect, invalidating this model. The residual memory effect for HCMs demonstrates that everyday amnesia is not associated with complete memory loss and distinguishes between decision models. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":15698,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology: General","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.7,"publicationDate":"2024-07-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140863222","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Ghosting: Social rejection without explanation, but not without care. 鬼混:社交拒绝:不做解释,但并非不关心。
IF 3.7 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-07-01 Epub Date: 2024-05-23 DOI: 10.1037/xge0001590
YeJin Park, Nadav Klein

Many social ties end when one side rejects the other, but rejection does not need to happen directly. Ghosting-the act of ending a relationship by ignoring another person's attempts to connect-is a common way of ending social ties. The present experiments first establish the key characteristics of ghosting and distinguish it from other rejection behaviors (Pilot Studies 1a-1c). The experiments then proceed to explore the relational and motivational implications of this behavior, finding that ghosters (those who ghost) care about the well-being of ghostees (those who are ghosted) more than ghostees realize. This result occurs in recalled instances of ghosting (Experiment 1), when ghosting in real time (Experiment 2), and when refraining from ghosting is monetarily costly (Experiment 3). We find that this occurs partly because ghostees underestimate the other-oriented motives involved in ghosting, misunderstanding that ghosters ghost partly as a way to end a tie while avoiding hurting ghostees' feelings (Experiments 4-6). Indeed, greater other-oriented motives lead to a higher likelihood of ghosting others (Experiment 7). A final experiment finds relational consequences whereby ghostees miss out on opportunities for future help exchange due to their underestimation of the extent to which ghosters care about them (Experiment 8). Ghosting is social rejection without explanation or feedback, but not without care. This study highlights how prosocial motives can drive rejection behaviors and the role of interpersonal accuracy in mitigating the negative effects of social rejection. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

许多社会关系都是在一方拒绝另一方时结束的,但拒绝并不一定要直接发生。幽灵行为--通过无视他人的联系尝试来结束关系的行为--是结束社会关系的一种常见方式。本实验首先确定了幽灵行为的主要特征,并将其与其他拒绝行为区分开来(试验研究 1a-1c)。实验接着探讨了这种行为对关系和动机的影响,发现鬼魂(鬼魂的主人)比鬼魂的主人更关心鬼魂(被鬼魂的主人)的幸福。这一结果出现在回顾性鬼魂行为(实验 1)、实时鬼魂行为(实验 2)以及避免鬼魂行为需要付出金钱代价(实验 3)的情况下。我们发现,出现这种情况的部分原因是被鬼魂低估了鬼魂纠缠中的他者导向动机,误以为鬼魂纠缠者纠缠的部分原因是为了结束双方的关系,同时避免伤害被鬼魂的感情(实验4-6)。事实上,更强的他者导向动机会导致更高的鬼魂缠绕可能性(实验 7)。最后一个实验发现,由于受鬼者低估了鬼魂对他们的关心程度,他们错过了未来交换帮助的机会(实验8)。幽灵行为是没有解释或反馈的社会拒绝,但并非没有关心。这项研究强调了亲社会动机是如何驱动拒绝行为的,以及人际交往的准确性在减轻社交拒绝的负面影响方面所起的作用。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA,保留所有权利)。
{"title":"Ghosting: Social rejection without explanation, but not without care.","authors":"YeJin Park, Nadav Klein","doi":"10.1037/xge0001590","DOIUrl":"10.1037/xge0001590","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Many social ties end when one side rejects the other, but rejection does not need to happen directly. Ghosting-the act of ending a relationship by ignoring another person's attempts to connect-is a common way of ending social ties. The present experiments first establish the key characteristics of ghosting and distinguish it from other rejection behaviors (Pilot Studies 1a-1c). The experiments then proceed to explore the relational and motivational implications of this behavior, finding that ghosters (those who ghost) care about the well-being of ghostees (those who are ghosted) more than ghostees realize. This result occurs in recalled instances of ghosting (Experiment 1), when ghosting in real time (Experiment 2), and when refraining from ghosting is monetarily costly (Experiment 3). We find that this occurs partly because ghostees underestimate the other-oriented motives involved in ghosting, misunderstanding that ghosters ghost partly as a way to end a tie while avoiding hurting ghostees' feelings (Experiments 4-6). Indeed, greater other-oriented motives lead to a higher likelihood of ghosting others (Experiment 7). A final experiment finds relational consequences whereby ghostees miss out on opportunities for future help exchange due to their underestimation of the extent to which ghosters care about them (Experiment 8). Ghosting is social rejection without explanation or feedback, but not without care. This study highlights how prosocial motives can drive rejection behaviors and the role of interpersonal accuracy in mitigating the negative effects of social rejection. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":15698,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology: General","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.7,"publicationDate":"2024-07-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141081721","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Motives matter more with age: Adult age differences in response to sociomoral violations. 年龄越大,动机越重要:成年人对违反社会道德行为反应的年龄差异。
IF 3.7 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-07-01 Epub Date: 2024-05-02 DOI: 10.1037/xge0001578
Alyssa R Minton, Jason S Snyder, Nathaniel A Young, Verena Graupmann, Joseph A Mikels

Moral judgments and emotional reactions to sociomoral violations are heavily impacted by a perpetrator's intentions and desires, which pose a threat to social harmony. Given that older adults are more motivated to maintain interpersonal harmony relative to younger adults, older adults may be more reactive to malicious desires. In three studies, we investigated adult age differences in moral judgments and emotional reactions to sociomoral violations. In all studies, participants read scenarios in which a perpetrator either (a) desired to harm another but nothing happened, or (b) harmed another accidentally without malicious desire. Study 2 incorporated additional scenarios designed to evoke anger and disgust without explicitly implicating another person to evaluate whether age differences emerge only when sociomoral violations against another are salient. In Study 3, we examined the combined effects of malicious desires and harmful outcomes by including scenarios in which (a) harmful desires were coupled with harmful outcomes, and (b) benign desires were coupled with benign outcomes. Predominantly across the studies, older adults judged perpetrators who desired to harm another more harshly but judged perpetrators who accidentally harmed another more leniently than younger adults. Emotional reactions generally corresponded with the differences in judgments. Taken together, this work suggests that desires more strongly impact older relative to younger adults' judgments and emotional reactions in sociomoral contexts. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

道德判断和对违反社会道德行为的情绪反应在很大程度上受到犯罪者意图和欲望的影响,而犯罪者的意图和欲望对社会和谐构成威胁。与年轻人相比,老年人更愿意维护人际和谐,因此老年人对恶意欲望的反应可能会更强烈。在三项研究中,我们调查了成年人在道德判断和对违反社会道德行为的情绪反应方面的年龄差异。在所有研究中,参与者阅读的情景都是犯罪者(a)想要伤害他人但什么都没发生,或者(b)在没有恶意的情况下意外伤害了他人。研究 2 加入了额外的情景,旨在唤起愤怒和厌恶,但不明确涉及他人,以评估是否只有当社会道德侵犯他人时才会出现年龄差异。在研究 3 中,我们考察了恶意欲望和有害结果的综合影响,包括以下情景:(a) 有害欲望与有害结果相结合;(b) 善意欲望与善意结果相结合。在所有研究中,老年人对想要伤害他人的肇事者的判断主要比年轻人严厉,但对意外伤害他人的肇事者的判断则比年轻人宽松。情绪反应通常与判断的差异相对应。综上所述,这项研究表明,在社会道德情境中,欲望对老年人的判断和情绪反应的影响比对年轻人的影响更大。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Motives matter more with age: Adult age differences in response to sociomoral violations.","authors":"Alyssa R Minton, Jason S Snyder, Nathaniel A Young, Verena Graupmann, Joseph A Mikels","doi":"10.1037/xge0001578","DOIUrl":"10.1037/xge0001578","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Moral judgments and emotional reactions to sociomoral violations are heavily impacted by a perpetrator's intentions and desires, which pose a threat to social harmony. Given that older adults are more motivated to maintain interpersonal harmony relative to younger adults, older adults may be more reactive to malicious desires. In three studies, we investigated adult age differences in moral judgments and emotional reactions to sociomoral violations. In all studies, participants read scenarios in which a perpetrator either (a) desired to harm another but nothing happened, or (b) harmed another accidentally without malicious desire. Study 2 incorporated additional scenarios designed to evoke anger and disgust without explicitly implicating another person to evaluate whether age differences emerge only when sociomoral violations against another are salient. In Study 3, we examined the combined effects of malicious desires and harmful outcomes by including scenarios in which (a) harmful desires were coupled with harmful outcomes, and (b) benign desires were coupled with benign outcomes. Predominantly across the studies, older adults judged perpetrators who desired to harm another more harshly but judged perpetrators who accidentally harmed another more leniently than younger adults. Emotional reactions generally corresponded with the differences in judgments. Taken together, this work suggests that desires more strongly impact older relative to younger adults' judgments and emotional reactions in sociomoral contexts. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":15698,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology: General","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.7,"publicationDate":"2024-07-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11250574/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140859887","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Exploring variability in risk taking with large language models. 利用大型语言模型探索风险承担的可变性。
IF 3.7 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-07-01 Epub Date: 2024-05-02 DOI: 10.1037/xge0001607
Sudeep Bhatia

What are the sources of individual-level differences in risk taking, and how do they depend on the domain or situation in which the decision is being made? Psychologists currently answer such questions with psychometric methods, which analyze correlations across participant responses in survey data sets. In this article, we analyze the preferences that give rise to these correlations. Our approach uses (a) large language models (LLMs) to quantify everyday risky behaviors in terms of the attributes or reasons that may describe those behaviors, and (b) decision models to map these attributes and reasons onto participant responses. We show that LLM-based decision models can explain observed correlations between behaviors in terms of the reasons different behaviors elicit and explain observed correlations between individuals in terms of the weights different individuals place on reasons, thereby providing a decision theoretic foundation for psychometric findings. Since LLMs can generate quantitative representations for nearly any naturalistic decision, they can be used to make accurate out-of-sample predictions for hundreds of everyday behaviors, predict the reasons why people may or may not want to engage in these behaviors, and interpret these reasons in terms of core psychological constructs. Our approach has important theoretical and practical implications for the study of heterogeneity in everyday behavior. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

风险承担的个体差异来源是什么,它们如何取决于做出决定的领域或情况?心理学家目前使用心理测量方法来回答这些问题,这种方法分析调查数据集中参与者回答之间的相关性。在本文中,我们将分析产生这些相关性的偏好。我们的方法使用:(a)大语言模型(LLM),根据可能描述这些行为的属性或原因量化日常风险行为;(b)决策模型,将这些属性和原因映射到参与者的回答中。我们的研究表明,基于 LLM 的决策模型可以根据不同行为引起的原因来解释观察到的行为之间的相关性,并根据不同个体对原因的重视程度来解释观察到的个体之间的相关性,从而为心理测量结果提供决策理论基础。由于 LLM 几乎可以为任何自然决策生成定量表征,因此它们可以用于对数百种日常行为进行精确的样本外预测,预测人们可能想要或可能不想要参与这些行为的原因,并根据核心心理结构解释这些原因。我们的方法对日常行为的异质性研究具有重要的理论和实践意义。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Exploring variability in risk taking with large language models.","authors":"Sudeep Bhatia","doi":"10.1037/xge0001607","DOIUrl":"10.1037/xge0001607","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>What are the sources of individual-level differences in risk taking, and how do they depend on the domain or situation in which the decision is being made? Psychologists currently answer such questions with psychometric methods, which analyze correlations across participant responses in survey data sets. In this article, we analyze the preferences that give rise to these correlations. Our approach uses (a) large language models (LLMs) to quantify everyday risky behaviors in terms of the attributes or reasons that may describe those behaviors, and (b) decision models to map these attributes and reasons onto participant responses. We show that LLM-based decision models can explain observed correlations between behaviors in terms of the reasons different behaviors elicit and explain observed correlations between individuals in terms of the weights different individuals place on reasons, thereby providing a decision theoretic foundation for psychometric findings. Since LLMs can generate quantitative representations for nearly any naturalistic decision, they can be used to make accurate out-of-sample predictions for hundreds of everyday behaviors, predict the reasons why people may or may not want to engage in these behaviors, and interpret these reasons in terms of core psychological constructs. Our approach has important theoretical and practical implications for the study of heterogeneity in everyday behavior. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":15698,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology: General","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.7,"publicationDate":"2024-07-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140865144","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The exaggerated benefits of failure. 夸大失败的好处。
IF 3.7 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-07-01 Epub Date: 2024-06-10 DOI: 10.1037/xge0001610
Lauren Eskreis-Winkler, Kaitlin Woolley, Eda Erensoy, Minhee Kim

Commencement speakers, business leaders, and the popular press tell us that failure has at least one benefit: It fuels success. Does it? Across 11 studies, including a field study of medical professionals, predictors overestimated the rate at which people course correct following failure (Studies 1-4). Predictors overestimated the likelihood that professionals who fail a professional exam (e.g., the bar exam, the medical boards) pass a retest (Studies 1a, 1b, and 2a), the likelihood that patients improve their health after a crisis (e.g., heart attack, drug overdose; Studies 2b and 6), and the probability, more generally, of learning from one's mistakes (Studies 3-5). This effect was specific to overestimating success following failure (Study 4) and erasing mention of an initial failure that had actually occurred corrected the problem (Studies 2a and 2b). The success overestimate was due, at least in part, to the belief that people attend to failure more than they do (Studies 5 and 6). Correcting this overestimate had policy implications. Citizens apprised of the sobering true rate of postfailure success increased their support for rehabilitative initiatives aimed at helping struggling populations (e.g., people with addiction, ex-convicts) learn from past mistakes (Studies 7a-7c). (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

毕业典礼上的演讲者、商界领袖和大众媒体都告诉我们,失败至少有一个好处:它能促进成功。是这样吗?在 11 项研究(包括一项对医学专业人员的实地研究)中,预测者高估了人们在失败后的正确率(研究 1-4)。预测者高估了专业考试(如律师资格考试、医学委员会考试)失败的专业人员通过复试的可能性(研究 1a、1b 和 2a)、病人在危机(如心脏病发作、药物过量;研究 2b 和 6)后改善健康状况的可能性,以及从错误中吸取教训的可能性(研究 3-5)。这种效应具体表现为高估失败后的成功(研究 4),以及抹去最初失败的提及,而实际上失败已经纠正了问题(研究 2a 和 2b)。高估成功的原因至少有一部分是由于人们认为失败比实际情况更重要(研究 5 和 6)。纠正这种高估具有政策意义。当公民了解到失败后成功的真实比率时,他们会更加支持旨在帮助陷入困境的人群(如吸毒成瘾者、前罪犯)从过去的错误中吸取教训的康复计划(研究 7a-7c)。(PsycInfo 数据库记录 (c) 2024 APA,版权所有。)
{"title":"The exaggerated benefits of failure.","authors":"Lauren Eskreis-Winkler, Kaitlin Woolley, Eda Erensoy, Minhee Kim","doi":"10.1037/xge0001610","DOIUrl":"10.1037/xge0001610","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Commencement speakers, business leaders, and the popular press tell us that failure has at least one benefit: It fuels success. Does it? Across 11 studies, including a field study of medical professionals, predictors overestimated the rate at which people course correct following failure (Studies 1-4). Predictors overestimated the likelihood that professionals who fail a professional exam (e.g., the bar exam, the medical boards) pass a retest (Studies 1a, 1b, and 2a), the likelihood that patients improve their health after a crisis (e.g., heart attack, drug overdose; Studies 2b and 6), and the probability, more generally, of learning from one's mistakes (Studies 3-5). This effect was specific to overestimating success following failure (Study 4) and erasing mention of an initial failure that had actually occurred corrected the problem (Studies 2a and 2b). The success overestimate was due, at least in part, to the belief that people attend to failure more than they do (Studies 5 and 6). Correcting this overestimate had policy implications. Citizens apprised of the sobering true rate of postfailure success increased their support for rehabilitative initiatives aimed at helping struggling populations (e.g., people with addiction, ex-convicts) learn from past mistakes (Studies 7a-7c). (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":15698,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology: General","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.7,"publicationDate":"2024-07-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141296181","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The magnitude of the testing effect is independent of retrieval practice performance. 测试效果的大小与检索练习的成绩无关。
IF 3.7 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-07-01 Epub Date: 2024-05-02 DOI: 10.1037/xge0001593
Jason C K Chan, Sara D Davis, Aslı Yurtsever, Sarah J Myers

Practicing retrieval is a potent learning enhancer. Theoretical accounts of the testing effect generally suggest that the magnitude of the testing effect is dependent on retrieval practice performance, such that conditions that promote better retrieval practice performance should result in a greater testing effect. Empirical evidence, however, has been mixed. Although some studies showed a positive association between retrieval practice performance and the testing effect, others have shown either no relation or the reverse. In the present study, we experimentally manipulated retrieval practice performance using a retrieval-based response deadline manipulation and an encoding-based study trial manipulation. Across six experiments, the magnitude of the testing effect was independent of retrieval practice performance. However, when we aggregated the data across the experiments, participants with superior retrieval practice performance showed a greater testing effect-an individual difference. This dissociation between experimental and correlational outcomes suggests that the positive relation between retrieval practice performance and the testing effect is not causal, and indeed, simulation data showed that the correlation between retrieval practice performance and testing effect was an artifact. We discuss the challenges these findings present to existing accounts of the testing effect. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

检索练习是一种有效的学习促进手段。关于测试效应的理论解释一般认为,测试效应的大小取决于检索练习的表现,因此,促进检索练习表现更好的条件应导致更大的测试效应。然而,经验证据却喜忧参半。虽然有些研究显示检索练习成绩与测试效果之间存在正相关,但其他研究则显示两者之间没有关系或关系相反。在本研究中,我们利用基于检索的反应截止时间操纵和基于编码的学习试验操纵,对检索练习成绩进行了实验操纵。在六次实验中,测试效应的大小与检索练习成绩无关。然而,当我们汇总各实验数据时,检索练习成绩优秀的参与者表现出了更大的测试效应--个体差异。实验结果和相关结果之间的这种分离表明,检索练习成绩和测试效果之间的正相关关系并不是因果关系,事实上,模拟数据表明,检索练习成绩和测试效果之间的相关关系是一种伪现象。我们将讨论这些发现给现有的测试效应理论带来的挑战。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved)。
{"title":"The magnitude of the testing effect is independent of retrieval practice performance.","authors":"Jason C K Chan, Sara D Davis, Aslı Yurtsever, Sarah J Myers","doi":"10.1037/xge0001593","DOIUrl":"10.1037/xge0001593","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Practicing retrieval is a potent learning enhancer. Theoretical accounts of the testing effect generally suggest that the magnitude of the testing effect is dependent on retrieval practice performance, such that conditions that promote better retrieval practice performance should result in a greater testing effect. Empirical evidence, however, has been mixed. Although some studies showed a positive association between retrieval practice performance and the testing effect, others have shown either no relation or the reverse. In the present study, we experimentally manipulated retrieval practice performance using a retrieval-based response deadline manipulation and an encoding-based study trial manipulation. Across six experiments, the magnitude of the testing effect was independent of retrieval practice performance. However, when we aggregated the data across the experiments, participants with superior retrieval practice performance showed a greater testing effect-an individual difference. This dissociation between experimental and correlational outcomes suggests that the positive relation between retrieval practice performance and the testing effect is not causal, and indeed, simulation data showed that the correlation between retrieval practice performance and testing effect was an artifact. We discuss the challenges these findings present to existing accounts of the testing effect. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":15698,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology: General","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.7,"publicationDate":"2024-07-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140848918","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Task switch costs scale with dissimilarity between task rules. 任务切换成本随任务规则之间的差异而增加。
IF 3.7 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-07-01 Epub Date: 2024-05-02 DOI: 10.1037/xge0001598
Bettina Bustos, J Toby Mordkoff, Eliot Hazeltine, Jiefeng Jiang

Cognitive flexibility enables humans to voluntarily switch tasks. Task switching requires replacing the previously active task representation with a new one, an operation that typically results in a switch cost. Thus, understanding cognitive flexibility requires understanding how tasks are represented in the brain. We hypothesize that task representations are cognitive map-like, such that the magnitude of the difference between task representations reflects their conceptual differences: The greater the distinction between the two task representations, the more updating is required. This hypothesis predicts that switch costs should increase with between task dissimilarity. To test this hypothesis, we use an experimental design that parametrically manipulates the similarity between task rules. We observe that response time scales with the dissimilarity between the task rules. The findings shed light on the organizational principles of task representations and extend the conventional binary task-switch effect (task repeat vs. switch) to a theoretical framework with parametric task switches. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

认知灵活性使人类能够自愿切换任务。任务切换需要用一个新的任务表征替换之前的活动任务表征,这一操作通常会产生切换成本。因此,要理解认知灵活性,就必须了解大脑是如何表征任务的。我们假设任务表征类似于认知地图,因此任务表征之间的差异大小反映了它们在概念上的不同:两个任务表征之间的差异越大,所需的更新就越多。根据这一假设,切换成本会随着任务之间差异的增加而增加。为了验证这一假设,我们采用了一种实验设计,从参数上操纵任务规则之间的相似性。我们观察到,响应时间与任务规则之间的不相似性成比例关系。这些发现揭示了任务表征的组织原理,并将传统的二元任务切换效应(任务重复与切换)扩展到了参数任务切换的理论框架中。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Task switch costs scale with dissimilarity between task rules.","authors":"Bettina Bustos, J Toby Mordkoff, Eliot Hazeltine, Jiefeng Jiang","doi":"10.1037/xge0001598","DOIUrl":"10.1037/xge0001598","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Cognitive flexibility enables humans to voluntarily switch tasks. Task switching requires replacing the previously active task representation with a new one, an operation that typically results in a switch cost. Thus, understanding cognitive flexibility requires understanding how tasks are represented in the brain. We hypothesize that task representations are cognitive map-like, such that the magnitude of the difference between task representations reflects their conceptual differences: The greater the distinction between the two task representations, the more updating is required. This hypothesis predicts that switch costs should increase with between task dissimilarity. To test this hypothesis, we use an experimental design that parametrically manipulates the similarity between task rules. We observe that response time scales with the dissimilarity between the task rules. The findings shed light on the organizational principles of task representations and extend the conventional binary task-switch effect (task repeat vs. switch) to a theoretical framework with parametric task switches. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":15698,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology: General","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.7,"publicationDate":"2024-07-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11250929/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140851947","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Communicative efficiency in multimodal language directed at children and adults. 针对儿童和成人的多模态语言的交流效率。
IF 3.7 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-07-01 Epub Date: 2024-06-06 DOI: 10.1037/xge0001588
Beata Grzyb, Stefan L Frank, Gabriella Vigliocco

The ecology of human communication is face to face. In these contexts, speakers dynamically modify their communication across vocal (e.g., speaking rate) and gestural (e.g., cospeech gestures related in meaning to the content of speech) channels while speaking. What is the function of these adjustments? Here we ask whether speakers dynamically make these adjustments to increase communicative success, and decrease cognitive effort while speaking. We assess whether speakers modulate word durations and produce iconic (i.e., imagistically evoking properties of referents) gestures depending on the predictability of each word they utter. Predictability is operationalized as surprisal and computed from computational language models trained on corpora of child-directed, or adult-directed language. Using data from a novel corpus (Ecological Language Corpus) of naturalistic interactions between adult-child (aged 3-4), and adult-adult, we show that surprisal predicts speakers' multimodal adjustments and that some of these effects are modulated by whether the comprehender is a child or an adult. Thus, communicative efficiency applies generally across vocal and gestural communicative channels not being limited to structural properties of language or vocal modality. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

人类交流的生态环境是面对面的。在这种情况下,说话者在说话时会动态地调整他们在声音(如语速)和手势(如与说话内容相关的共同言语手势)方面的交流。这些调整的作用是什么?在这里,我们要问的是,说话者是否会在说话时动态地进行这些调整,以提高交际成功率并减少认知努力。我们将评估说话者是否会根据他们说出的每个单词的可预测性来调整单词的持续时间并做出标志性(即通过想象唤起所指事物的属性)手势。可预测性被操作化为惊奇,并通过在儿童或成人语言语料库中训练的计算语言模型进行计算。通过使用来自成人-儿童(3-4 岁)和成人-成人之间自然互动的新型语料库(生态语言语料库)的数据,我们发现,惊奇可以预测说话者的多模态调整,而且其中一些效果会受到理解者是儿童还是成人的影响。因此,交际效率普遍适用于发声和手势交际渠道,而不局限于语言或发声方式的结构属性。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Communicative efficiency in multimodal language directed at children and adults.","authors":"Beata Grzyb, Stefan L Frank, Gabriella Vigliocco","doi":"10.1037/xge0001588","DOIUrl":"10.1037/xge0001588","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The ecology of human communication is face to face. In these contexts, speakers dynamically modify their communication across vocal (e.g., speaking rate) and gestural (e.g., cospeech gestures related in meaning to the content of speech) channels while speaking. What is the function of these adjustments? Here we ask whether speakers dynamically make these adjustments to increase communicative success, and decrease cognitive effort while speaking. We assess whether speakers modulate word durations and produce iconic (i.e., imagistically evoking properties of referents) gestures depending on the predictability of each word they utter. Predictability is operationalized as surprisal and computed from computational language models trained on corpora of child-directed, or adult-directed language. Using data from a novel corpus (Ecological Language Corpus) of naturalistic interactions between adult-child (aged 3-4), and adult-adult, we show that surprisal predicts speakers' multimodal adjustments and that some of these effects are modulated by whether the comprehender is a child or an adult. Thus, communicative efficiency applies generally across vocal and gestural communicative channels not being limited to structural properties of language or vocal modality. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":15698,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology: General","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.7,"publicationDate":"2024-07-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141261719","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Aging impairs reactive attentional control but not proactive distractor inhibition. 衰老会损害反应注意控制,但不会损害主动分心抑制。
IF 3.7 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-07-01 Epub Date: 2024-05-23 DOI: 10.1037/xge0001602
Andy Jeesu Kim, Joshua Senior, Sonali Chu, Mara Mather

Older adults tend to be more prone to distraction compared with young adults, and this age-related deficit has been attributed to a deficiency in inhibitory processing. However, recent findings challenge the notion that aging leads to global impairments in inhibition. To reconcile these mixed findings, we investigated how aging modulates multiple mechanisms of attentional control by tracking the timing and direction of eye movements. When engaged in feature-search mode and proactive distractor suppression, older adults made fewer first fixations to the target but inhibited the task-irrelevant salient distractor as effectively as did young adults. However, when engaged in singleton-search mode and required to reactively disengage from the distractor, older adults made significantly more first saccades toward the task-irrelevant salient distractor and showed increased fixation times in orienting to the target, longer dwell times on incorrect saccades, and increased saccadic reaction times compared with young adults. Our findings reveal that aging differently impairs attentional control depending on whether visual search requires proactive distractor suppression or reactive distractor disengagement. Furthermore, our oculomotor measures reveal both age-related deficits and age equivalence in various mechanisms of attention, including goal-directed orienting, selection history, disengagement, and distractor inhibition. These findings help explain why conclusions of age-related declines or age equivalence in mechanisms of attentional control are task specific and reveal that older adults do not exhibit global impairments in mechanisms of inhibition. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

与年轻人相比,老年人往往更容易分心,这种与年龄有关的缺陷被归因于抑制处理能力的不足。然而,最近的研究结果对衰老导致抑制能力全面受损的观点提出了质疑。为了调和这些混杂的研究结果,我们通过追踪眼球运动的时间和方向,研究了衰老如何调节注意力控制的多种机制。当处于特征搜索模式并主动抑制分心物时,老年人对目标的首次固定次数较少,但对与任务无关的突出分心物的抑制却与年轻人一样有效。然而,与年轻人相比,当采用单体搜索模式并要求老年人反应性地脱离分心物时,老年人对与任务无关的突出分心物的首次注视次数明显增多,而且在定向目标时的凝视时间增加,错误注视的停留时间延长,注视反应时间增加。我们的研究结果表明,根据视觉搜索需要主动抑制分心物还是被动脱离分心物,衰老会对注意力控制造成不同程度的损害。此外,我们的眼球运动测量结果显示,在目标定向、选择历程、脱离和分心抑制等各种注意机制方面,既存在与年龄相关的缺陷,也存在年龄等同性。这些发现有助于解释为什么注意力控制机制中与年龄相关的下降或年龄相等的结论是针对特定任务的,并揭示了老年人在抑制机制中并没有表现出全面的障碍。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Aging impairs reactive attentional control but not proactive distractor inhibition.","authors":"Andy Jeesu Kim, Joshua Senior, Sonali Chu, Mara Mather","doi":"10.1037/xge0001602","DOIUrl":"10.1037/xge0001602","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Older adults tend to be more prone to distraction compared with young adults, and this age-related deficit has been attributed to a deficiency in inhibitory processing. However, recent findings challenge the notion that aging leads to global impairments in inhibition. To reconcile these mixed findings, we investigated how aging modulates multiple mechanisms of attentional control by tracking the timing and direction of eye movements. When engaged in feature-search mode and proactive distractor suppression, older adults made fewer first fixations to the target but inhibited the task-irrelevant salient distractor as effectively as did young adults. However, when engaged in singleton-search mode and required to reactively disengage from the distractor, older adults made significantly more first saccades toward the task-irrelevant salient distractor and showed increased fixation times in orienting to the target, longer dwell times on incorrect saccades, and increased saccadic reaction times compared with young adults. Our findings reveal that aging differently impairs attentional control depending on whether visual search requires proactive distractor suppression or reactive distractor disengagement. Furthermore, our oculomotor measures reveal both age-related deficits and age equivalence in various mechanisms of attention, including goal-directed orienting, selection history, disengagement, and distractor inhibition. These findings help explain why conclusions of age-related declines or age equivalence in mechanisms of attentional control are task specific and reveal that older adults do not exhibit global impairments in mechanisms of inhibition. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":15698,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology: General","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.7,"publicationDate":"2024-07-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11250690/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141081685","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Journal of Experimental Psychology: General
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1