首页 > 最新文献

Journal of Experimental Psychology: General最新文献

英文 中文
Loyalty from a personal point of view: A cross-cultural prototype study of loyalty. 从个人角度看忠诚度:忠诚度的跨文化原型研究。
IF 4.1 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-10-21 DOI: 10.1037/xge0001623
Samuel Murray,Gino Marttelo Carmona Díaz,Laura Sofía Vega-Plazas,William Jiménez-Leal,Santiago Amaya
Loyalty is considered central to people's moral life, yet little is known about how people think about what it means to be loyal. We used a prototype approach to understand how loyalty is represented in Colombia and the United States and how these representations mediate attributions of loyalty and moral judgments of loyalty violations. Across seven studies (N = 1,984), we found cross-cultural similarities in the associative meaning of loyalty (Study 1) but found differences in the centrality of features associated with loyalty (Study 2) and the latent structure of loyalty representations (Study 3). Colombians represent loyalty in terms of more general moral characteristics, while U.S. participants represent loyalty in terms of interpersonal commitment, both in contrast with current approaches to loyalty. By comparing representations of loyalty and honesty, we establish that differences in loyalty conceptualizations reflect a different way of thinking about loyalty rather than morality more generally (Study 4). Further, Colombians attributed greater loyalty to individuals with general moral characteristics compared to participants from the U.S. sample (Study 5) and were more likely to classify nonloyal values as loyalty-related (Study 6). While the centrality of prototypical features predicts categorizing norm violations as loyalty-related, differences in prototypical structure account for differences in the severity of moral judgment for such violations (Study 7), which suggests that loyalty representations have similar functions, even though these representations differ in structural characteristics. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).
忠诚被认为是人们道德生活的核心,但人们如何看待忠诚的含义却鲜为人知。我们采用原型法来了解哥伦比亚和美国是如何表述忠诚的,以及这些表述是如何对忠诚的归因和对违反忠诚的道德判断起中介作用的。通过七项研究(N = 1,984),我们发现忠诚的联想意义具有跨文化相似性(研究 1),但在忠诚相关特征的中心性(研究 2)和忠诚表征的潜在结构(研究 3)方面存在差异。哥伦比亚人用更普遍的道德特征来表征忠诚,而美国受试者则用人际承诺来表征忠诚,这两种表征方式都与当前的忠诚表征方式不同。通过比较对忠诚和诚实的表述,我们发现忠诚概念的不同反映了对忠诚的不同思考方式,而不是一般意义上的道德观(研究 4)。此外,与美国样本的参与者相比,哥伦比亚人对具有一般道德特征的个人的忠诚度更高(研究 5),并且更有可能将非忠诚价值观归类为与忠诚相关的价值观(研究 6)。虽然原型特征的中心性预示着将违反规范的行为归类为与忠诚相关,但原型结构的差异却解释了对此类违反规范行为的道德判断严重程度的差异(研究 7),这表明忠诚表征具有相似的功能,即使这些表征在结构特征上存在差异。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Loyalty from a personal point of view: A cross-cultural prototype study of loyalty.","authors":"Samuel Murray,Gino Marttelo Carmona Díaz,Laura Sofía Vega-Plazas,William Jiménez-Leal,Santiago Amaya","doi":"10.1037/xge0001623","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/xge0001623","url":null,"abstract":"Loyalty is considered central to people's moral life, yet little is known about how people think about what it means to be loyal. We used a prototype approach to understand how loyalty is represented in Colombia and the United States and how these representations mediate attributions of loyalty and moral judgments of loyalty violations. Across seven studies (N = 1,984), we found cross-cultural similarities in the associative meaning of loyalty (Study 1) but found differences in the centrality of features associated with loyalty (Study 2) and the latent structure of loyalty representations (Study 3). Colombians represent loyalty in terms of more general moral characteristics, while U.S. participants represent loyalty in terms of interpersonal commitment, both in contrast with current approaches to loyalty. By comparing representations of loyalty and honesty, we establish that differences in loyalty conceptualizations reflect a different way of thinking about loyalty rather than morality more generally (Study 4). Further, Colombians attributed greater loyalty to individuals with general moral characteristics compared to participants from the U.S. sample (Study 5) and were more likely to classify nonloyal values as loyalty-related (Study 6). While the centrality of prototypical features predicts categorizing norm violations as loyalty-related, differences in prototypical structure account for differences in the severity of moral judgment for such violations (Study 7), which suggests that loyalty representations have similar functions, even though these representations differ in structural characteristics. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).","PeriodicalId":15698,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology: General","volume":"8 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":4.1,"publicationDate":"2024-10-21","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142486376","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Simulation requires activation of self-knowledge to change self-concept. 模拟需要激活自我认知来改变自我概念。
IF 4.1 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-10-17 DOI: 10.1037/xge0001663
M J Schneider,Jordan Rubin-McGregor,Jacob Elder,Brent L Hughes,Diana I Tamir
Simulating other people can shift one's self-concept, an effect known as simulation-induced malleability. How does imagining others shift the self? We propose that the activation of self-knowledge is the key factor by which simulation of others alters one's self-concept. We test this possibility across four studies that each manipulate self-knowledge activation indirectly during simulation and measure the impact on subsequent self-ratings. Results demonstrate that increasing activation of self-knowledge during simulation is associated with increased self-concept change. People experienced greater self-concept change when simulating similar others (Studies 1 and 2). People also generalized simulation-induced changes to aspects of the self-concept that were semantically similar to the simulated content (Study 3). Finally, people who are less likely to recruit self-knowledge (i.e., older adults) during simulation were less susceptible to self-concept change (Study 4). These studies highlight self-knowledge activation as an essential component of the effects of simulation on self-rated change. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).
模拟他人可以改变一个人的自我概念,这种效应被称为模拟诱导的可塑性。想象他人如何改变自我?我们认为,自我认知的激活是模拟他人改变自我概念的关键因素。我们在四项研究中测试了这种可能性,每项研究都在模拟过程中间接操纵了自知之明的激活,并测量了其对后续自我评价的影响。结果表明,在模拟过程中增加自我知识的激活与自我概念变化的增加有关。人们在模拟相似的他人时,自我概念会发生更大的变化(研究 1 和 2)。人们还将模拟引起的变化泛化到与模拟内容语义相似的自我概念方面(研究 3)。最后,在模拟过程中较少运用自我知识的人(即老年人)较不易受自我概念变化的影响(研究 4)。这些研究强调,自我认知激活是模拟对自我评价变化影响的重要组成部分。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Simulation requires activation of self-knowledge to change self-concept.","authors":"M J Schneider,Jordan Rubin-McGregor,Jacob Elder,Brent L Hughes,Diana I Tamir","doi":"10.1037/xge0001663","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/xge0001663","url":null,"abstract":"Simulating other people can shift one's self-concept, an effect known as simulation-induced malleability. How does imagining others shift the self? We propose that the activation of self-knowledge is the key factor by which simulation of others alters one's self-concept. We test this possibility across four studies that each manipulate self-knowledge activation indirectly during simulation and measure the impact on subsequent self-ratings. Results demonstrate that increasing activation of self-knowledge during simulation is associated with increased self-concept change. People experienced greater self-concept change when simulating similar others (Studies 1 and 2). People also generalized simulation-induced changes to aspects of the self-concept that were semantically similar to the simulated content (Study 3). Finally, people who are less likely to recruit self-knowledge (i.e., older adults) during simulation were less susceptible to self-concept change (Study 4). These studies highlight self-knowledge activation as an essential component of the effects of simulation on self-rated change. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).","PeriodicalId":15698,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology: General","volume":"3 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":4.1,"publicationDate":"2024-10-17","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142449331","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Moral judgment is sensitive to bargaining power. 道德判断对讨价还价的能力很敏感。
IF 4.1 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-10-17 DOI: 10.1037/xge0001678
Arthur Le Pargneux,Fiery Cushman
For contractualist accounts of morality, actions are moral if they correspond to what rational or reasonable agents would agree to do, were they to negotiate explicitly. This, in turn, often depends on each party's bargaining power, which varies with each party's stakes in the potential agreement and available alternatives in case of disagreement. If there is an asymmetry, with one party enjoying higher bargaining power than another, this party can usually get a better deal, as often happens in real negotiations. A strong test of contractualist accounts of morality, then, is whether moral judgments do take bargaining power into account. We explore this in five preregistered experiments (n = 3,025; U.S.-based Prolific participants). We construct scenarios depicting everyday social interactions between two parties in which one of them can perform a mutually beneficial but unpleasant action. We find that the same actions (asking the other to perform the unpleasant action or explicitly refusing to do it) are perceived as less morally appropriate when performed by the party with lower bargaining power, as compared to the party with higher bargaining power. In other words, participants tend to give more moral leeway to parties with better bargaining positions and to hold disadvantaged parties to stricter moral standards. This effect appears to depend only on the relative bargaining power of each party but not on the magnitude of the bargaining power asymmetry between them. We discuss implications for contractualist theories of moral cognition and the emergence and persistence of unfair norms and inequality. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).
对于契约主义的道德观而言,如果行为符合理性或合理的行为人在明确协商的情况下会同意做的事情,那么这些行为就是道德的。而这又往往取决于各方的讨价还价能力,这种能力随各方在潜在协议中的利害关系以及在出现分歧时的其他选择而变化。如果存在不对称,一方的讨价还价能力高于另一方,那么这一方通常能获得更好的协议,这在实际谈判中经常发生。因此,对契约主义道德观的一个有力检验就是道德判断是否考虑了讨价还价的能力。我们在五个预先登记的实验(n = 3,025; 美国 Prolific 参与者)中探讨了这一问题。我们构建了一些情景,描述了双方之间的日常社会互动,其中一方可以采取对双方都有利但却令人不快的行动。我们发现,与讨价还价能力较强的一方相比,讨价还价能力较弱的一方采取同样的行动(要求对方采取不愉快的行动或明确拒绝采取该行动)时,其道德感较弱。换句话说,参与者倾向于给谈判地位较高的一方更多的道德余地,而对处于劣势的一方则要求更严格的道德标准。这种效应似乎只取决于各方的相对议价能力,而不取决于双方议价能力不对称的程度。我们讨论了道德认知的契约主义理论以及不公平规范和不平等的出现和持续存在的影响。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Moral judgment is sensitive to bargaining power.","authors":"Arthur Le Pargneux,Fiery Cushman","doi":"10.1037/xge0001678","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/xge0001678","url":null,"abstract":"For contractualist accounts of morality, actions are moral if they correspond to what rational or reasonable agents would agree to do, were they to negotiate explicitly. This, in turn, often depends on each party's bargaining power, which varies with each party's stakes in the potential agreement and available alternatives in case of disagreement. If there is an asymmetry, with one party enjoying higher bargaining power than another, this party can usually get a better deal, as often happens in real negotiations. A strong test of contractualist accounts of morality, then, is whether moral judgments do take bargaining power into account. We explore this in five preregistered experiments (n = 3,025; U.S.-based Prolific participants). We construct scenarios depicting everyday social interactions between two parties in which one of them can perform a mutually beneficial but unpleasant action. We find that the same actions (asking the other to perform the unpleasant action or explicitly refusing to do it) are perceived as less morally appropriate when performed by the party with lower bargaining power, as compared to the party with higher bargaining power. In other words, participants tend to give more moral leeway to parties with better bargaining positions and to hold disadvantaged parties to stricter moral standards. This effect appears to depend only on the relative bargaining power of each party but not on the magnitude of the bargaining power asymmetry between them. We discuss implications for contractualist theories of moral cognition and the emergence and persistence of unfair norms and inequality. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).","PeriodicalId":15698,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology: General","volume":"108 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":4.1,"publicationDate":"2024-10-17","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142449330","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Psychological underpinnings of partisan bias in tie formation on social media. 社交媒体上党派偏见形成纽带的心理基础。
IF 4.1 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-10-14 DOI: 10.1037/xge0001662
Mohsen Mosleh,Cameron Martel,David G Rand
Individuals preferentially reciprocate connections with copartisans versus counter-partisans online. However, the mechanisms underlying this partisan bias remain unclear. Do individuals simply prefer viewing politically congenial content, or do they additionally prefer socially connecting with copartisans? Is this driven by preference for in-party ties or distaste for out-party ties? In a Twitter (now called X) field experiment, we created bot accounts varying by partisanship and whether they identified as bots or humans. We randomly assigned Twitter users (N = 3,013) to be followed by one of these accounts. We found evidence for social motivation-users were much more likely to reciprocate links to copartisan relative to counter-partisan accounts when the accounts identified as humans versus bots. We also found evidence for both in-party preference and out-party dispreference-users were as likely to follow back copartisan accounts as they were unlikely to followback counter-partisan accounts, compared to politically neutral accounts. A follow-up survey experiment (N = 990) provides further evidence for distinct roles of issue polarization, out-party animosity, and in-party affinity in moderating follow-back decisions online. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).
在网络上,个人更倾向于与同党建立互惠联系,而不是与反党建立互惠联系。然而,这种党派偏见的内在机制仍不清楚。个人只是更喜欢浏览政治上相近的内容,还是更喜欢与同党建立社交联系?这是出于对党内关系的偏好还是对党外关系的厌恶?在 Twitter(现名为 X)的现场实验中,我们创建了不同党派的机器人账户,以及它们是机器人还是人类。我们随机分配 Twitter 用户(N = 3,013)关注其中一个账户。我们发现了社交动机的证据--相对于反党账户,当账户识别为人类而非机器人时,用户更有可能与同党账户建立互惠链接。我们还发现了党内偏好和党外偏好的证据--与政治中立的账户相比,用户有可能回关注共党账户,也不太可能回关注反党账户。一项后续调查实验(N = 990)进一步证明了议题两极化、党外敌意和党内亲和力在调节在线跟进决策中的不同作用。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Psychological underpinnings of partisan bias in tie formation on social media.","authors":"Mohsen Mosleh,Cameron Martel,David G Rand","doi":"10.1037/xge0001662","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/xge0001662","url":null,"abstract":"Individuals preferentially reciprocate connections with copartisans versus counter-partisans online. However, the mechanisms underlying this partisan bias remain unclear. Do individuals simply prefer viewing politically congenial content, or do they additionally prefer socially connecting with copartisans? Is this driven by preference for in-party ties or distaste for out-party ties? In a Twitter (now called X) field experiment, we created bot accounts varying by partisanship and whether they identified as bots or humans. We randomly assigned Twitter users (N = 3,013) to be followed by one of these accounts. We found evidence for social motivation-users were much more likely to reciprocate links to copartisan relative to counter-partisan accounts when the accounts identified as humans versus bots. We also found evidence for both in-party preference and out-party dispreference-users were as likely to follow back copartisan accounts as they were unlikely to followback counter-partisan accounts, compared to politically neutral accounts. A follow-up survey experiment (N = 990) provides further evidence for distinct roles of issue polarization, out-party animosity, and in-party affinity in moderating follow-back decisions online. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).","PeriodicalId":15698,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology: General","volume":"1 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":4.1,"publicationDate":"2024-10-14","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142439225","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Contrastive adaptation effects along a voice-nonvoice continuum. 语音-非语音连续体的对比适应效应
IF 4.1 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-10-14 DOI: 10.1037/xge0001672
Zi Gao,Andrew J Oxenham
Adaptation to the environment is a universal property of perception across all sensory modalities. It can enhance the salience of new events in an ongoing background and helps maintain perceptual constancy in the face of variable sensory input. Several contrastive adaptation effects have been identified using sounds within the categories of human voice and musical instruments. The present study investigated whether such contrast effects can occur between voice and nonvoice stimulus categories. A 10-step continuum between "voice" (/a/, /o/, or /u/ vowels) and "instrument" (bassoon, horn, or viola) sounds was generated for each of the nine possible pairs. In each trial, an adaptor, either a voice or instrument, was played four times and was followed by a target from along the appropriate continuum. When trials with voice and instrumental adaptors were grouped into separate blocks, strong contrastive adaptation effects were observed, with the target more likely to be identified as a voice following instrumental adaptors and vice versa (Experiment 1). The effects were not observed for visual image adaptors (Experiment 2). The effects were somewhat larger when the adaptors and the target were presented to the same than to different ears, but significant adaptation was observed in both conditions, suggesting contributions of central mechanisms, following binaural integration (Experiment 3). The effect accumulated when the same type of adaptor was presented consecutively and persisted following the end of the adaptors (Experiment 4). The discovery of voice-nonvoice contrastive pairs opens the possibility of studying perceptual or neuronal voice selectivity while keeping acoustic features constant. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).
适应环境是所有感官模式中感知的一个普遍特性。它可以提高新事件在当前背景下的显著性,并有助于在面对多变的感官输入时保持感知的恒定性。利用人声和乐器类别中的声音,已经发现了几种对比适应效应。本研究调查了声音和非声音刺激类别之间是否会产生这种对比效应。在 "声音"(/a/、/o/或/u/元音)和 "乐器"(巴松管、圆号或中提琴)之间,为每对可能的九种声音生成了一个 10 级连续体。在每次试验中,都会播放四次声音或乐器的适配音,然后再播放相应连续体中的目标音。如果将声音和乐器适配器的试验分成不同的组块,就会观察到强烈的对比适配效应,在乐器适配器之后,目标更有可能被识别为声音,反之亦然(实验 1)。视觉图像适配器则没有观察到这种效应(实验 2)。当适配器和目标出现在同一双耳朵而不是不同一双耳朵时,效果更大一些,但在两种条件下都观察到了显著的适应性,这表明在双耳整合后,中枢机制做出了贡献(实验 3)。当连续呈现相同类型的适配器时,这种效应会累积,并在适配器结束后继续存在(实验 4)。声音-非声音对比对的发现为研究知觉或神经元的声音选择性提供了可能性,同时保持声音特征不变。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Contrastive adaptation effects along a voice-nonvoice continuum.","authors":"Zi Gao,Andrew J Oxenham","doi":"10.1037/xge0001672","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/xge0001672","url":null,"abstract":"Adaptation to the environment is a universal property of perception across all sensory modalities. It can enhance the salience of new events in an ongoing background and helps maintain perceptual constancy in the face of variable sensory input. Several contrastive adaptation effects have been identified using sounds within the categories of human voice and musical instruments. The present study investigated whether such contrast effects can occur between voice and nonvoice stimulus categories. A 10-step continuum between \"voice\" (/a/, /o/, or /u/ vowels) and \"instrument\" (bassoon, horn, or viola) sounds was generated for each of the nine possible pairs. In each trial, an adaptor, either a voice or instrument, was played four times and was followed by a target from along the appropriate continuum. When trials with voice and instrumental adaptors were grouped into separate blocks, strong contrastive adaptation effects were observed, with the target more likely to be identified as a voice following instrumental adaptors and vice versa (Experiment 1). The effects were not observed for visual image adaptors (Experiment 2). The effects were somewhat larger when the adaptors and the target were presented to the same than to different ears, but significant adaptation was observed in both conditions, suggesting contributions of central mechanisms, following binaural integration (Experiment 3). The effect accumulated when the same type of adaptor was presented consecutively and persisted following the end of the adaptors (Experiment 4). The discovery of voice-nonvoice contrastive pairs opens the possibility of studying perceptual or neuronal voice selectivity while keeping acoustic features constant. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).","PeriodicalId":15698,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology: General","volume":"58 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":4.1,"publicationDate":"2024-10-14","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142439226","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Unclearly immoral: Low self-concept clarity increases moral disengagement. 不明确的不道德:自我概念不明确会增加道德脱离。
IF 4.1 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-10-10 DOI: 10.1037/xge0001654
Jiaqian Wang,Maferima Touré-Tillery
This research examines the effect of self-concept clarity (i.e., having self-beliefs that are clearly and confidently defined, internally consistent, and stable) on moral behavior. Seven preregistered studies (N = 3,373) document that low (vs. high) self-concept clarity decreases moral behavior (e.g., donation, volunteering, tax compliance, honesty in an incentivized game). This effect occurs because low self-concept clarity increases moral disengagement, leading people to behave in morally questionable manners without damaging their self-concept. As evidence for this proposed underlying mechanism, we show that the effect of self-concept clarity on moral behavior is mediated by state moral disengagement and attenuates (a) among people with low trait moral disengagement, (b) among people with high trait moral identity internalization, and (c) in the presence of an honor pledge cueing moral engagement. We then show that the effect holds only when a prosocial act is congruent with personal values. Overall, these findings contribute to the literature on self-concept and moral self-regulation and have implications for how to promote morality and curb unethical behavior in society. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).
本研究探讨了自我概念清晰度(即拥有定义明确、自信、内在一致且稳定的自我信念)对道德行为的影响。七项预先登记的研究(N = 3,373)表明,低(与高)自我概念清晰度会减少道德行为(如捐赠、志愿服务、遵守税法、在激励游戏中诚实)。之所以会产生这种效应,是因为低自我概念清晰度会增加道德疏离感,导致人们在不损害自我概念的情况下做出道德上有问题的行为。作为这一潜在机制的证据,我们证明了自我概念清晰度对道德行为的影响是由状态道德脱离所中介的,并且在以下情况下会减弱:(a)特质道德脱离程度低的人,(b)特质道德认同内化程度高的人,以及(c)在有荣誉承诺提示道德参与的情况下。然后我们证明,只有当亲社会行为与个人价值观一致时,这种效应才会成立。总之,这些研究结果为有关自我概念和道德自我调节的文献做出了贡献,并对如何在社会中促进道德和遏制不道德行为产生了影响。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Unclearly immoral: Low self-concept clarity increases moral disengagement.","authors":"Jiaqian Wang,Maferima Touré-Tillery","doi":"10.1037/xge0001654","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/xge0001654","url":null,"abstract":"This research examines the effect of self-concept clarity (i.e., having self-beliefs that are clearly and confidently defined, internally consistent, and stable) on moral behavior. Seven preregistered studies (N = 3,373) document that low (vs. high) self-concept clarity decreases moral behavior (e.g., donation, volunteering, tax compliance, honesty in an incentivized game). This effect occurs because low self-concept clarity increases moral disengagement, leading people to behave in morally questionable manners without damaging their self-concept. As evidence for this proposed underlying mechanism, we show that the effect of self-concept clarity on moral behavior is mediated by state moral disengagement and attenuates (a) among people with low trait moral disengagement, (b) among people with high trait moral identity internalization, and (c) in the presence of an honor pledge cueing moral engagement. We then show that the effect holds only when a prosocial act is congruent with personal values. Overall, these findings contribute to the literature on self-concept and moral self-regulation and have implications for how to promote morality and curb unethical behavior in society. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).","PeriodicalId":15698,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology: General","volume":"17 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":4.1,"publicationDate":"2024-10-10","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142436386","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Mental simulation of the approximal future: Imagining what might happen next. 对近似未来的心理模拟:想象接下来可能发生的事情
IF 4.1 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-10-07 DOI: 10.1037/xge0001667
Vannia A Puig,Ruthie Poizner,Katriel Read,Karl K Szpunar
In the course of daily life, various events-such as driving in suboptimal weather conditions, going on a first date, or walking home alone at night-evoke cognitions about what might happen next in the context of ongoing experience. Nonetheless, little is currently known about the phenomenological experience of anticipating events that might occur next-or what we refer to as simulation of the approximal future. We present novel evidence from a retrospective survey, a diary study, and an experimental laboratory study indicating that people commonly experience simulations of the approximal future, and that simulations of the approximal future can be reliably distinguished, in terms of their valence and function, from simulations of future events that are expected to occur in spatiotemporal contexts that are distinct from ongoing experience. Simulation of the approximal future represents an understudied mental experience that carries important implications for understanding the nature of constructive perceptual and memory-based processes as they pertain to event cognition, threat detection, individual differences, and psychopathology. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).
在日常生活中,各种事件--如在不理想的天气条件下开车、第一次约会或晚上独自走回家--都会唤起人们对正在进行的体验中下一步可能发生的事情的认知。然而,目前人们对预测接下来可能发生的事件的现象学体验--或者说我们所说的对近似未来的模拟--知之甚少。我们从一项回顾性调查、一项日记研究和一项实验室实验研究中提出了新的证据,表明人们通常会经历近似未来的模拟,而且近似未来的模拟在其价值和功能方面,可以可靠地与对未来事件的模拟区分开来,这些未来事件预计会在不同于当前经验的时空背景下发生。对近似未来的模拟代表了一种未被充分研究的心理体验,它对理解基于建构性知觉和记忆的过程的性质具有重要意义,因为它们与事件认知、威胁检测、个体差异和心理病理学有关。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA,保留所有权利)。
{"title":"Mental simulation of the approximal future: Imagining what might happen next.","authors":"Vannia A Puig,Ruthie Poizner,Katriel Read,Karl K Szpunar","doi":"10.1037/xge0001667","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/xge0001667","url":null,"abstract":"In the course of daily life, various events-such as driving in suboptimal weather conditions, going on a first date, or walking home alone at night-evoke cognitions about what might happen next in the context of ongoing experience. Nonetheless, little is currently known about the phenomenological experience of anticipating events that might occur next-or what we refer to as simulation of the approximal future. We present novel evidence from a retrospective survey, a diary study, and an experimental laboratory study indicating that people commonly experience simulations of the approximal future, and that simulations of the approximal future can be reliably distinguished, in terms of their valence and function, from simulations of future events that are expected to occur in spatiotemporal contexts that are distinct from ongoing experience. Simulation of the approximal future represents an understudied mental experience that carries important implications for understanding the nature of constructive perceptual and memory-based processes as they pertain to event cognition, threat detection, individual differences, and psychopathology. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).","PeriodicalId":15698,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology: General","volume":"30 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":4.1,"publicationDate":"2024-10-07","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142386313","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
A cautionary note against selective applications of the Bayes factor. 警惕选择性应用贝叶斯系数。
IF 4.1 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-10-07 DOI: 10.1037/xge0001666
Marcel R Schreiner,Wilfried Kunde
Bayes factor analysis becomes increasingly popular, among other reasons, because it allows to provide evidence for the null hypothesis, which is not easily possible with the traditional frequentist approach. A conceivable strategy that apparently takes favorable aspects of both approaches on board is to use traditional frequentist analyses first and to support theoretically interesting nil effects by Bayesian analyses thereafter. Here, we asked whether such a selective application of Bayesian analyses to only nonsignificant effects of foregoing frequentist analyses creates bias. In two simulation studies, we observed that such selective application of Bayesian analyses, in fact, severely overestimates evidence in favor of the null hypotheses, when a true population effect exists. While this bias can be attenuated by using more informative priors in the Bayesian analyses, we recommend to not apply such selective combination of analytical approaches, but instead to use either frequentist or Bayesian analyses consistently. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).
贝叶斯因子分析之所以越来越受欢迎,除其他原因外,还因为它可以为零假设提供证据,而传统的频数主义方法则很难做到这一点。一种可以想象的策略显然兼顾了两种方法的有利方面,即首先使用传统的频数分析,然后通过贝叶斯分析来支持理论上有趣的零效应。在此,我们提出这样一个问题:选择性地将贝叶斯分析应用于非显著效应,而不使用频数分析,是否会产生偏差?在两项模拟研究中,我们观察到,当存在真正的群体效应时,这种选择性应用贝叶斯分析实际上会严重高估有利于零假设的证据。虽然这种偏差可以通过在贝叶斯分析中使用信息量更大的先验来减弱,但我们建议不要有选择性地结合使用这种分析方法,而应一致地使用频数分析或贝叶斯分析。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"A cautionary note against selective applications of the Bayes factor.","authors":"Marcel R Schreiner,Wilfried Kunde","doi":"10.1037/xge0001666","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/xge0001666","url":null,"abstract":"Bayes factor analysis becomes increasingly popular, among other reasons, because it allows to provide evidence for the null hypothesis, which is not easily possible with the traditional frequentist approach. A conceivable strategy that apparently takes favorable aspects of both approaches on board is to use traditional frequentist analyses first and to support theoretically interesting nil effects by Bayesian analyses thereafter. Here, we asked whether such a selective application of Bayesian analyses to only nonsignificant effects of foregoing frequentist analyses creates bias. In two simulation studies, we observed that such selective application of Bayesian analyses, in fact, severely overestimates evidence in favor of the null hypotheses, when a true population effect exists. While this bias can be attenuated by using more informative priors in the Bayesian analyses, we recommend to not apply such selective combination of analytical approaches, but instead to use either frequentist or Bayesian analyses consistently. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).","PeriodicalId":15698,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology: General","volume":"14 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":4.1,"publicationDate":"2024-10-07","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142386314","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Connector hubs in semantic network contribute to creative thinking. 语义网络中的连接枢纽有助于创造性思维。
IF 4.1 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-10-07 DOI: 10.1037/xge0001675
Li He,Yoed N Kenett,Kaixiang Zhuang,Jiangzhou Sun,Qunlin Chen,Jiang Qiu
Semantic memory offers a rich repository of raw materials (e.g., various concepts and connections between concepts) for creative thinking, represented as a semantic network. Similar to other networks, the semantic network exhibits a modular structure characterized by modules with dense internal connections and sparse connections between them. This organizational principle facilitates the routine storage and retrieval of information but may impede creativity. The present study investigated the effect of hub concepts with varying connection patterns on creative thinking from the perspective of a modular structured semantic network. By analyzing a large-scale semantic network, connector hubs (C-hubs) and provincial hubs (P-hubs) were identified based on their intra- and intermodule connections. These hubs were used as cue words in the alternative uses task, a widely used measure of creative thinking. Across four experiments, behavioral and neural evidence indicated that C-hubs facilitate the generation of more novel and remote ideas compared to P-hubs. However, this effect is predominantly observed in the early stage of the creative thinking process, involving changes in brain activation and functional connectivity in core regions of the default mode network and the frontoparietal network, including the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, angular gyrus, and precuneus. Neural findings suggest that the superior performance of C-hubs relies on stronger interactions between automatic spreading activation, controlled semantic retrieval, and attentional regulation of salient information. These results provide insight into how concepts with varying semantic connection patterns facilitate and constrain different stages of the creative thinking process through the modular structure of semantic network. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).
语义记忆为创造性思维提供了丰富的原材料库(如各种概念和概念之间的联系),表现为语义网络。与其他网络类似,语义网络呈现模块化结构,其特点是模块内部联系密集,模块之间的联系稀疏。这种组织原则有利于信息的常规存储和检索,但可能会阻碍创造力的发挥。本研究从模块化结构语义网络的角度,研究了具有不同连接模式的枢纽概念对创造性思维的影响。通过分析大规模语义网络,根据模块内和模块间的连接,确定了连接器枢纽(C-枢纽)和省枢纽(P-枢纽)。这些枢纽在另类使用任务中被用作提示词,这是一种广泛用于衡量创造性思维的方法。在四项实验中,行为和神经证据表明,与 P 中枢相比,C 中枢有助于产生更多新颖和遥远的想法。不过,这种效应主要出现在创造性思维过程的早期阶段,涉及默认模式网络和前顶叶网络核心区域(包括背外侧前额叶皮层、角回和楔前区)的大脑激活和功能连接变化。神经研究结果表明,C-枢纽的卓越表现依赖于自动扩散激活、受控语义检索和对突出信息的注意调节之间更强的相互作用。这些结果让我们深入了解了具有不同语义连接模式的概念是如何通过语义网络的模块化结构促进和制约创造性思维过程的不同阶段的。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Connector hubs in semantic network contribute to creative thinking.","authors":"Li He,Yoed N Kenett,Kaixiang Zhuang,Jiangzhou Sun,Qunlin Chen,Jiang Qiu","doi":"10.1037/xge0001675","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/xge0001675","url":null,"abstract":"Semantic memory offers a rich repository of raw materials (e.g., various concepts and connections between concepts) for creative thinking, represented as a semantic network. Similar to other networks, the semantic network exhibits a modular structure characterized by modules with dense internal connections and sparse connections between them. This organizational principle facilitates the routine storage and retrieval of information but may impede creativity. The present study investigated the effect of hub concepts with varying connection patterns on creative thinking from the perspective of a modular structured semantic network. By analyzing a large-scale semantic network, connector hubs (C-hubs) and provincial hubs (P-hubs) were identified based on their intra- and intermodule connections. These hubs were used as cue words in the alternative uses task, a widely used measure of creative thinking. Across four experiments, behavioral and neural evidence indicated that C-hubs facilitate the generation of more novel and remote ideas compared to P-hubs. However, this effect is predominantly observed in the early stage of the creative thinking process, involving changes in brain activation and functional connectivity in core regions of the default mode network and the frontoparietal network, including the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, angular gyrus, and precuneus. Neural findings suggest that the superior performance of C-hubs relies on stronger interactions between automatic spreading activation, controlled semantic retrieval, and attentional regulation of salient information. These results provide insight into how concepts with varying semantic connection patterns facilitate and constrain different stages of the creative thinking process through the modular structure of semantic network. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).","PeriodicalId":15698,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology: General","volume":"72 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":4.1,"publicationDate":"2024-10-07","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142386312","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Analytic racecraft: Race-based averages create illusory group differences in perceptions of racism. 种族分析技术:基于种族的平均值会在对种族主义的认知上造成虚幻的群体差异。
IF 4.1 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-10-07 DOI: 10.1037/xge0001673
Joel E Martinez
Research practices used by social scientists to understand and dismantle the psychological foundations that uphold racist hierarchies can backfire when they rely on racecraft. Racecraft ideology assumes the reality of race(s), an assumption that shapes study designs and inferences to the detriment of theoretical and practical goals. I showcase how racecraft manifests in studies seeking to quantify how perceptions of sociopolitical stimuli differ across racialized perceivers (e.g., black, white, latinx). The typical analysis for quantifying perceptions focuses on comparing group averages, which assumes the existence of discrete "races" whose perceptions can be sufficiently summarized by averages. Across three studies, I used variance component analyses on racism ratings of anti-immigrant tweets from differently racialized perceivers (N = 1,211) to show there was much larger disagreement than agreement within race categories, even when there were average differences in perceptions across race categories. This analysis shows how analytic practices can bolster different assumptions about the nature of race, some of which reify the illusion that race categories are stable cohesive groups. Researchers can improve their analytic inferences and avoid producing race-reifying caricatures of peoples' perceptions by adding variance mapping to their toolkits and attending to racialization as a dynamic process-needed improvements within the psychological study of race and racism, group-based beliefs, and antiracist research endeavors. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).
社会科学家为了解和瓦解维护种族主义等级制度的心理基础而采取的研究做法,如果依赖于种族思想,就会适得其反。种族意识形态假定了种族的真实性,这种假定影响了研究设计和推论,从而损害了理论和实践目标。我将展示种族法如何体现在寻求量化不同种族感知者(如黑人、白人、拉丁裔)对社会政治刺激的感知差异的研究中。量化感知的典型分析侧重于比较群体平均值,这就假定存在离散的 "种族",其感知可以用平均值充分概括。在三项研究中,我对来自不同种族的感知者(N = 1,211)的反移民推文的种族主义评分进行了方差分析,结果显示,即使不同种族类别的感知存在平均差异,种族类别内的分歧也远远大于一致。这项分析表明了分析实践如何支持对种族本质的不同假设,其中一些假设还强化了种族类别是稳定的内聚群体的假象。研究人员可以改进他们的分析推论,通过在工具包中添加方差图,并将种族化作为一个动态过程来关注,避免产生种族化的人们认知漫画--这是对种族和种族主义的心理学研究、基于群体的信念以及反种族主义研究工作所需要的改进。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA,保留所有权利)。
{"title":"Analytic racecraft: Race-based averages create illusory group differences in perceptions of racism.","authors":"Joel E Martinez","doi":"10.1037/xge0001673","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/xge0001673","url":null,"abstract":"Research practices used by social scientists to understand and dismantle the psychological foundations that uphold racist hierarchies can backfire when they rely on racecraft. Racecraft ideology assumes the reality of race(s), an assumption that shapes study designs and inferences to the detriment of theoretical and practical goals. I showcase how racecraft manifests in studies seeking to quantify how perceptions of sociopolitical stimuli differ across racialized perceivers (e.g., black, white, latinx). The typical analysis for quantifying perceptions focuses on comparing group averages, which assumes the existence of discrete \"races\" whose perceptions can be sufficiently summarized by averages. Across three studies, I used variance component analyses on racism ratings of anti-immigrant tweets from differently racialized perceivers (N = 1,211) to show there was much larger disagreement than agreement within race categories, even when there were average differences in perceptions across race categories. This analysis shows how analytic practices can bolster different assumptions about the nature of race, some of which reify the illusion that race categories are stable cohesive groups. Researchers can improve their analytic inferences and avoid producing race-reifying caricatures of peoples' perceptions by adding variance mapping to their toolkits and attending to racialization as a dynamic process-needed improvements within the psychological study of race and racism, group-based beliefs, and antiracist research endeavors. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).","PeriodicalId":15698,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology: General","volume":"8 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":4.1,"publicationDate":"2024-10-07","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142386315","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Journal of Experimental Psychology: General
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1