首页 > 最新文献

Journal of memory and language最新文献

英文 中文
Cross-linguistic differences in gender congruency effects: Evidence from meta-analyses 性别一致性效应的跨语言差异:来自元分析的证据
IF 4.3 1区 心理学 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2023-08-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.jml.2023.104428
Audrey Bürki , Emiel van den Hoven , Niels Schiller , Nikolay Dimitrov

It has been proposed that the order in which words are prepared for production depends on the speaker’s language. When producing the translation equivalent of the small cat, speakers of German or Dutch select the gender-marked determiner at a relatively early stage of production. Speakers of French or Italian postpone the encoding of a determiner or adjective until the phonological form of the noun is available. Hence, even though the words are produced in the same order (e.g., die kleine Katze in German, le petit chat in French), they are not planned in the same order and might require different amounts of advanced planning prior to production onset. This distinction between early and late selection languages was proposed to account for the observation that speakers of Germanic and Slavic languages, but not of Romance languages, are slower to name pictures in the context of a distractor word of a different gender. Meta-analyses are conducted to provide the first direct test of this cross-linguistic difference and to test a prediction of the late selection hypothesis. They confirm the existence of the gender congruency effect in German/Slavic languages and its absence in Romance languages when target and distractor words are presented simultaneously. They do not allow confirming the hypothesis that in the latter languages, a similar effect emerges when the presentation of the distractor is delayed. Overall, these analyses confirm the cross-linguistic difference but show that the evidence available to date is not sufficient to confirm or reject the late selection hypothesis as an explanation of this difference. We highlight specific directions for future research.

有人提出,单词准备生产的顺序取决于说话人的语言。在翻译小猫时,讲德语或荷兰语的人在相对较早的阶段就选择了性别标记限定词。说法语或意大利语的人会把限定词或形容词的编码推迟到名词的音系形式可用为止。因此,即使单词以相同的顺序产生(例如,德语中的die kleine Katze,法语中的le petit chat),它们也不会以相同的顺序进行计划,并且可能需要在生产开始之前进行不同数量的高级计划。早期选择语言和晚期选择语言之间的这种区别,是为了解释这样一种观察结果:说日耳曼语和斯拉夫语的人,而不是罗曼语的人,在不同性别的干扰词上下文中,给图片命名的速度较慢。荟萃分析提供了这种跨语言差异的第一个直接测试,并测试了晚期选择假说的预测。他们证实了当目标词和干扰词同时出现时,德语/斯拉夫语存在性别一致性效应,而罗曼语不存在性别一致性效应。他们无法证实在后一种语言中,当分心物的呈现延迟时,也会出现类似的效果。总的来说,这些分析证实了跨语言差异,但表明迄今为止可用的证据不足以证实或拒绝晚期选择假说作为这种差异的解释。我们强调了未来研究的具体方向。
{"title":"Cross-linguistic differences in gender congruency effects: Evidence from meta-analyses","authors":"Audrey Bürki ,&nbsp;Emiel van den Hoven ,&nbsp;Niels Schiller ,&nbsp;Nikolay Dimitrov","doi":"10.1016/j.jml.2023.104428","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.jml.2023.104428","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>It has been proposed that the order in which words are prepared for production depends on the speaker’s language. When producing the translation equivalent of <em>the small cat</em>, speakers of German or Dutch select the gender-marked determiner at a relatively early stage of production. Speakers of French or Italian postpone the encoding of a determiner or adjective until the phonological form of the noun is available. Hence, even though the words are produced in the same order (e.g., <em>die kleine Katze</em> in German, <em>le petit chat in French</em>), they are not planned in the same order and might require different amounts of advanced planning prior to production onset. This distinction between early and late selection languages was proposed to account for the observation that speakers of Germanic and Slavic languages, but not of Romance languages, are slower to name pictures in the context of a distractor word of a different gender. Meta-analyses are conducted to provide the first direct test of this cross-linguistic difference and to test a prediction of the late selection hypothesis. They confirm the existence of the gender congruency effect in German/Slavic languages and its absence in Romance languages when target and distractor words are presented simultaneously. They do not allow confirming the hypothesis that in the latter languages, a similar effect emerges when the presentation of the distractor is delayed. Overall, these analyses confirm the cross-linguistic difference but show that the evidence available to date is not sufficient to confirm or reject the late selection hypothesis as an explanation of this difference. We highlight specific directions for future research.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":16493,"journal":{"name":"Journal of memory and language","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":4.3,"publicationDate":"2023-08-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"48614642","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
A systematic evaluation of factors affecting referring expression choice in passage completion tasks 文章完成任务中影响指称表达选择因素的系统评价
IF 4.3 1区 心理学 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2023-06-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.jml.2023.104413
Vera Demberg, Ekaterina Kravtchenko, Jia E. Loy

There is a long-standing controversy around the question of whether referent predictability affects pronominalization: while there are good theoretical reasons for this prediction (e.g., Arnold, 2008), the experimental evidence has been rather mixed.

We here report on three highly powered studies that manipulate a range of factors that have differed between previous studies, in order to determine more exactly under which conditions a predictability effect on pronominalization can be found.

We use a constrained as well as a free reference task, and manipulate verb type, antecedent ambiguity, length of NP and whether the stimuli are presented within a story context or not. Our results find the story context to be the single important factor that allows to elicit an effect of predictability on pronoun choice, in line with (Rosa and Arnold, 2017; Weatherford and Arnold, 2021). We also propose a parametrization for a rational speech act model, that reconciles the findings between many of the experiments in the literature.

关于指称物的可预测性是否影响代词化的问题,存在着长期的争议:虽然这种预测有很好的理论依据(例如,Arnold,2008),但实验证据却相当混杂。我们在这里报告了三项强有力的研究,这些研究操纵了一系列与先前研究不同的因素,以更准确地确定在哪些条件下可以发现对代词化的可预测性影响。我们使用约束和自由参考任务,并操纵动词类型、先行词歧义、NP长度以及刺激是否在故事上下文中呈现。我们的研究结果发现,故事背景是引发代词选择可预测性影响的唯一重要因素,这与(Rosa和Arnold,2017;Weatherford和Arnol,2021)一致。我们还提出了理性言语行为模型的参数化,该模型调和了文献中许多实验的发现。
{"title":"A systematic evaluation of factors affecting referring expression choice in passage completion tasks","authors":"Vera Demberg,&nbsp;Ekaterina Kravtchenko,&nbsp;Jia E. Loy","doi":"10.1016/j.jml.2023.104413","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.jml.2023.104413","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>There is a long-standing controversy around the question of whether referent predictability affects pronominalization: while there are good theoretical reasons for this prediction (e.g., Arnold, 2008), the experimental evidence has been rather mixed.</p><p>We here report on three highly powered studies that manipulate a range of factors that have differed between previous studies, in order to determine more exactly under which conditions a predictability effect on pronominalization can be found.</p><p>We use a constrained as well as a free reference task, and manipulate verb type, antecedent ambiguity, length of NP and whether the stimuli are presented within a story context or not. Our results find the story context to be the single important factor that allows to elicit an effect of predictability on pronoun choice, in line with (Rosa and Arnold, 2017; Weatherford and Arnold, 2021). We also propose a parametrization for a rational speech act model, that reconciles the findings between many of the experiments in the literature.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":16493,"journal":{"name":"Journal of memory and language","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":4.3,"publicationDate":"2023-06-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC10029927/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9559160","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
Inhibitory control of the dominant language: Reversed language dominance is the tip of the iceberg 优势语言的抑制性控制:反向语言优势是冰山一角
IF 4.3 1区 心理学 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2023-06-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.jml.2023.104410
Matthew Goldrick , Tamar H. Gollan

Theories of speech production have proposed that in contexts where multiple languages are produced, bilinguals inhibit the dominant language with the goal of making both languages equally accessible. This process often overshoots this goal, leading to a surprising pattern: better performance in the nondominant vs dominant language, or reversed language dominance effects. However, the reliability of this effect in single word production studies with cued language switches has been challenged by a recent meta-analysis. Correcting for errors in this analysis, we find that dominance effects are reliably reduced and reversed during language mixing. Reversed dominance has also consistently been reported in the production of connected speech elicited by reading aloud of mixed language paragraphs. When switching, bilinguals produced translation-equivalent intrusion errors (e.g., saying pero instead of but) more often when intending to produce words in the dominant language. We show this dominant language vulnerability is not exclusive to switching out of the nondominant language and extends to non-switch words, linking connected speech results to patterns first reported in single-picture naming studies. Reversed language dominance is a robust phenomenon that reflects the tip of the iceberg of inhibitory control of the dominant language in bilingual language production.

语言产生理论提出,在多种语言产生的环境中,双语者抑制了主导语言,目的是使两种语言都能平等地使用。这个过程经常超过这个目标,导致一个令人惊讶的模式:非主导语言比主导语言表现更好,或者相反的语言优势效应。然而,最近的一项荟萃分析对这一效应在有提示语言转换的单字生成研究中的可靠性提出了挑战。在修正本分析中的错误后,我们发现优势效应在语言混合过程中可靠地减少和逆转。在大声朗读混合语言段落引发的连接言语的产生中也一直有反向支配的报道。当转换时,双语者在打算用主导语言表达单词时,更经常产生翻译等效的入侵错误(例如,说pero而不是but)。我们发现,这种主导语言的脆弱性并不局限于切换出非主导语言,也延伸到非切换词,将连接的语音结果与首次在单图片命名研究中报告的模式联系起来。反向语言优势是一种强大的现象,它反映了双语语言产生中主导语言抑制控制的冰山一角。
{"title":"Inhibitory control of the dominant language: Reversed language dominance is the tip of the iceberg","authors":"Matthew Goldrick ,&nbsp;Tamar H. Gollan","doi":"10.1016/j.jml.2023.104410","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.jml.2023.104410","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Theories of speech production have proposed that in contexts where multiple languages are produced, bilinguals inhibit the dominant language with the goal of making both languages equally accessible. This process often overshoots this goal, leading to a surprising pattern: better performance in the nondominant vs dominant language, or <em>reversed language dominance</em> effects. However, the reliability of this effect in single word production studies with cued language switches has been challenged by a recent <em>meta</em>-analysis. Correcting for errors in this analysis, we find that dominance effects are reliably reduced and reversed during language mixing. Reversed dominance has also consistently been reported in the production of connected speech elicited by reading aloud of mixed language paragraphs. When switching, bilinguals produced translation-equivalent intrusion errors (e.g., saying<!--> <em>pero<!--> </em>instead of<!--> <em>but</em>) more often when intending to produce words in the dominant language. We show this dominant language vulnerability is not exclusive to switching out of the nondominant language and extends to non-switch words, linking connected speech results to patterns first reported in single-picture naming studies. Reversed language dominance is a robust phenomenon that reflects the tip of the iceberg of inhibitory control of the dominant language in bilingual language production.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":16493,"journal":{"name":"Journal of memory and language","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":4.3,"publicationDate":"2023-06-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC9983628/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10847704","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 3
Evaluating the conceptual strategy change account of test-potentiated new learning in list recall 表回忆中测试增强新学习的概念策略改变机制评价
IF 4.3 1区 心理学 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2023-06-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.jml.2023.104412
Shaun Boustani , Caleb Owens , Hilary J. Don , Chunliang Yang , David R. Shanks

Prior testing potentiates new learning, an effect known as test-potentiated new learning (TPNL). Research using lists of related words has established that testing, by free recall, also increases semantic clustering of later recall output. It has been suggested that this is evidence that testing induces a strategy change in encoding and retrieval towards greater conceptual organisation. The current research evaluated whether this conceptual strategy change explains TPNL in three experiments. We found a) that a retrieval task that did not increase semantic clustering (list discrimination) consistently produced TPNL, and b) that factors (word-relatedness and list structure) that influenced the amount of semantic clustering had no effect on the magnitude of TPNL. These results suggest that conceptual strategy change is neither necessary nor sufficient for TPNL and is more likely to be an effect of testing, rather than a cause of TPNL.

先前的测试增强了新的学习,这种效应被称为测试增强的新学习(TPNL)。使用相关单词列表的研究已经证实,通过自由回忆的测试,也增加了后来回忆输出的语义聚类。有人认为,这是测试导致编码和检索策略发生变化的证据,有利于更大的概念组织。目前的研究在三个实验中评估了这种概念策略的改变是否解释了TPNL。我们发现a)不增加语义聚类(列表辨别)的检索任务始终产生TPNL, b)影响语义聚类数量的因素(词相关性和列表结构)对TPNL的大小没有影响。这些结果表明,概念策略的改变既不是TPNL的必要条件,也不是充分条件,更可能是测试的结果,而不是TPNL的原因。
{"title":"Evaluating the conceptual strategy change account of test-potentiated new learning in list recall","authors":"Shaun Boustani ,&nbsp;Caleb Owens ,&nbsp;Hilary J. Don ,&nbsp;Chunliang Yang ,&nbsp;David R. Shanks","doi":"10.1016/j.jml.2023.104412","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.jml.2023.104412","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Prior testing potentiates new learning, an effect known as test-potentiated new learning (TPNL). Research using lists of related words has established that testing, by free recall, also increases semantic clustering of later recall output. It has been suggested that this is evidence that testing induces a strategy change in encoding and retrieval towards greater conceptual organisation. The current research evaluated whether this conceptual strategy change explains TPNL in three experiments. We found a) that a retrieval task that did not increase semantic clustering (list discrimination) consistently produced TPNL, and b) that factors (word-relatedness and list structure) that influenced the amount of semantic clustering had no effect on the magnitude of TPNL. These results suggest that conceptual strategy change is neither necessary nor sufficient for TPNL and is more likely to be an effect of testing, rather than a cause of TPNL.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":16493,"journal":{"name":"Journal of memory and language","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":4.3,"publicationDate":"2023-06-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"47248445","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Number feature distortion modulates cue-based retrieval in reading 数字特征失真调制了基于线索的阅读检索
IF 4.3 1区 心理学 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2023-02-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.jml.2022.104400
Himanshu Yadav , Garrett Smith , Sebastian Reich , Shravan Vasishth

In sentence comprehension, what are the cognitive constraints that determine number agreement computation? Two broad classes of theoretical proposals are: (i) Representation distortion accounts, which assume that the number feature on the subject noun gets overwritten probabilistically by the number feature on a non-subject noun, leading to a non-veridical memory trace of the subject noun; and (ii) The cue-based retrieval account, a general account of dependency completion processes which assumes that the features on the subject noun remain intact, and that processing difficulty is only a function of the memory constraints on dependency completion. However, both these classes of model fail to account for the full spectrum of number agreement patterns observed in published studies. Using 17 benchmark datasets on number agreement from four languages, we implement seven computational models: three variants of representation distortion, two cue-based retrieval models, and two hybrid models that assume both representation-distortion and retrieval. Quantitative model comparison shows that the best fit is achieved by a hybrid model that assumes both feature distortion (specifically, feature percolation) and cue-based retrieval; numerically, the second-best quantitative fit was achieved by a distortion-based model of number attraction that assumes grammaticality bias during reading. More broadly, the work furnishes comprehensive evidence to support the idea that cue-based retrieval theory, which aims to be a general account of dependency completion, needs to incorporate a feature distortion process.

在句子理解中,决定数字一致性计算的认知约束是什么?两大类理论建议是:(i)表征失真说,它假设主名词上的数字特征被非主名词上的数字特征概率地覆盖,导致主名词的非真实性记忆痕迹;(ii)基于线索的检索解释,这是一种依赖完成过程的一般解释,它假设主语的特征保持不变,并且处理难度仅是依赖完成的记忆约束的函数。然而,这两类模型都不能解释在已发表的研究中观察到的数字一致模式的全部范围。使用来自4种语言的17个关于数字一致性的基准数据集,我们实现了7个计算模型:3个表征失真的变体,2个基于线索的检索模型,以及2个同时假设表征失真和检索的混合模型。定量模型比较表明,混合模型同时假设特征失真(具体而言,特征渗透)和基于线索的检索,可以获得最佳拟合;在数字上,第二好的定量拟合是通过基于扭曲的数字吸引模型实现的,该模型假设阅读过程中存在语法偏差。更广泛地说,这项工作提供了全面的证据来支持基于线索的检索理论,该理论旨在成为依赖完成的一般解释,需要纳入特征失真过程。
{"title":"Number feature distortion modulates cue-based retrieval in reading","authors":"Himanshu Yadav ,&nbsp;Garrett Smith ,&nbsp;Sebastian Reich ,&nbsp;Shravan Vasishth","doi":"10.1016/j.jml.2022.104400","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.jml.2022.104400","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>In sentence comprehension, what are the cognitive constraints that determine number agreement computation? Two broad classes of theoretical proposals are: (i) <em>Representation distortion accounts</em>, which assume that the number feature on the subject noun gets overwritten probabilistically by the number feature on a non-subject noun, leading to a non-veridical memory trace of the subject noun; and (ii) The <em>cue-based retrieval account</em>, a general account of dependency completion processes which assumes that the features on the subject noun remain intact, and that processing difficulty is only a function of the memory constraints on dependency completion. However, both these classes of model fail to account for the full spectrum of number agreement patterns observed in published studies. Using 17 benchmark datasets on number agreement from four languages, we implement seven computational models: three variants of representation distortion, two cue-based retrieval models, and two hybrid models that assume both representation-distortion and retrieval. Quantitative model comparison shows that the best fit is achieved by a hybrid model that assumes both feature distortion (specifically, feature percolation) and cue-based retrieval; numerically, the second-best quantitative fit was achieved by a distortion-based model of number attraction that assumes grammaticality bias during reading. More broadly, the work furnishes comprehensive evidence to support the idea that cue-based retrieval theory, which aims to be a general account of dependency completion, needs to incorporate a feature distortion process.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":16493,"journal":{"name":"Journal of memory and language","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":4.3,"publicationDate":"2023-02-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"48532953","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 7
The interplay between syntactic and non-syntactic structure in language production 语言生产中句法结构和非句法结构的相互作用
IF 4.3 1区 心理学 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2023-02-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.jml.2022.104385
Kumiko Fukumura, Shi Zhang

Speakers frequently reuse earlier encountered structures. A long-standing view in language production research is that this structural priming is driven by the persistence of abstract syntax, independent from unordered, conceptual representations. However, evidence has been building that non-syntactic information can also influence structural choice. Here we examined whether and how the syntactic priming of relative clause structures might interact with the priming of the conceptual category order of adjectives in noun phrase production. Study 1 found that speakers are more likely to produce relative clause structures (spotted bow that’s green) after having heard relative clause structures (striped lock that’s blue) as opposed to an alternative structure (striped blue lock), and they also tended to repeat the conceptual order of the prime, with more pattern-first orders after pattern-first primes than after color-first primes. Critically, we found larger syntactic priming when the conceptual order of the prime persisted more in the target and larger conceptual order priming when the syntactic structure of the prime persisted more in the target. Studies 2 and 3 found that conceptual category order priming can be enhanced by adjective overlap as well as noun overlap between prime and target, whereas syntactic priming can only be enhanced by noun overlap. These results supported the interactive priming account: Although the syntactic structure and the conceptual order are represented at different levels and hence can be activated independently, the link between them is also primed, which enhances priming at both levels.

演讲者经常重复使用以前遇到的结构。语言生产研究中的一个长期观点是,这种结构启动是由抽象语法的持久性驱动的,独立于无序的概念表示。然而,有证据表明,非句法信息也会影响结构选择。在这里,我们考察了在名词短语产生中,关系从句结构的句法启动是否以及如何与形容词概念范畴顺序的启动相互作用。研究1发现,与替代结构(条纹蓝锁)相比,说话者在听到相对从句结构(蓝色的条纹锁)后更有可能产生相对从句结构,而且他们也倾向于重复素数的概念顺序,在图案第一素数之后比在颜色第一素数之后具有更多的图案第一阶。至关重要的是,当素数的概念顺序在目标中持续得更多时,我们发现了更大的句法启动,而当素数的句法结构在目标中保持得更多时则发现了更大规模的概念顺序启动。研究2和3发现,形容词重叠以及主词和宾语之间的名词重叠可以增强概念范畴顺序启动,而句法启动只能通过名词重叠来增强。这些结果支持了交互启动解释:尽管句法结构和概念顺序在不同层面上表现出来,因此可以独立激活,但它们之间的联系也被启动,这增强了两个层面的启动。
{"title":"The interplay between syntactic and non-syntactic structure in language production","authors":"Kumiko Fukumura,&nbsp;Shi Zhang","doi":"10.1016/j.jml.2022.104385","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.jml.2022.104385","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Speakers frequently reuse earlier encountered structures. A long-standing view in language production research is that this <em>structural priming</em> is driven by the persistence of abstract syntax, independent from unordered, conceptual representations. However, evidence has been building that non-syntactic information can also influence structural choice. Here we examined whether and how the syntactic priming of relative clause structures might interact with the priming of the conceptual category order of adjectives in noun phrase production. Study 1 found that speakers are more likely to produce relative clause structures (<em>spotted bow that’s green</em>) after having heard relative clause structures (<em>striped lock that’s blue</em>) as opposed to an alternative structure (<em>striped blue lock</em>), and they also tended to repeat the conceptual order of the prime, with more pattern-first orders after pattern-first primes than after color-first primes. Critically, we found larger syntactic priming when the conceptual order of the prime persisted more in the target and larger conceptual order priming when the syntactic structure of the prime persisted more in the target. Studies 2 and 3 found that conceptual category order priming can be enhanced by adjective overlap as well as noun overlap between prime and target, whereas syntactic priming can only be enhanced by noun overlap. These results supported the interactive priming account: Although the syntactic structure and the conceptual order are represented at different levels and hence can be activated independently, the link between them is also primed, which enhances priming at both levels.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":16493,"journal":{"name":"Journal of memory and language","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":4.3,"publicationDate":"2023-02-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"43633823","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
Beware influential findings that have not been replicated 小心那些没有被复制的有影响力的发现
IF 4.3 1区 心理学 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2023-02-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.jml.2022.104390
Kathleen Rastle, Jason Chan, Anne Cleary, Penny Pexman, Adrian Staub
{"title":"Beware influential findings that have not been replicated","authors":"Kathleen Rastle,&nbsp;Jason Chan,&nbsp;Anne Cleary,&nbsp;Penny Pexman,&nbsp;Adrian Staub","doi":"10.1016/j.jml.2022.104390","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.jml.2022.104390","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":16493,"journal":{"name":"Journal of memory and language","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":4.3,"publicationDate":"2023-02-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"49037487","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Weaker than you might imagine: Determining imageability effects on word recognition 比你想象的要弱:决定可想象性对单词识别的影响
IF 4.3 1区 心理学 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2023-02-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.jml.2022.104398
Agata Dymarska , Louise Connell , Briony Banks

Imageability – the ease of generating a mental image for a word – has been commonly used as a predictor of word recognition but its effects are highly variable across the literature, raising questions about the robustness and stability of the construct. We compared six existing imageability norms in their ability to predict RT and accuracy in lexical decision and word naming across thousands of words. Results showed that, when lexical and sensorimotor sources of variance were partialled out, imageability predicted little unique variance in word recognition performance and effect sizes varied greatly between norms. Further analysis suggested that such heterogenous effect sizes are likely due to inconsistent strategies in how participants interpret and rate imageability in norming studies, despite consistent instructions. Our findings suggest that the ease of generating a mental image for a word does not reliably facilitate word recognition and that imageability ratings should be used with caution in such research.

可想象性——为一个单词生成心理图像的难易程度——通常被用作单词识别的预测指标,但其影响在文献中是高度可变的,这引发了对该结构的稳健性和稳定性的质疑。我们比较了六种现有的可想象性规范在数千个单词的词汇决策和单词命名方面预测RT和准确性的能力。结果表明,当词汇和感觉运动的差异源被分离出来时,可想象性预测单词识别表现的独特差异很小,不同规范之间的效应大小差异很大。进一步的分析表明,这种异质效应可能是由于参与者在规范研究中如何解释和评价可想象性的策略不一致,尽管有一致的指示。我们的研究结果表明,为一个单词生成心理图像的难易程度并不能可靠地促进单词识别,在这类研究中应该谨慎使用可想象性评级。
{"title":"Weaker than you might imagine: Determining imageability effects on word recognition","authors":"Agata Dymarska ,&nbsp;Louise Connell ,&nbsp;Briony Banks","doi":"10.1016/j.jml.2022.104398","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.jml.2022.104398","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Imageability – the ease of generating a mental image for a word – has been commonly used as a predictor of word recognition but its effects are highly variable across the literature, raising questions about the robustness and stability of the construct. We compared six existing imageability norms in their ability to predict RT and accuracy in lexical decision and word naming across thousands of words. Results showed that, when lexical and sensorimotor sources of variance were partialled out, imageability predicted little unique variance in word recognition performance and effect sizes varied greatly between norms. Further analysis suggested that such heterogenous effect sizes are likely due to inconsistent strategies in how participants interpret and rate imageability in norming studies, despite consistent instructions. Our findings suggest that the ease of generating a mental image for a word does not reliably facilitate word recognition and that imageability ratings should be used with caution in such research.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":16493,"journal":{"name":"Journal of memory and language","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":4.3,"publicationDate":"2023-02-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"48487017","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Number and syllabification of following consonants influence use of long versus short vowels in English disyllables 以下辅音的数量和音节影响英语双音节中长元音和短元音的使用
IF 4.3 1区 心理学 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2023-02-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.jml.2022.104399
Rebecca Treiman, Brett Kessler, Kayla Hensley

Spelling-to-sound translation in English is particularly complex for vowels. For example, the pronunciations of ‹a› include the long vowel of ‹paper› and ‹sacred› and the short vowel of ‹cactus› and ‹happy›. We examined the factors that are associated with use of long versus short vowels by conducting analyses of English disyllabic words with single medial consonants and consonant sequences and three behavioral studies in which a total of 119 university students pronounced nonwords with these structures. The vocabulary analyses show that both the number of medial consonants and their syllabification influence vowel length. Participants were influenced by these aspects of context, some of which are not explicitly taught as a part of reading instruction. Although these results point to implicit statistical learning, participants produced fewer long vowels before single medial consonants than anticipated based on our vocabulary statistics for spelling-to-sound correspondences in disyllabic words. Participants also produced more long vowels before two identical consonant letters than anticipated given these statistics. We consider the reasons for these outcomes, and we also use the behavioral data to test two models of spelling-to-sound translation.

英语中的拼写到发音的翻译对于元音来说尤其复杂。例如,Şa›的发音包括Şpaper›和Ş神圣›的长元音,以及Şcactus›和⑪happy›的短元音。我们通过对具有单中间辅音和辅音序列的英语双音节单词的分析,以及对119名大学生发音具有这些结构的非单词的三项行为研究,检验了与长短元音使用相关的因素。词汇分析表明,中间辅音的数量及其音节划分都会影响元音长度。参与者受到语境的这些方面的影响,其中一些并没有作为阅读教学的一部分明确教授。尽管这些结果表明了内隐统计学习,但根据我们对双音节单词拼写与发音对应的词汇统计,参与者在单个中间辅音之前产生的长元音比预期的要少。根据这些统计数据,参与者在两个相同的辅音字母之前产生的长元音也比预期的要多。我们考虑了这些结果的原因,我们还使用行为数据来测试拼写到声音翻译的两个模型。
{"title":"Number and syllabification of following consonants influence use of long versus short vowels in English disyllables","authors":"Rebecca Treiman,&nbsp;Brett Kessler,&nbsp;Kayla Hensley","doi":"10.1016/j.jml.2022.104399","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.jml.2022.104399","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Spelling-to-sound translation in English is particularly complex for vowels. For example, the pronunciations of ‹a› include the long vowel of ‹p<strong>a</strong>per› and ‹s<strong>a</strong>cred› and the short vowel of ‹c<strong>a</strong>ctus› and ‹h<strong>a</strong>ppy›. We examined the factors that are associated with use of long versus short vowels by conducting analyses of English disyllabic words with single medial consonants and consonant sequences and three behavioral studies in which a total of 119 university students pronounced nonwords with these structures. The vocabulary analyses show that both the number of medial consonants and their syllabification influence vowel length. Participants were influenced by these aspects of context, some of which are not explicitly taught as a part of reading instruction. Although these results point to implicit statistical learning, participants produced fewer long vowels before single medial consonants than anticipated based on our vocabulary statistics for spelling-to-sound correspondences in disyllabic words. Participants also produced more long vowels before two identical consonant letters than anticipated given these statistics. We consider the reasons for these outcomes, and we also use the behavioral data to test two models of spelling-to-sound translation.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":16493,"journal":{"name":"Journal of memory and language","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":4.3,"publicationDate":"2023-02-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC10100579/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9672113","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The episodic encoding of talker voice attributes across diverse voices 不同语音中说话人语音属性的情景编码
IF 4.3 1区 心理学 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2023-02-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.jml.2022.104376
William Clapp , Charlotte Vaughn , Meghan Sumner

In this study, we replicated and extended Experiment 1 of Palmeri et al. (1993) in two experiments. Using the continuous recognition memory paradigm, we investigated effects of a demographically heterogeneous set of talkers varying across race, gender, and regional accent (Exp. 1) and effects of two demographically homogeneous sets of talkers (8 identifiably white male or 8 identifiably Black male talkers) across two listener populations (white and Black listeners) (Exp. 2). Words repeated in the same voice were recognized more quickly and accurately than words repeated in a different voice in both experiments, as found in the original study. This pattern is extremely robust. However, we also found differences across talker conditions, number of voices, lag, false alarms, and d’ that differ from the original study (Exp. 1). In addition, we found effects of talker, talker context, and listener population suggesting that social ideologies and experiences greatly influence the encoding of and memory for spoken words (Exp. 2).

在本研究中,我们在两个实验中复制和扩展了Palmeri等人(1993)的实验1。使用连续识别记忆范式,我们研究了一组因种族、性别和地区口音而异的人口统计学异质说话者的影响(实验1),以及两组人口统计学同质说话者(8名可识别的白人男性或8名可确定的黑人男性说话者)对两个听众群体(白人和黑人听众)的影响(试验2)。在最初的研究中发现,在两个实验中,用同一个声音重复的单词比用不同声音重复的词识别得更快、更准确。这种模式非常稳健。然而,我们也发现了与原始研究不同的说话者条件、声音数量、滞后、误报和d'之间的差异(实验1)。此外,我们发现谈话者、谈话者语境和听众群体的影响表明,社会意识形态和经验对口语的编码和记忆有很大影响(实验2)。
{"title":"The episodic encoding of talker voice attributes across diverse voices","authors":"William Clapp ,&nbsp;Charlotte Vaughn ,&nbsp;Meghan Sumner","doi":"10.1016/j.jml.2022.104376","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.jml.2022.104376","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>In this study, we replicated and extended Experiment 1 of <span>Palmeri et al. (1993)</span> in two experiments. Using the continuous recognition memory paradigm, we investigated effects of a demographically heterogeneous set of talkers varying across race, gender, and regional accent (Exp. 1) and effects of two demographically homogeneous sets of talkers (8 identifiably white male or 8 identifiably Black male talkers) across two listener populations (white and Black listeners) (Exp. 2). Words repeated in the same voice were recognized more quickly and accurately than words repeated in a different voice in both experiments, as found in the original study. This pattern is extremely robust. However, we also found differences across talker conditions, number of voices, lag, false alarms, and d’ that differ from the original study (Exp. 1). In addition, we found effects of talker, talker context, and listener population suggesting that social ideologies and experiences greatly influence the encoding of and memory for spoken words (Exp. 2).</p></div>","PeriodicalId":16493,"journal":{"name":"Journal of memory and language","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":4.3,"publicationDate":"2023-02-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"48099027","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 3
期刊
Journal of memory and language
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1