首页 > 最新文献

Journal of personality and social psychology最新文献

英文 中文
People update their injunctive norm and moral beliefs after receiving descriptive norm information. 人们在接受描述性规范信息后,会更新他们的强制性规范和道德信念。
IF 6.4 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, SOCIAL Pub Date : 2024-11-11 DOI: 10.1037/pspa0000416
Paul Deutchman, Gordon Kraft-Todd, Liane Young, Katherine McAuliffe

How do descriptive norms shape injunctive norm beliefs, and what does this tell us about the cognitive processes underlying social norm cognition? Across six studies (N = 2,671), we examined whether people update their injunctive norm beliefs-as well as their moral judgments and behavioral intentions-after receiving descriptive norm information about how common (or uncommon) a behavior is. Specifically, we manipulated the descriptive normativity of behaviors, describing behaviors as uncommon (20% of people were doing the behavior) or common (80% of people were doing the behavior), and the type of behavior across studies (fairness, conventional, harm, preference). To measure belief updating, we assessed beliefs prior to and after receiving information about the descriptive norm. We had three main findings: First, participants positively updated their prior injunctive norm beliefs, moral judgments, and behavioral intentions (i.e., rated behaviors more injunctively normative and moral) after receiving a common descriptive norm and negatively updated their beliefs (i.e., rated behaviors less injunctive and moral) after receiving an uncommon descriptive norm, and updated to a larger extent for the common than uncommon descriptive norm. Second, participants were more likely to update their beliefs about what is moral for others compared to what is moral for the self. Third, participants updated their beliefs to a greater extent for fairness and conventional behaviors compared to harm behaviors and preferences. Together, our findings suggest that descriptive norms shape our injunctive norm beliefs and moral judgments and help to paint a fuller picture of the social cognition of social norms. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

描述性规范是如何形成强制性规范信念的,这对我们了解社会规范认知的认知过程有何启示?通过六项研究(N = 2,671),我们考察了人们在接收到关于某种行为有多常见(或不常见)的描述性规范信息后,是否会更新他们的强制规范信念--以及他们的道德判断和行为意图。具体来说,我们操纵了行为的描述性规范,将行为描述为不常见(20% 的人在做该行为)或常见(80% 的人在做该行为),并操纵了不同研究中的行为类型(公平、常规、伤害、偏好)。为了测量信念更新情况,我们评估了接受描述性规范信息之前和之后的信念。我们有三个主要发现:首先,参与者在接受了常见的描述性规范后,会积极更新他们之前的强制规范信念、道德判断和行为意图(即对行为的强制规范性和道德性评价更高),而在接受了不常见的描述性规范后,会消极更新他们的信念(即对行为的强制规范性和道德性评价更低)。其次,与对自己的道德评价相比,受试者更倾向于更新对他人的道德评价。第三,与伤害行为和偏好相比,参与者对公平和常规行为的信念更新程度更高。总之,我们的研究结果表明,描述性规范会影响我们的强制规范信念和道德判断,并有助于更全面地描绘社会规范的社会认知。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"People update their injunctive norm and moral beliefs after receiving descriptive norm information.","authors":"Paul Deutchman, Gordon Kraft-Todd, Liane Young, Katherine McAuliffe","doi":"10.1037/pspa0000416","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/pspa0000416","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>How do descriptive norms shape injunctive norm beliefs, and what does this tell us about the cognitive processes underlying social norm cognition? Across six studies (<i>N</i> = 2,671), we examined whether people update their injunctive norm beliefs-as well as their moral judgments and behavioral intentions-after receiving descriptive norm information about how common (or uncommon) a behavior is. Specifically, we manipulated the descriptive normativity of behaviors, describing behaviors as uncommon (20% of people were doing the behavior) or common (80% of people were doing the behavior), and the type of behavior across studies (fairness, conventional, harm, preference). To measure belief updating, we assessed beliefs prior to and after receiving information about the descriptive norm. We had three main findings: First, participants positively updated their prior injunctive norm beliefs, moral judgments, and behavioral intentions (i.e., rated behaviors more injunctively normative and moral) after receiving a common descriptive norm and negatively updated their beliefs (i.e., rated behaviors less injunctive and moral) after receiving an uncommon descriptive norm, and updated to a larger extent for the common than uncommon descriptive norm. Second, participants were more likely to update their beliefs about what is moral for <i>others</i> compared to what is moral for the <i>self</i>. Third, participants updated their beliefs to a greater extent for fairness and conventional behaviors compared to harm behaviors and preferences. Together, our findings suggest that descriptive norms shape our injunctive norm beliefs and moral judgments and help to paint a fuller picture of the social cognition of social norms. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":16691,"journal":{"name":"Journal of personality and social psychology","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":6.4,"publicationDate":"2024-11-11","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142622618","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Extended artificial intelligence aversion: People deny humanness to artificial intelligence users. 扩展的人工智能厌恶:人们否认人工智能用户的人性。
IF 6.4 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, SOCIAL Pub Date : 2024-11-11 DOI: 10.1037/pspi0000480
Jianning Dang, Li Liu

Artificial intelligence (AI) tools are often perceived as lacking humanlike qualities, leading to a preference for human experts over AI assistance. Extending prior research on AI aversion, the current research explores the potential aversion toward those using AI to seek advice. Through eight preregistered studies (total N = 2,317) across multiple AI use scenarios, we found that people denied humanness, especially emotional capacity and human nature traits, to AI advice seekers in comparison to human advice seekers (Studies 1-5 and S1-S3). This is because people perceived less similarity between themselves and AI advice seekers (vs. human advice seekers), with a stronger mediating role of perceived similarity among individuals with greater aversion to AI (Studies 2 and S1). Dehumanization of AI advice seekers predicted less behavioral support for (Study 3) and helping intention toward (Studies S2 and S3) them and could be alleviated through anthropomorphism-related interventions, such as perceiving humanlike qualities in AI or utilizing generative AI (Studies 4 and 5). These findings represent an important theoretical step in advancing research on AI aversion and add to the ongoing discussion on the potential adverse outcomes of AI, focusing on AI users. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

人工智能(AI)工具通常被认为缺乏类似人类的品质,从而导致人们更倾向于使用人类专家而非人工智能辅助工具。目前的研究扩展了之前关于人工智能厌恶的研究,探讨了人们对使用人工智能寻求建议的潜在厌恶。通过对多个人工智能使用场景的八项预先登记研究(总人数 = 2317 人),我们发现,与人类咨询者相比,人们否认人工智能咨询者的人性,尤其是情感能力和人性特征(研究 1-5 和 S1-S3)。这是因为人们认为自己与人工智能建议寻求者(与人类建议寻求者相比)之间的相似性较低,而在对人工智能更反感的个体中,相似性的中介作用更强(研究 2 和 S1)。对人工智能建议寻求者的非人化预示着对他们的行为支持(研究 3)和帮助意向(研究 S2 和 S3)较少,可以通过拟人化相关的干预措施来缓解,如在人工智能中感知类似人类的品质或利用生成式人工智能(研究 4 和 5)。这些研究结果代表了推进人工智能厌恶研究的重要理论步骤,并为正在进行的关于人工智能潜在不良后果的讨论增添了新的内容,其重点是人工智能用户。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Extended artificial intelligence aversion: People deny humanness to artificial intelligence users.","authors":"Jianning Dang, Li Liu","doi":"10.1037/pspi0000480","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/pspi0000480","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Artificial intelligence (AI) tools are often perceived as lacking humanlike qualities, leading to a preference for human experts over AI assistance. Extending prior research on AI aversion, the current research explores the potential aversion toward those using AI to seek advice. Through eight preregistered studies (total <i>N</i> = 2,317) across multiple AI use scenarios, we found that people denied humanness, especially emotional capacity and human nature traits, to AI advice seekers in comparison to human advice seekers (Studies 1-5 and S1-S3). This is because people perceived less similarity between themselves and AI advice seekers (vs. human advice seekers), with a stronger mediating role of perceived similarity among individuals with greater aversion to AI (Studies 2 and S1). Dehumanization of AI advice seekers predicted less behavioral support for (Study 3) and helping intention toward (Studies S2 and S3) them and could be alleviated through anthropomorphism-related interventions, such as perceiving humanlike qualities in AI or utilizing generative AI (Studies 4 and 5). These findings represent an important theoretical step in advancing research on AI aversion and add to the ongoing discussion on the potential adverse outcomes of AI, focusing on AI users. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":16691,"journal":{"name":"Journal of personality and social psychology","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":6.4,"publicationDate":"2024-11-11","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142622563","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The development of personality-From metatraits to facets-Across adolescence and into adulthood in a sample of Mexican-origin youth. 人格的发展从元特征到方面在墨西哥裔青年的样本中,跨越青春期并进入成年。
IF 6.4 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, SOCIAL Pub Date : 2024-06-01 Epub Date: 2023-10-05 DOI: 10.1037/pspp0000487
Whitney R Ringwald, Aleksandra Kaurin, Katherine M Lawson, Aidan G C Wright, Richard W Robins

The time between adolescence and adulthood is a transformative period of development. During these years, youth are exploring work, relationships, and worldviews while gaining the capacities needed to take on adult roles. These social and psychological processes are reflected in how personality develops across this period. Most youth personality development research has focused on the Big Five domains, ignoring the hierarchical structure of personality and missing broader, higher order processes and more specific, lower order processes. Toward a more comprehensive account, this study examines how personality develops from adolescence into the early years of adulthood at the metatrait (stability, plasticity), domain (Big Five), and facet levels. Data come from a longitudinal study of Mexican-origin youth (N = 645) with few socioeconomic resources who were assessed 5 times from Ages 14 to 23. We used latent growth curve models to investigate mean-level change, rank-order consistency, and the maintenance of trajectories for self-reported personality metatraits, domains, and facets. We found distinct developmental processes unfolding at each level of the hierarchy, including (a) mean-level changes in the metatraits and domains indicating increases in exploratory tendencies (i.e., plasticity) and maturity (i.e., increases in agreeableness and conscientiousness, decreases in neuroticism), and divergent change patterns between facets within each domain indicating nuanced maturational processes; (b) comparable levels of rank-order consistency for metatraits, domains, and facets; and (c) evidence that deviations from youth's developmental trajectories did not persist over time. Our findings offer insights into personality development that would be impossible to glean from the domain-level alone and adds needed sociocultural diversity to the literature. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

从青春期到成年期是一个转变性的发展时期。在这些年里,年轻人正在探索工作、人际关系和世界观,同时获得扮演成人角色所需的能力。这些社会和心理过程反映在这一时期的人格发展过程中。大多数青少年人格发展研究都集中在五大领域,忽视了人格的层次结构,错过了更广泛、更高阶的过程和更具体、更低阶的过程。为了更全面地描述,这项研究考察了人格是如何从青春期发展到成年早期的,包括元特质(稳定性、可塑性)、领域(五大)和方面。数据来自一项对墨西哥裔青年(N=645)的纵向研究,他们的社会经济资源很少,从14岁到23岁接受了5次评估。我们使用潜在增长曲线模型来研究自我报告的人格元特征、领域和方面的平均水平变化、等级顺序一致性以及轨迹的维持。我们发现,不同的发展过程在层次结构的每一个层次上展开,包括(a)元特征和领域的平均水平变化,表明探索倾向(即可塑性)和成熟度(即宜人性和尽责性的增加,神经质的降低)的增加,每个领域内各方面之间的差异变化模式表明了微妙的成熟过程;(b) 元性状、领域和方面的等级顺序一致性的可比水平;以及(c)有证据表明,偏离青年的发展轨迹并没有随着时间的推移而持续。我们的发现提供了对个性发展的见解,这是单从领域层面无法收集到的,并为文献增加了所需的社会文化多样性。(PsycInfo数据库记录(c)2023 APA,保留所有权利)。
{"title":"The development of personality-From metatraits to facets-Across adolescence and into adulthood in a sample of Mexican-origin youth.","authors":"Whitney R Ringwald, Aleksandra Kaurin, Katherine M Lawson, Aidan G C Wright, Richard W Robins","doi":"10.1037/pspp0000487","DOIUrl":"10.1037/pspp0000487","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The time between adolescence and adulthood is a transformative period of development. During these years, youth are exploring work, relationships, and worldviews while gaining the capacities needed to take on adult roles. These social and psychological processes are reflected in how personality develops across this period. Most youth personality development research has focused on the Big Five domains, ignoring the hierarchical structure of personality and missing broader, higher order processes and more specific, lower order processes. Toward a more comprehensive account, this study examines how personality develops from adolescence into the early years of adulthood at the metatrait (stability, plasticity), domain (Big Five), and facet levels. Data come from a longitudinal study of Mexican-origin youth (<i>N</i> = 645) with few socioeconomic resources who were assessed 5 times from Ages 14 to 23. We used latent growth curve models to investigate mean-level change, rank-order consistency, and the maintenance of trajectories for self-reported personality metatraits, domains, and facets. We found distinct developmental processes unfolding at each level of the hierarchy, including (a) mean-level changes in the metatraits and domains indicating increases in exploratory tendencies (i.e., plasticity) and maturity (i.e., increases in agreeableness and conscientiousness, decreases in neuroticism), and divergent change patterns between facets within each domain indicating nuanced maturational processes; (b) comparable levels of rank-order consistency for metatraits, domains, and facets; and (c) evidence that deviations from youth's developmental trajectories did not persist over time. Our findings offer insights into personality development that would be impossible to glean from the domain-level alone and adds needed sociocultural diversity to the literature. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":16691,"journal":{"name":"Journal of personality and social psychology","volume":" ","pages":"1140-1160"},"PeriodicalIF":6.4,"publicationDate":"2024-06-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC10995111/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"41159225","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Gender differences and variability in creative ability: A systematic review and meta-analysis of the greater male variability hypothesis in creativity. 创造力的性别差异和变异性:创造力中男性变异性较大假说的系统综述和荟萃分析。
IF 6.4 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, SOCIAL Pub Date : 2024-06-01 Epub Date: 2023-10-05 DOI: 10.1037/pspp0000484
Christa L Taylor, Sameh Said-Metwaly, Anaëlle Camarda, Baptiste Barbot

Society is largely shaped by creativity, making it critical to understand why, despite minimal mean gender differences in creative ability, substantial differences exist in the creative achievement of men and women. Although the greater male variability hypothesis (GMVH) in creativity has been proposed to explain women's underrepresentation as eminent creators, studies examining the GMVH are sparse and limited. This systematic review and meta-analysis were conducted to examine whether the GMVH in creativity can adequately explain the gender gap in creative achievement. We examined the GMVH in creativity, along with mean gender differences, in a range of indicators of creativity and across different sample characteristics and measurement approaches. Effect sizes (k = 1,003) were calculated using information retrieved from 194 studies (N = 68,525). Data were analyzed using three-level meta-analysis and metaregression and publication bias was evaluated using Egger's regression test and contour-enhanced funnel plots. Results revealed minimal gender differences overall, with a slight mean advantage for females (g = -0.10, 95% CI [-0.13, -0.06]) and a trivial variability advantage for males (lnVR = 0.02, 95% CI [0.004, 0.04]) in creative ability scores. However, the magnitude of the effect sizes was moderated by creative domain, task type, scoring type, and study region for mean differences and by country-level gender egalitarianism values for variability. Taken together, gender differences in the mean and variability of creative ability scores are minimal and inconsistent across different contexts, suggesting that the GMVH may not provide much explanatory power for the gender gap in creative achievement. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

社会在很大程度上是由创造力塑造的,因此很难理解为什么尽管创造力的平均性别差异很小,但男性和女性的创造力成就却存在巨大差异。尽管有人提出了创造力中的男性变异性较大假说(GMVH)来解释女性作为杰出创造者的代表性不足,但对GMVH的研究却很少且有限。这项系统综述和荟萃分析旨在检验创造力中的GMVH是否能够充分解释创造力成就中的性别差距。我们在一系列创造力指标以及不同样本特征和测量方法中检查了创造力的GMVH,以及平均性别差异。使用从194项研究(N=68525)中检索的信息计算效果大小(k=1003)。使用三级荟萃分析和元回归分析数据,并使用Egger回归检验和轮廓增强漏斗图评估发表偏倚。结果显示,总体而言,性别差异很小,在创造力得分方面,女性有轻微的平均优势(g=0.10,95%CI[-0.13,-0.06]),男性有轻微的变异优势(lnVR=0.02,95%CI[0.004,0.04])。然而,影响大小的大小受到创意领域、任务类型、评分类型和研究区域的平均差异以及国家层面性别平等主义价值观的可变性的调节。总之,创造力得分的平均值和可变性的性别差异是最小的,并且在不同的背景下是不一致的,这表明GMVH可能不能为创造性成就的性别差距提供太多的解释力。(PsycInfo数据库记录(c)2023 APA,保留所有权利)。
{"title":"Gender differences and variability in creative ability: A systematic review and meta-analysis of the greater male variability hypothesis in creativity.","authors":"Christa L Taylor, Sameh Said-Metwaly, Anaëlle Camarda, Baptiste Barbot","doi":"10.1037/pspp0000484","DOIUrl":"10.1037/pspp0000484","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Society is largely shaped by creativity, making it critical to understand why, despite minimal mean gender differences in creative ability, substantial differences exist in the creative achievement of men and women. Although the greater male variability hypothesis (GMVH) in creativity has been proposed to explain women's underrepresentation as eminent creators, studies examining the GMVH are sparse and limited. This systematic review and meta-analysis were conducted to examine whether the GMVH in creativity can adequately explain the gender gap in creative achievement. We examined the GMVH in creativity, along with mean gender differences, in a range of indicators of creativity and across different sample characteristics and measurement approaches. Effect sizes (<i>k</i> = 1,003) were calculated using information retrieved from 194 studies (<i>N</i> = 68,525). Data were analyzed using three-level meta-analysis and metaregression and publication bias was evaluated using Egger's regression test and contour-enhanced funnel plots. Results revealed minimal gender differences overall, with a slight mean advantage for females (<i>g</i> = -0.10, 95% CI [-0.13, -0.06]) and a trivial variability advantage for males (lnVR = 0.02, 95% CI [0.004, 0.04]) in creative ability scores. However, the magnitude of the effect sizes was moderated by creative domain, task type, scoring type, and study region for mean differences and by country-level gender egalitarianism values for variability. Taken together, gender differences in the mean and variability of creative ability scores are minimal and inconsistent across different contexts, suggesting that the GMVH may not provide much explanatory power for the gender gap in creative achievement. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":16691,"journal":{"name":"Journal of personality and social psychology","volume":" ","pages":"1161-1179"},"PeriodicalIF":6.4,"publicationDate":"2024-06-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"41141311","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The civilian's dilemma: Civilians exhibit automatic defensive responses to the police. 平民的困境:平民对警察表现出自动的防御反应。
IF 6.4 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, SOCIAL Pub Date : 2024-05-01 Epub Date: 2023-10-05 DOI: 10.1037/pspi0000439
Vincenzo J Olivett, David S March

Interactions between police officers and civilians incur for both police and civilians the possibility of danger due to a nonzero likelihood of encountering a physical threat. A body of work examining the implications of threat processes during police-civilian interactions focuses almost exclusively on the perspective of police officers, under the auspice that police use-of-force decisions stem from perceptions and misperceptions of threat (e.g., research on the shooter bias). Almost no research has examined these dynamics from the perspective of civilians whose encounter with police involves interacting with an armed and potentially dangerous individual. In the current work, we advance the idea that just as police may respond to civilians as threats, civilians may respond to the police as threats. That is, among civilians, encountering the police may evoke a combination of defensive bodily and behavioral responses. Across three studies (N = 603) each utilizing unique measures of defensive behavioral and physiological responding, we found that people more rapidly avoid police than nonpolice, demonstrate enhanced defensive freeze responses to police than nonpolice, and evince larger defensive physiological preparation toward police than nonpolice. In light of these patterns, we discuss the implications of defensive responses for shaping civilian behavior in real-world encounters with the police. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

警察和平民之间的互动使警察和平民都有可能面临危险,因为遇到人身威胁的可能性为零。一系列研究警民互动过程中威胁过程影响的工作几乎完全集中在警察的角度,因为警察使用武力的决定源于对威胁的感知和误解(例如,对枪手偏见的研究)。几乎没有任何研究从平民的角度来研究这些动态,因为平民与警察的接触涉及与武装和潜在危险的个人的互动。在目前的工作中,我们提出了这样一种观点,即正如警察可以将平民作为威胁来回应一样,平民也可以将警察作为威胁来应对。也就是说,在平民中,遇到警察可能会引起防御性的身体和行为反应。在三项研究中(N=603),每项研究都利用了独特的防御行为和生理反应措施,我们发现,人们比非虱子更迅速地躲避警察,对警察的防御冷冻反应比无虱子更强,对警察表现出比无虱更大的防御生理准备。根据这些模式,我们讨论了在现实世界中与警察的遭遇中,防御反应对塑造平民行为的影响。(PsycInfo数据库记录(c)2023 APA,保留所有权利)。
{"title":"The civilian's dilemma: Civilians exhibit automatic defensive responses to the police.","authors":"Vincenzo J Olivett, David S March","doi":"10.1037/pspi0000439","DOIUrl":"10.1037/pspi0000439","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Interactions between police officers and civilians incur for both police and civilians the possibility of danger due to a nonzero likelihood of encountering a physical threat. A body of work examining the implications of threat processes during police-civilian interactions focuses almost exclusively on the perspective of police officers, under the auspice that police use-of-force decisions stem from perceptions and misperceptions of threat (e.g., research on the shooter bias). Almost no research has examined these dynamics from the perspective of civilians whose encounter with police involves interacting with an armed and potentially dangerous individual. In the current work, we advance the idea that just as police may respond to civilians as threats, civilians may respond to the police as threats. That is, among civilians, encountering the police may evoke a combination of defensive bodily and behavioral responses. Across three studies (<i>N</i> = 603) each utilizing unique measures of defensive behavioral and physiological responding, we found that people more rapidly avoid police than nonpolice, demonstrate enhanced defensive freeze responses to police than nonpolice, and evince larger defensive physiological preparation toward police than nonpolice. In light of these patterns, we discuss the implications of defensive responses for shaping civilian behavior in real-world encounters with the police. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":16691,"journal":{"name":"Journal of personality and social psychology","volume":" ","pages":"841-851"},"PeriodicalIF":6.4,"publicationDate":"2024-05-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"41149593","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Getting under the skin? Influences of work-family experiences on personality trait adaptation and reciprocal relationships. 深入人心?工作与家庭经历对人格特质适应和互惠关系的影响。
IF 6.4 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, SOCIAL Pub Date : 2024-04-01 Epub Date: 2023-08-10 DOI: 10.1037/pspp0000476
Wen-Dong Li, Jiexin Wang, Tammy Allen, Xin Zhang, Kaili Yu, Hong Zhang, Jason L Huang, Mengqiao Liu, Andrew Li

The literature on personality trait development has mainly focused on influences of life experiences in one single life domain (e.g., work or family) separate from one another and has primarily examined personality development in early life stages. Thus, less attention has been devoted to influences from interplays across different life domains and personality development in middle and late adulthood. Synthesizing the literature on personality science and organizational research, we built a theoretical model and investigated what, how, and why the interplay between two central life domains-work and family-may be related to personality trait development of people at their middle and late life stages, and more important, change-related reciprocal relationships between personality traits and work-family experiences. Generally, convergent findings with data from two longitudinal studies (National Survey of Midlife in the United States, maximum N = 3,192, three waves; and Health and Retirement Study, maximum N = 1,133, three waves except anxiety) revealed that work-to-family conflict, family-to-work conflict, work-to-family facilitation, and family-to-work facilitation mostly had lagged effects on changes of Conscientiousness, Extraversion, and Neuroticism, and the influences were generally channeled through changes of anxiety. Personality traits also had lagged influences on changes of work-family experiences, with some influences deteriorating over time. Change-related reciprocal relationships were recorded mainly between Neuroticism and Extraversion with work-family experiences. Some selection effects were larger than socialization effects. Our research contributes to the personality and the work-family literature and represents a useful example of cross-fertilization of research in different areas of psychology to advance personality research. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

有关人格特质发展的文献主要集中于单个生活领域(如工作或家庭)的生活经历对人格特质发展的影响,并且主要研究生命早期阶段的人格发展。因此,人们较少关注不同生活领域的相互影响以及成年中期和晚期的人格发展。综合人格科学和组织研究方面的文献,我们建立了一个理论模型,并研究了两个核心生活领域--工作和家庭--之间的相互作用与中晚年人的人格特质发展之间的关系,以及更重要的人格特质与工作和家庭经历之间与变化相关的互惠关系。总体而言,与两项纵向研究(美国全国中年调查,最大样本数=3 192,共三波;健康与退休研究,最大样本数=1 133,共三波,焦虑除外)数据的趋同结果显示,工作与家庭的冲突、家庭与工作的冲突、工作对家庭的促进作用以及家庭对工作的促进作用大多对自觉性、外向性和神经质的变化具有滞后效应,而且这些影响一般通过焦虑的变化传导。人格特质对工作-家庭体验变化的影响也是滞后的,有些影响会随着时间的推移而减弱。与变化相关的互惠关系主要存在于神经质和外向性与工作-家庭经历之间。一些选择效应大于社会化效应。我们的研究为人格和工作-家庭文献做出了贡献,也是心理学不同领域研究相互促进以推动人格研究的一个有益范例。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Getting under the skin? Influences of work-family experiences on personality trait adaptation and reciprocal relationships.","authors":"Wen-Dong Li, Jiexin Wang, Tammy Allen, Xin Zhang, Kaili Yu, Hong Zhang, Jason L Huang, Mengqiao Liu, Andrew Li","doi":"10.1037/pspp0000476","DOIUrl":"10.1037/pspp0000476","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The literature on personality trait development has mainly focused on influences of life experiences in one single life domain (e.g., work or family) separate from one another and has primarily examined personality development in early life stages. Thus, less attention has been devoted to influences from interplays across different life domains and personality development in middle and late adulthood. Synthesizing the literature on personality science and organizational research, we built a theoretical model and investigated what, how, and why the interplay between two central life domains-work and family-may be related to personality trait development of people at their middle and late life stages, and more important, change-related reciprocal relationships between personality traits and work-family experiences. Generally, convergent findings with data from two longitudinal studies (National Survey of Midlife in the United States, maximum <i>N</i> = 3,192, three waves; and Health and Retirement Study, maximum <i>N</i> = 1,133, three waves except anxiety) revealed that work-to-family conflict, family-to-work conflict, work-to-family facilitation, and family-to-work facilitation mostly had lagged effects on changes of Conscientiousness, Extraversion, and Neuroticism, and the influences were generally channeled through changes of anxiety. Personality traits also had lagged influences on changes of work-family experiences, with some influences deteriorating over time. Change-related reciprocal relationships were recorded mainly between Neuroticism and Extraversion with work-family experiences. Some selection effects were larger than socialization effects. Our research contributes to the personality and the work-family literature and represents a useful example of cross-fertilization of research in different areas of psychology to advance personality research. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":16691,"journal":{"name":"Journal of personality and social psychology","volume":" ","pages":"694-718"},"PeriodicalIF":6.4,"publicationDate":"2024-04-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9958389","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Dual-promotion: Bragging better by promoting peers. 双重晋升:通过提拔同行更好地吹嘘自己。
IF 6.4 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, SOCIAL Pub Date : 2024-04-01 Epub Date: 2023-08-10 DOI: 10.1037/pspi0000431
Eric M VanEpps, Einav Hart, Maurice E Schweitzer

To create favorable impressions and receive credit, individuals need to share information about their past accomplishments. Broadcasting one's past accomplishments or claiming credit to demonstrate competence, however, can harm perceptions of warmth and likability. In fact, prior work has conceptualized self-promotion as a hydraulic challenge: tactics that boost perceptions along one dimension (e.g., competence) harm perceptions along other dimensions (e.g., warmth). In this work, we identify a novel approach to self-promotion: We show that by combining self-promotion with other-promotion (complimenting or giving credit to others), which we term "dual-promotion," individuals can project both warmth and competence to make better impressions on observers than they do by only self-promoting. In seven preregistered studies, including analyses of annual reports from members of Congress and experiments using social network, workplace, and political contexts (total N = 1,448), we show that individuals who engage in dual-promotion create more favorable impressions of warmth and competence than those who only engage in self-promotion. The beneficial effects of dual-promotion are robust to both competitive and noncompetitive contexts and extend to behavioral intentions. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

为了给人留下好印象并获得荣誉,个人需要分享有关其过去成就的信息。然而,宣传自己过去的成就或声称自己有能力,会损害人们对其热情和可亲程度的感知。事实上,先前的研究已将自我推销概念化为一种水力挑战:在一个维度(如能力)上提升感知的策略会损害在其他维度(如温暖)上的感知。在这项研究中,我们发现了一种新的自我推销方法:我们的研究表明,通过将自我推销与他人推销(赞美他人或给予他人荣誉)相结合(我们称之为 "双重推销"),个体可以同时表现出热情和能力,从而给观察者留下比只进行自我推销更好的印象。在七项预先登记的研究中,包括对国会议员年度报告的分析,以及利用社交网络、工作场所和政治背景进行的实验(总人数 = 1,448),我们表明,与只进行自我推销的人相比,进行双重推销的人更能给人留下热情和能力兼备的好印象。双重宣传的有利效应在竞争性和非竞争性情境中都是稳健的,并能延伸到行为意向中。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Dual-promotion: Bragging better by promoting peers.","authors":"Eric M VanEpps, Einav Hart, Maurice E Schweitzer","doi":"10.1037/pspi0000431","DOIUrl":"10.1037/pspi0000431","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>To create favorable impressions and receive credit, individuals need to share information about their past accomplishments. Broadcasting one's past accomplishments or claiming credit to demonstrate competence, however, can harm perceptions of warmth and likability. In fact, prior work has conceptualized self-promotion as a hydraulic challenge: tactics that boost perceptions along one dimension (e.g., competence) harm perceptions along other dimensions (e.g., warmth). In this work, we identify a novel approach to self-promotion: We show that by combining self-promotion with other-promotion (complimenting or giving credit to others), which we term \"dual-promotion,\" individuals can project both warmth and competence to make better impressions on observers than they do by only self-promoting. In seven preregistered studies, including analyses of annual reports from members of Congress and experiments using social network, workplace, and political contexts (total <i>N</i> = 1,448), we show that individuals who engage in dual-promotion create more favorable impressions of warmth and competence than those who only engage in self-promotion. The beneficial effects of dual-promotion are robust to both competitive and noncompetitive contexts and extend to behavioral intentions. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":16691,"journal":{"name":"Journal of personality and social psychology","volume":" ","pages":"603-623"},"PeriodicalIF":6.4,"publicationDate":"2024-04-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10108877","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Golden gazes: Gaze direction and emotional context promote prosocial behavior by increasing attributions of empathy and perspective-taking. 金色凝视:凝视方向和情感背景通过增加移情和视角转换的归因来促进亲社会行为。
IF 6.4 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, SOCIAL Pub Date : 2024-04-01 Epub Date: 2023-10-26 DOI: 10.1037/pspi0000437
Leticia Micheli, Christina Breil, Anne Böckler

Prosocial behavior is fundamental to societies. But when and toward whom do humans act generously? We investigate the impact of a listeners' gaze direction and the emotional context of the story heard on (a) perceptions of their social cognition skills and (b) prosocial decisions toward them. In three experiments (two preregistered, N = 486), human participants witnessed prerecorded video encounters between a listener (visible) and a speaker (audible, not visible). The listener either established eye contact, averted gaze, or showed a mixed gaze pattern (gaze direction), while the speaker told a neutral or negatively valenced autobiographic episode (emotional context). Participants rated the listeners' empathy and perspective-taking after each video and played the trust game (Study 1) or the dictator game (Study 2) with the listener. Replicating previous findings, occasional gaze avoidance, especially during negative narrations, increased attributions of social understanding to the listener. Critically, mediation analyses revealed that listeners perceived as empathic and taking perspective were ultimately treated with more trust and generosity in strategic and nonstrategic economic games, suggesting that social signals and contextual cues can serve as an indication of another's reputation, thereby promoting indirect reciprocity. Last, in Study 3, we show that emotional context, but not listeners' gaze behavior, promoted the spread of generosity toward anonymous, previously unobserved individuals in a dictator game, driven by social cognition skills attributed to the listener. We conclude that social signals and contextual cues can be important drivers of cooperation in societies via mechanisms such as indirect reciprocity and social contagion of generosity. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

亲社会行为是社会的基础。但是,人类什么时候、对谁慷慨解囊呢?我们调查了听众的凝视方向和听到的故事的情感背景对(a)他们对社会认知技能的感知和(b)对他们的亲社会决策的影响。在三个实验中(两个预先录制,N=486),人类参与者目睹了听众(可见)和扬声器(可听,不可见)之间预先录制的视频相遇。听众要么建立眼神交流,转移视线,要么表现出混合的凝视模式(凝视方向),而演讲者则讲述中性或负面评价的自传体情节(情感背景)。参与者在每段视频后对听众的同理心和视角进行评分,并与听众玩信任游戏(研究1)或独裁者游戏(研究2)。重复先前的发现,偶尔的凝视回避,尤其是在负面叙述中,增加了听众对社会理解的归因。至关重要的是,中介分析显示,在战略和非战略经济游戏中,被认为具有同理心和观点的听众最终会得到更多的信任和慷慨,这表明社会信号和上下文线索可以作为他人声誉的指标,从而促进间接互惠。最后,在研究3中,我们发现,在独裁者游戏中,情感环境,而不是听众的凝视行为,促进了慷慨向匿名的、以前未被注意到的个人的传播,这是由听众的社会认知技能驱动的。我们得出的结论是,社会信号和背景线索可以通过间接互惠和慷慨的社会传染等机制成为社会合作的重要驱动力。(PsycInfo数据库记录(c)2023 APA,保留所有权利)。
{"title":"Golden gazes: Gaze direction and emotional context promote prosocial behavior by increasing attributions of empathy and perspective-taking.","authors":"Leticia Micheli, Christina Breil, Anne Böckler","doi":"10.1037/pspi0000437","DOIUrl":"10.1037/pspi0000437","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Prosocial behavior is fundamental to societies. But when and toward whom do humans act generously? We investigate the impact of a listeners' gaze direction and the emotional context of the story heard on (a) perceptions of their social cognition skills and (b) prosocial decisions toward them. In three experiments (two preregistered, <i>N</i> = 486), human participants witnessed prerecorded video encounters between a listener (visible) and a speaker (audible, not visible). The listener either established eye contact, averted gaze, or showed a mixed gaze pattern (<i>gaze direction</i>), while the speaker told a neutral or negatively valenced autobiographic episode <i>(emotional context</i>). Participants rated the listeners' empathy and perspective-taking after each video and played the trust game (Study 1) or the dictator game (Study 2) with the listener. Replicating previous findings, occasional gaze avoidance, especially during negative narrations, increased attributions of social understanding to the listener. Critically, mediation analyses revealed that listeners perceived as empathic and taking perspective were ultimately treated with more trust and generosity in strategic and nonstrategic economic games, suggesting that social signals and contextual cues can serve as an indication of another's reputation, thereby promoting indirect reciprocity. Last, in Study 3, we show that emotional context, but not listeners' gaze behavior, promoted the spread of generosity toward anonymous, previously unobserved individuals in a dictator game, driven by social cognition skills attributed to the listener. We conclude that social signals and contextual cues can be important drivers of cooperation in societies via mechanisms such as indirect reciprocity and social contagion of generosity. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":16691,"journal":{"name":"Journal of personality and social psychology","volume":" ","pages":"643-659"},"PeriodicalIF":6.4,"publicationDate":"2024-04-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"50161884","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Microinclusions: Treating women as respected work partners increases a sense of fit in technology companies. 微观包容:将女性视为受尊重的工作伙伴,能增强女性与科技公司的契合度。
IF 6.4 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, SOCIAL Pub Date : 2024-03-01 Epub Date: 2023-08-21 DOI: 10.1037/pspi0000430
Gregg A Muragishi, Lauren Aguilar, Priyanka B Carr, Gregory M Walton

When people enter new work settings, we theorized that they are vulnerable to questioning whether they will be received in ways that allow them to contribute to shared goals. If so, treatment that clarifies the stance that others take toward the self, which we call microinclusions, that convey a receptivity and supportiveness to one's contributions may bolster a sense of fit. Further, in examining this question in technology contexts, we theorized that such microinclusions may be particularly impactful for women for whom underrepresentation and negative stereotypes make opportunities to contribute especially fraught. Four primary experiments (N = 1,861, Nwomen in STEM = 1,430) tested this theorizing. In Experiment 1, both men and women at a large technology company anticipated greater fit in a work group described with microinclusions, yet this effect was greatest for women. Experiments 2-4 replicated this effect among women science, technology, engineering, and mathematics (STEM) professionals and college students considering a career in technology and showed that women's anticipated fit arose over and beyond socially warm treatment (Experiment 2); arose more when the microinclusion came from a man (vs. another woman; Experiment 3); and arose even when observing another woman receive a microinclusion (Experiment 4). Microinclusions also increased women's commitment to the company (Experiments 2 and 4) and reduced their anticipated experience of stereotype threat (Experiment 3). This research highlights the ambiguity women face in technology settings about whether they will be received in ways that allow them to contribute to shared work goals and the importance of treatment from coworkers that affirms this opportunity. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

当人们进入新的工作环境时,我们推测他们很容易质疑自己是否会被接纳,从而为共同目标做出贡献。如果是这样,那么澄清他人对自己的态度(我们称之为 "微观包容")、传达对自己贡献的接受和支持的待遇可能会增强适应感。此外,在研究技术背景下的这一问题时,我们推测这种微观包容可能会对女性产生特别大的影响,因为对于女性来说,代表性不足和负面的刻板印象会使她们做出贡献的机会变得尤为渺茫。四个主要实验(N=1,861,STEM 中的女性人数=1,430)验证了这一理论。在实验 1 中,一家大型科技公司的男性和女性都预期在一个被描述为微观排斥的工作小组中会有更大的适应性,但这种效应对女性最大。实验 2-4 在女性科学、技术、工程和数学(STEM)专业人士和考虑从事技术工作的大学生中复制了这一效应,结果表明,女性的预期适合度超过了社会温暖待遇(实验 2);当微排斥来自男性(相对于其他女性;实验 3)时,女性的预期适合度更高,甚至当观察到其他女性接受微排斥时,女性的预期适合度也会提高(实验 4)。微观排斥也增加了女性对公司的承诺(实验 2 和 4),减少了她们对刻板印象威胁的预期体验(实验 3)。这项研究强调了女性在技术环境中面临的模糊性,即她们是否会被接纳,从而为共同的工作目标做出贡献,以及同事肯定这种机会的重要性。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Microinclusions: Treating women as respected work partners increases a sense of fit in technology companies.","authors":"Gregg A Muragishi, Lauren Aguilar, Priyanka B Carr, Gregory M Walton","doi":"10.1037/pspi0000430","DOIUrl":"10.1037/pspi0000430","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>When people enter new work settings, we theorized that they are vulnerable to questioning whether they will be received in ways that allow them to contribute to shared goals. If so, treatment that clarifies the stance that others take toward the self, which we call microinclusions, that convey a receptivity and supportiveness to one's contributions may bolster a sense of fit. Further, in examining this question in technology contexts, we theorized that such microinclusions may be particularly impactful for women for whom underrepresentation and negative stereotypes make opportunities to contribute especially fraught. Four primary experiments (<i>N</i> = 1,861, <i>N</i><sub>women in STEM</sub> = 1,430) tested this theorizing. In Experiment 1, both men and women at a large technology company anticipated greater fit in a work group described with microinclusions, yet this effect was greatest for women. Experiments 2-4 replicated this effect among women science, technology, engineering, and mathematics (STEM) professionals and college students considering a career in technology and showed that women's anticipated fit arose over and beyond socially warm treatment (Experiment 2); arose more when the microinclusion came from a man (vs. another woman; Experiment 3); and arose even when observing another woman receive a microinclusion (Experiment 4). Microinclusions also increased women's commitment to the company (Experiments 2 and 4) and reduced their anticipated experience of stereotype threat (Experiment 3). This research highlights the ambiguity women face in technology settings about whether they will be received in ways that allow them to contribute to shared work goals and the importance of treatment from coworkers that affirms this opportunity. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":16691,"journal":{"name":"Journal of personality and social psychology","volume":" ","pages":"431-460"},"PeriodicalIF":6.4,"publicationDate":"2024-03-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10022943","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Divergent effects of warmth and competence social rejection: An explanation based on the need-threat model. 温暖和能力社会排斥的差异效应:基于需求-威胁模型的解释。
IF 6.4 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, SOCIAL Pub Date : 2024-03-01 Epub Date: 2023-10-05 DOI: 10.1037/pspi0000440
Feifei Chen, Tieyuan Guo, Jian Wang

Based on the need-threat model, we hypothesized that "warmth rejection" threatens belongingness more than "competence rejection," whereas competence rejection threatens sense of efficacy more than warmth rejection. To restore threatened belongingness, warmth (vs. competence) rejection was predicted to result in higher affiliative responses. In contrast, to restore the threatened sense of efficacy, competence (vs. warmth) rejection would lead to higher self-focus. Across six studies, we found that the participants exhibited more affiliative responses after being rejected due to low warmth than due to low competence (Studies 1-6), whereas they became more self-focused after being rejected due to low competence than due to low warmth (Studies 3-6). Furthermore, the effect of warmth rejection on affiliation was mediated by perceived threat to belongingness (Studies 4-6), whereas the effect of competence rejection on self-focus was mediated by perceived threat to control and belongingness (Studies 4-6). The studies provided converging evidence that the effects of social rejection depend on the perception of why rejection occurs. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

基于需求-威胁模型,我们假设“温暖拒绝”比“能力拒绝”更能威胁归属感,而能力拒绝比温暖拒绝更能威胁效能感。为了恢复受威胁的归属感,预测温暖(相对于能力)拒绝会导致更高的依恋反应。相反,为了恢复受到威胁的效能感,能力(相对于温暖)拒绝会导致更高的自我关注。在六项研究中,我们发现参与者在因低热情而被拒绝后比因低能力而被拒绝时表现出更多的依恋反应(研究1-6),而他们在因低能力被拒绝后变得更专注于自我(研究3-6)。此外,温暖拒绝对归属感的影响是由感知的归属威胁介导的(研究4-6),而能力拒绝对自我专注的影响是通过感知的控制威胁和归属感介导的。这些研究提供了一致的证据,证明社会排斥的影响取决于对排斥发生原因的感知。(PsycInfo数据库记录(c)2023 APA,保留所有权利)。
{"title":"Divergent effects of warmth and competence social rejection: An explanation based on the need-threat model.","authors":"Feifei Chen, Tieyuan Guo, Jian Wang","doi":"10.1037/pspi0000440","DOIUrl":"10.1037/pspi0000440","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Based on the need-threat model, we hypothesized that \"warmth rejection\" threatens belongingness more than \"competence rejection,\" whereas competence rejection threatens sense of efficacy more than warmth rejection. To restore threatened belongingness, warmth (vs. competence) rejection was predicted to result in higher affiliative responses. In contrast, to restore the threatened sense of efficacy, competence (vs. warmth) rejection would lead to higher self-focus. Across six studies, we found that the participants exhibited more affiliative responses after being rejected due to low warmth than due to low competence (Studies 1-6), whereas they became more self-focused after being rejected due to low competence than due to low warmth (Studies 3-6). Furthermore, the effect of warmth rejection on affiliation was mediated by perceived threat to belongingness (Studies 4-6), whereas the effect of competence rejection on self-focus was mediated by perceived threat to control and belongingness (Studies 4-6). The studies provided converging evidence that the effects of social rejection depend on the perception of why rejection occurs. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":16691,"journal":{"name":"Journal of personality and social psychology","volume":" ","pages":"461-476"},"PeriodicalIF":6.4,"publicationDate":"2024-03-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"41127555","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Journal of personality and social psychology
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1