首页 > 最新文献

Journal of personality and social psychology最新文献

英文 中文
Of preferences and priors: Motivated reasoning in partisans' evaluations of scientific evidence. 偏好与先验:党派对科学证据评价中的动机推理。
IF 6.4 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, SOCIAL Pub Date : 2024-11-01 DOI: 10.1037/pspa0000417
Jared B Celniker, Peter H Ditto

Despite decades of research, it has been difficult to resolve debates about the existence and nature of partisan bias-the tendency to evaluate information more positively when it supports, rather than challenges, one's political views. Whether partisans display partisan biases, and whether any such biases reflect motivated reasoning, remains contested. We conducted four studies (total N = 4,010) in which participants who made unblinded evaluations of politically relevant science were compared to participants who made blinded evaluations of the same study. The blinded evaluations-judgments of a study's quality given before knowing whether its results were politically congenial-served as impartial benchmarks against which unblinded participants' potentially biased evaluations were compared. We also modeled the influence of partisans' preferences and prior beliefs to test accounts of partisan judgment more stringently than past research. Across our studies, we found evidence of politically motivated reasoning, as unblinded partisans' preferences and prior beliefs independently biased their evaluations. We contend that conceptual confusion between descriptive and normative (e.g., Bayesian) models of political cognition has impeded the resolution of long-standing theoretical debates, and we discuss how our results may help advance more integrative theorizing. We also consider how the blinding paradigm can help researchers address further theoretical disputes (e.g., whether liberals and conservatives are similarly biased), and we discuss the implications of our results for addressing partisan biases within and beyond social science. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

尽管经过了几十年的研究,关于党派偏见的存在和本质的争论一直难以解决——当信息支持而不是挑战一个人的政治观点时,人们倾向于更积极地评价信息。党派人士是否表现出党派偏见,以及这种偏见是否反映了动机推理,这些问题仍存在争议。我们进行了四项研究(总N = 4,010),其中对政治相关科学进行非盲法评估的参与者与对同一研究进行盲法评估的参与者进行比较。盲法评估——在知道研究结果是否具有政治亲和性之前对研究质量做出判断——作为公正的基准,与非盲法参与者的潜在偏见评估进行比较。我们还模拟了党派偏好和先前信念的影响,以比过去的研究更严格地测试党派判断的解释。在我们的研究中,我们发现了政治动机推理的证据,因为无党派人士的偏好和先前的信念独立地影响了他们的评估。我们认为,政治认知的描述性和规范性(如贝叶斯)模型之间的概念混淆阻碍了长期存在的理论争论的解决,我们讨论了我们的结果如何有助于推进更综合的理论化。我们还考虑了盲化范式如何帮助研究人员解决进一步的理论争议(例如,自由派和保守派是否有类似的偏见),并讨论了我们的结果对解决社会科学内外的党派偏见的影响。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA,版权所有)。
{"title":"Of preferences and priors: Motivated reasoning in partisans' evaluations of scientific evidence.","authors":"Jared B Celniker, Peter H Ditto","doi":"10.1037/pspa0000417","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/pspa0000417","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Despite decades of research, it has been difficult to resolve debates about the existence and nature of partisan bias-the tendency to evaluate information more positively when it supports, rather than challenges, one's political views. Whether partisans display partisan biases, and whether any such biases reflect motivated reasoning, remains contested. We conducted four studies (total N = 4,010) in which participants who made unblinded evaluations of politically relevant science were compared to participants who made blinded evaluations of the same study. The blinded evaluations-judgments of a study's quality given before knowing whether its results were politically congenial-served as impartial benchmarks against which unblinded participants' potentially biased evaluations were compared. We also modeled the influence of partisans' preferences and prior beliefs to test accounts of partisan judgment more stringently than past research. Across our studies, we found evidence of politically motivated reasoning, as unblinded partisans' preferences and prior beliefs independently biased their evaluations. We contend that conceptual confusion between descriptive and normative (e.g., Bayesian) models of political cognition has impeded the resolution of long-standing theoretical debates, and we discuss how our results may help advance more integrative theorizing. We also consider how the blinding paradigm can help researchers address further theoretical disputes (e.g., whether liberals and conservatives are similarly biased), and we discuss the implications of our results for addressing partisan biases within and beyond social science. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":16691,"journal":{"name":"Journal of personality and social psychology","volume":"127 5","pages":"986-1011"},"PeriodicalIF":6.4,"publicationDate":"2024-11-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142786136","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The interplay of multiple unconditioned stimuli in evaluative conditioning: A weighted averaging framework for attitude formation via stimulus co-occurrences. 评价性条件反射中多重非条件刺激的相互作用:通过刺激共现形成态度的加权平均框架。
IF 6.4 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, SOCIAL Pub Date : 2024-11-01 Epub Date: 2024-08-29 DOI: 10.1037/pspa0000401
Moritz Ingendahl, Tobias Vogel, Johanna Woitzel, Nike Bücker, Jule Boers, Hans Alves

Evaluative conditioning (EC) is a key effect in attitude formation, leading to changes in the liking of neutral attitude objects due to their pairing with positive or negative stimuli. Despite EC's significance, current theories and most empirical findings are limited to stimulus pairings with a single affective stimulus at a time. In contrast, social environments often involve more complex combinations of affective stimuli. In this article, we introduce a novel framework grounded in information integration research to understand how conditioned attitudes develop in the presence of multiple affective stimuli. Through 10 experiments with different designs, measures, materials, and pairing procedures, we find that individuals' conditioned attitudes follow the average valence of all affective stimuli present with a stronger weighting of negative stimuli. This weighted averaging rule bears two implications for EC in more complex stimulus combinations. First, EC effects are nonmonotonous, such that additional stimuli of the same valence do not produce incremental EC effects. Second, EC effects are interdependent, such that the impact of one stimulus is weakest when accompanied by another negative stimulus and strongest when no other affective stimulus is present. We examine different cognitive processes underlying this weighted averaging rule, including potential differences in pairing memory or changes in the affective stimuli's valence when other stimuli are present. Our findings present a novel theoretical perspective on EC and offer valuable insights into attitude change from stimulus co-occurrences in stimulus-rich environments. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

评价性条件反射(EC)是态度形成过程中的一个关键效应,它通过将中性态度对象与正面或负面刺激配对,导致中性态度对象的喜好发生变化。尽管评价性条件反射具有重要意义,但目前的理论和大多数实证研究结果都仅限于刺激与单一情感刺激的配对。相比之下,社会环境中的情感刺激组合往往更为复杂。在本文中,我们引入了一个基于信息整合研究的新框架,以了解在多重情感刺激下条件态度是如何形成的。通过 10 个不同设计、不同测量方法、不同材料和不同配对程序的实验,我们发现个体的条件态度会跟随所有情感刺激的平均效价,而负面刺激的权重更高。这种加权平均法则对更复杂的刺激组合中的EC有两个影响。首先,EC效应是非单调的,因此额外的相同情绪刺激不会产生递增的EC效应。其次,EC效应是相互依存的,当一个刺激伴随着另一个负面刺激时,EC效应最弱,而当没有其他情感刺激时,EC效应最强。我们研究了这一加权平均规则背后的不同认知过程,包括配对记忆的潜在差异或其他刺激出现时情感刺激价态的变化。我们的研究结果提出了一个关于情感共振的新的理论视角,并为在刺激丰富的环境中通过刺激共现来改变态度提供了有价值的见解。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"The interplay of multiple unconditioned stimuli in evaluative conditioning: A weighted averaging framework for attitude formation via stimulus co-occurrences.","authors":"Moritz Ingendahl, Tobias Vogel, Johanna Woitzel, Nike Bücker, Jule Boers, Hans Alves","doi":"10.1037/pspa0000401","DOIUrl":"10.1037/pspa0000401","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Evaluative conditioning (EC) is a key effect in attitude formation, leading to changes in the liking of neutral attitude objects due to their pairing with positive or negative stimuli. Despite EC's significance, current theories and most empirical findings are limited to stimulus pairings with a single affective stimulus at a time. In contrast, social environments often involve more complex combinations of affective stimuli. In this article, we introduce a novel framework grounded in information integration research to understand how conditioned attitudes develop in the presence of multiple affective stimuli. Through 10 experiments with different designs, measures, materials, and pairing procedures, we find that individuals' conditioned attitudes follow the average valence of all affective stimuli present with a stronger weighting of negative stimuli. This weighted averaging rule bears two implications for EC in more complex stimulus combinations. First, EC effects are nonmonotonous, such that additional stimuli of the same valence do not produce incremental EC effects. Second, EC effects are interdependent, such that the impact of one stimulus is weakest when accompanied by another negative stimulus and strongest when no other affective stimulus is present. We examine different cognitive processes underlying this weighted averaging rule, including potential differences in pairing memory or changes in the affective stimuli's valence when other stimuli are present. Our findings present a novel theoretical perspective on EC and offer valuable insights into attitude change from stimulus co-occurrences in stimulus-rich environments. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":16691,"journal":{"name":"Journal of personality and social psychology","volume":" ","pages":"964-985"},"PeriodicalIF":6.4,"publicationDate":"2024-11-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142108496","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Individual differences in the forms of personality trait trajectories. 个性特征轨迹形式的个体差异。
IF 6.4 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, SOCIAL Pub Date : 2024-11-01 Epub Date: 2024-08-22 DOI: 10.1037/pspp0000520
Amanda J Wright, Joshua J Jackson

Changes in personality are often modeled linearly or curvilinearly. It is a simplifying-yet untested-assumption that the chosen sample-level model form accurately depicts all person-level trajectories within the sample. Given the complexity of personality development, it seems unlikely that imposing a single model form across all individuals is appropriate. Although typical growth models can estimate individual trajectories that deviate from the average via random effects, they do not explicitly test whether people differ in the forms of their trajectories. This heterogeneity is valuable to uncover, though, as it may imply that different processes are driving change. The present study uses data from four longitudinal data sets (N = 26,469; Mage = 47.55) to empirically test the degree that people vary in best-fitting model forms for their Big Five personality development. Across data sets, there was substantial heterogeneity in best-fitting forms. Moreover, the type of form someone had was directly associated with their net and total amount of change across time, and these changes were substantially misquantified when a worse-fitting form was used. Variables such as gender, age, trait levels, and number of waves were also associated with people's types of forms. Lastly, comparisons of best-fitting forms from individual- and sample-level models indicated that consequential discrepancies arise from different levels of analysis (i.e., individual vs. sample) and alternative modeling choices (e.g., choice of time metric). Our findings highlight the importance of these individual differences for understanding personality change processes and suggest that a flexible, person-level approach to understanding personality development is necessary. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

人格变化通常以线性或曲线方式建模。一个简化但未经验证的假设是,所选择的样本水平模型形式能准确描述样本中所有个人水平的轨迹。鉴于人格发展的复杂性,在所有个体中强加一种单一的模型形式似乎并不合适。虽然典型的成长模型可以通过随机效应估算出偏离平均值的个体轨迹,但它们并不能明确检验出人们的轨迹形式是否存在差异。不过,这种异质性的揭示很有价值,因为它可能意味着不同的过程在推动变化。本研究利用四个纵向数据集(N = 26,469; Mage = 47.55)的数据,实证检验了人们在大五人格发展的最佳拟合模型形式上的差异程度。在所有数据集中,最佳拟合形式存在很大的异质性。此外,一个人所拥有的模型类型与其在不同时期的净变化量和总变化量直接相关,而当使用拟合度较差的模型时,这些变化会被严重误量化。性别、年龄、特质水平和波数等变量也与人们的表格类型有关。最后,对个人和样本模型的最佳拟合形式进行比较表明,不同的分析水平(即个人与样本)和不同的建模选择(如时间度量的选择)会产生相应的差异。我们的研究结果凸显了这些个体差异对于理解人格变化过程的重要性,并表明有必要采用灵活的、个人层面的方法来理解人格发展。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Individual differences in the forms of personality trait trajectories.","authors":"Amanda J Wright, Joshua J Jackson","doi":"10.1037/pspp0000520","DOIUrl":"10.1037/pspp0000520","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Changes in personality are often modeled linearly or curvilinearly. It is a simplifying-yet untested-assumption that the chosen sample-level model form accurately depicts all person-level trajectories within the sample. Given the complexity of personality development, it seems unlikely that imposing a single model form across all individuals is appropriate. Although typical growth models can estimate individual trajectories that deviate from the average via random effects, they do not explicitly test whether people differ in the forms of their trajectories. This heterogeneity is valuable to uncover, though, as it may imply that different processes are driving change. The present study uses data from four longitudinal data sets (<i>N</i> = 26,469; <i>M</i><sub>age</sub> = 47.55) to empirically test the degree that people vary in best-fitting model forms for their Big Five personality development. Across data sets, there was substantial heterogeneity in best-fitting forms. Moreover, the type of form someone had was directly associated with their net and total amount of change across time, and these changes were substantially misquantified when a worse-fitting form was used. Variables such as gender, age, trait levels, and number of waves were also associated with people's types of forms. Lastly, comparisons of best-fitting forms from individual- and sample-level models indicated that consequential discrepancies arise from different levels of analysis (i.e., individual vs. sample) and alternative modeling choices (e.g., choice of time metric). Our findings highlight the importance of these individual differences for understanding personality change processes and suggest that a flexible, person-level approach to understanding personality development is necessary. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":16691,"journal":{"name":"Journal of personality and social psychology","volume":" ","pages":"1062-1088"},"PeriodicalIF":6.4,"publicationDate":"2024-11-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142017845","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Asian = machine, Black = animal? The racial asymmetry of dehumanization. 亚洲人=机器,黑人=动物?非人化的种族不对称。
IF 6.4 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, SOCIAL Pub Date : 2024-11-01 Epub Date: 2024-07-11 DOI: 10.1037/pspi0000455
Hui Bai, Xian Zhao

How different racial minorities experience racism differently remains underexplored in existing research. Here, we show that Asian and Black people are often dehumanized differently. Twelve studies spotlight a racial asymmetry in dehumanization using a wide array of methods (experimental, archival, and computational) and data sources (online samples, word embeddings, and U.S. Bureau of Labor Statistics data): Whereas Black people are more often subjected to animalistic dehumanization, Asian people are predominantly subjected to mechanistic dehumanization. We demonstrate this asymmetry from the vantage point of victims (Studies 1a and 1b) and perpetrators (Studies 2a-2d). We further document the prevalence of this asymmetry across diverse domains, from everyday language (Study 3) to perceptions in the realms of romantic relationships (Study 4a), crime rates (Study 4b), and business skills (Study 4c). Finally, we demonstrate the asymmetry's real-world consequences in labor market segregation (Studies 5 and 6). Our findings shed light on the distinct experiences of racism encountered by different racial groups and, more critically, introduce a framework that unifies and integrates scattered empirical observations on perceptions of Asian people. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

在现有研究中,对不同少数种族如何以不同方式经历种族主义的探讨仍然不足。在这里,我们展示了亚裔和黑人经常被非人化的不同情况。十二项研究采用多种方法(实验法、档案法和计算法)和数据源(在线样本、词嵌入和美国劳工统计局数据),揭示了非人化中的种族不对称现象:黑人更常遭受动物性非人化,而亚裔则主要遭受机械性非人化。我们从受害者(研究 1a 和 1b)和施暴者(研究 2a-2d)的视角证明了这种不对称。我们进一步记录了这种不对称在不同领域的普遍存在,从日常用语(研究 3)到恋爱关系(研究 4a)、犯罪率(研究 4b)和商业技能(研究 4c)等领域的认知。最后,我们展示了不对称在劳动力市场隔离方面的现实后果(研究 5 和 6)。我们的研究结果揭示了不同种族群体遭遇种族主义的不同经历,更重要的是,我们引入了一个框架,统一并整合了对亚裔人看法的零散经验观察。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Asian = machine, Black = animal? The racial asymmetry of dehumanization.","authors":"Hui Bai, Xian Zhao","doi":"10.1037/pspi0000455","DOIUrl":"10.1037/pspi0000455","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>How different racial minorities experience racism differently remains underexplored in existing research. Here, we show that Asian and Black people are often dehumanized differently. Twelve studies spotlight a racial asymmetry in dehumanization using a wide array of methods (experimental, archival, and computational) and data sources (online samples, word embeddings, and U.S. Bureau of Labor Statistics data): Whereas Black people are more often subjected to animalistic dehumanization, Asian people are predominantly subjected to mechanistic dehumanization. We demonstrate this asymmetry from the vantage point of victims (Studies 1a and 1b) and perpetrators (Studies 2a-2d). We further document the prevalence of this asymmetry across diverse domains, from everyday language (Study 3) to perceptions in the realms of romantic relationships (Study 4a), crime rates (Study 4b), and business skills (Study 4c). Finally, we demonstrate the asymmetry's real-world consequences in labor market segregation (Studies 5 and 6). Our findings shed light on the distinct experiences of racism encountered by different racial groups and, more critically, introduce a framework that unifies and integrates scattered empirical observations on perceptions of Asian people. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":16691,"journal":{"name":"Journal of personality and social psychology","volume":" ","pages":"1038-1061"},"PeriodicalIF":6.4,"publicationDate":"2024-11-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141590576","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Thinking in 3D: A multidimensional mapping of the effects of distance on abstraction. 三维思维:距离对抽象的影响的多维映射。
IF 6.4 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, SOCIAL Pub Date : 2024-11-01 DOI: 10.1037/pspa0000424
Avi Gamoran, Britt Hadar, Michael Gilead

Despite a large body of research concerning the effects of psychological distance, our understanding about how different dimensions of distance interact and influence cognition is still limited. In this study, we moved beyond first-order approximations of the effects of psychological distance, to map the effects of multidimensional events as they appear in the world. We developed a novel experimental idiographic paradigm in which participants were asked to generate narratives of events. We simultaneously manipulated the prompts to adjust the perceived proximity in three dimensions of psychological distance, according to what individuals consider to be close to (or far from) them. Additionally, we trained an algorithm to identify the distances depicted in these narratives. Consistent with construal level theory, the results of our large-sample, preregistered analyses revealed that an increase in distance, irrespective of its type, led to more abstract representations and that experimentally manipulating distance on one dimension led to increased distance on the other dimensions. This was true for both traditional measures of linguistic abstraction and memory semanticization measures that quantify the amount of episodic detail. Results show that the effect of distance on abstraction was consistent across its various dimensions, confirming a monotonic and additive (i.e., linear) relationship. This sheds light on the mechanisms whereby psychological distance affects our thought and paves the way for more refined, integrative models of how our minds construct possible futures and alternative realities. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

尽管有大量关于心理距离影响的研究,但我们对不同维度的距离如何相互作用和影响认知的理解仍然有限。在这项研究中,我们超越了对心理距离影响的一阶近似,描绘了世界上出现的多维事件的影响。我们开发了一种新颖的实验具体范式,要求参与者对事件进行叙述。我们同时操纵提示,根据个人认为离他们近(或远)的程度,在心理距离的三个维度上调整感知到的接近度。此外,我们训练了一个算法来识别这些叙述中描述的距离。与解释水平理论一致,我们的大样本、预登记分析的结果显示,距离的增加,无论其类型如何,都会导致更抽象的表征,而实验上操纵一个维度上的距离会导致其他维度上的距离增加。对于语言抽象的传统测量和量化情景细节数量的记忆语义测量都是如此。结果表明,距离对抽象的影响在其各个维度上是一致的,证实了单调和加性(即线性)关系。这揭示了心理距离影响我们思维的机制,并为我们的大脑如何构建可能的未来和可选择的现实的更精细、更综合的模型铺平了道路。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA,版权所有)。
{"title":"Thinking in 3D: A multidimensional mapping of the effects of distance on abstraction.","authors":"Avi Gamoran, Britt Hadar, Michael Gilead","doi":"10.1037/pspa0000424","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/pspa0000424","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Despite a large body of research concerning the effects of psychological distance, our understanding about how different dimensions of distance interact and influence cognition is still limited. In this study, we moved beyond first-order approximations of the effects of psychological distance, to map the effects of multidimensional events as they appear in the world. We developed a novel experimental idiographic paradigm in which participants were asked to generate narratives of events. We simultaneously manipulated the prompts to adjust the perceived proximity in three dimensions of psychological distance, according to what individuals consider to be close to (or far from) them. Additionally, we trained an algorithm to identify the distances depicted in these narratives. Consistent with construal level theory, the results of our large-sample, preregistered analyses revealed that an increase in distance, irrespective of its type, led to more abstract representations and that experimentally manipulating distance on one dimension led to increased distance on the other dimensions. This was true for both traditional measures of linguistic abstraction and memory semanticization measures that quantify the amount of episodic detail. Results show that the effect of distance on abstraction was consistent across its various dimensions, confirming a monotonic and additive (i.e., linear) relationship. This sheds light on the mechanisms whereby psychological distance affects our thought and paves the way for more refined, integrative models of how our minds construct possible futures and alternative realities. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":16691,"journal":{"name":"Journal of personality and social psychology","volume":"127 5","pages":"949-963"},"PeriodicalIF":6.4,"publicationDate":"2024-11-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142786155","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The directed nature of social stereotypes. 社会定型观念的定向性。
IF 6.4 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, SOCIAL Pub Date : 2024-10-31 DOI: 10.1037/pspa0000425
Oliver Sng, Minyoung Choi, Keelah E G Williams, Rebecca Neel

Stereotypes are strategically complex. We propose that people hold not just stereotypes about what groups are generally like (e.g., "men are competitive") but stereotypes about how groups behave toward specific groups (e.g., "men are competitive toward")-what we call directed stereotypes. Across studies, we find that perceivers indeed hold directed stereotypes. Four studies examine directed stereotypes of sex and age (Studies 1 and 2; N = 541) and of race/ethnicity (of Asian/Black/Latino/White Americans; Studies 3 and 4; N = 769), with a focus on stereotypes of competitiveness, aggressiveness, cooperativeness, and communion. Across studies, directed stereotypes present unique patterns that both qualify and reverse well-documented stereotype patterns in the literature. For example, men are typically stereotyped as more competitive than women. However, directed stereotypes show that women are stereotyped to be more competitive than men, when this competitiveness is directed toward young women. Multiple such patterns emerge in the current data, across sex, age, and racial/ethnic stereotypes. Directed stereotypes also uniquely predict intergroup attitudes, over and above general stereotypes (Study 4). The idea of directed stereotypes is compatible with multiple theoretical perspectives and intuitive. However, they have been unexamined. We discuss the implications of the current work for thinking about the nature and measurement of social stereotypes, stereotype content, and social perception more broadly. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

刻板印象在战略上是复杂的。我们认为,人们持有的刻板印象不仅是关于群体一般是什么样的(例如,"男人争强好胜"),而且是关于群体如何对待特定群体的刻板印象(例如,"男人争强好胜")--我们称之为定向刻板印象。在各项研究中,我们发现感知者确实持有定向刻板印象。四项研究考察了性别和年龄的定向刻板印象(研究 1 和 2;N = 541)以及种族/民族的定向刻板印象(亚裔/黑人/拉美裔/美国白人;研究 3 和 4;N = 769),重点关注竞争性、攻击性、合作性和共融性的刻板印象。在所有的研究中,定向刻板印象呈现出独特的模式,这些模式既限定了文献中记载的刻板印象模式,也逆转了文献中记载的刻板印象模式。例如,男性通常被刻板印象为比女性更具竞争力。然而,定向刻板印象显示,当这种竞争性是针对年轻女性时,女性被刻板地认为比男性更具竞争性。目前的数据中出现了多种这样的模式,跨越了性别、年龄和种族/民族的刻板印象。在一般刻板印象之外,定向刻板印象还能独特地预测群体间态度(研究 4)。定向刻板印象的观点符合多种理论观点和直观性。然而,这些观点一直未得到研究。我们将讨论当前研究对社会刻板印象的性质和测量、刻板印象的内容以及更广泛的社会认知的影响。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved)。
{"title":"The directed nature of social stereotypes.","authors":"Oliver Sng, Minyoung Choi, Keelah E G Williams, Rebecca Neel","doi":"10.1037/pspa0000425","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/pspa0000425","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Stereotypes are strategically complex. We propose that people hold not just stereotypes about what groups are generally like (e.g., \"men are competitive\") but stereotypes about how groups behave toward specific groups (e.g., \"men are competitive toward\")-what we call <i>directed stereotypes.</i> Across studies, we find that perceivers indeed hold directed stereotypes. Four studies examine directed stereotypes of sex and age (Studies 1 and 2; <i>N</i> = 541) and of race/ethnicity (of Asian/Black/Latino/White Americans; Studies 3 and 4; <i>N</i> = 769), with a focus on stereotypes of competitiveness, aggressiveness, cooperativeness, and communion. Across studies, directed stereotypes present unique patterns that both qualify and reverse well-documented stereotype patterns in the literature. For example, men are typically stereotyped as more competitive than women. However, directed stereotypes show that women are stereotyped to be more competitive than men, when this competitiveness is directed toward young women. Multiple such patterns emerge in the current data, across sex, age, and racial/ethnic stereotypes. Directed stereotypes also uniquely predict intergroup attitudes, over and above general stereotypes (Study 4). The idea of directed stereotypes is compatible with multiple theoretical perspectives and intuitive. However, they have been unexamined. We discuss the implications of the current work for thinking about the nature and measurement of social stereotypes, stereotype content, and social perception more broadly. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":16691,"journal":{"name":"Journal of personality and social psychology","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":6.4,"publicationDate":"2024-10-31","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142546068","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Genetic and environmental contributions to adult attachment styles: Evidence from the Minnesota Twin Registry. 遗传和环境对成人依恋风格的影响:来自明尼苏达双胞胎登记处的证据。
IF 6.4 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, SOCIAL Pub Date : 2024-10-31 DOI: 10.1037/pspp0000516
Keely A Dugan, Jacob J Kunkel, R Chris Fraley, D A Briley, Matt McGue, Robert F Krueger, Glenn I Roisman

Attachment theory, as originally outlined by Bowlby (1973, 1980, 1969/1982), suggests that the ways people think, feel, and behave in close relationships are shaped by the dynamic interplay between their genes and their social environment. Research on adult attachment, however, has largely focused on the latter, providing only a partial picture of how attachment styles emerge and develop throughout life. The present research leveraged data from the Minnesota Twin Registry, a large sample of older adult twins (N = 1,377 twins; 678 pairs; Mage = 70.40 years, SD = 5.42), to examine the relative contributions of genetic and environmental factors to adult attachment styles. Participants reported on both their general attachment styles and relationship-specific attachments to their mothers, fathers, partners, and best friends. The results suggest that attachment styles are partly heritable (∼36%) and partly attributable to environmental factors that are not shared between twins (∼64%). Heritability estimates were somewhat higher for parent-specific attachment styles (∼51%), whereas nonshared environmental factors accounted for larger proportions of the variance in partner- and best friend-specific attachment styles. Using multivariate biometric models, we also examined the genetic and environmental factors underlying the covariation among people's relationship-specific attachment styles. The findings indicate that the similarities among people's avoidant tendencies in different relationships can be explained by a single, higher order latent factor (e.g., global avoidance). In contrast, the genetic and environmental factors underlying attachment anxiety appear to be more differentiated across specific close relationships. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

鲍尔比(Bowlby,1973 年、1980 年、1969 年/1982 年)最初提出的依恋理论认为,人们在亲密关系中的思维、感觉和行为方式是由其基因和社会环境之间的动态相互作用形成的。然而,对成人依恋关系的研究主要集中在后者,只能提供依恋风格如何在一生中出现和发展的部分情况。本研究利用明尼苏达双胞胎登记处(Minnesota Twin Registry)的数据(样本数=1,377对双胞胎,678对;年龄=70.40岁,标准差=5.42),对遗传和环境因素对成人依恋风格的相对贡献进行了研究。参与者报告了他们的一般依恋风格,以及与母亲、父亲、伴侣和好友的特定关系依恋。结果表明,依恋风格部分是遗传的(∼36%),部分归因于双胞胎之间不共享的环境因素(∼64%)。父母特异性依恋风格的遗传估计值略高(∼51%),而非共享环境因素在伴侣和挚友特异性依恋风格的变异中占较大比例。利用多变量生物计量模型,我们还研究了人们在特定关系中依恋方式的共变性背后的遗传和环境因素。研究结果表明,人们在不同关系中的回避倾向的相似性可以用一个单一的高阶潜在因素(如整体回避)来解释。相比之下,在特定的亲密关系中,依恋焦虑的遗传和环境因素似乎更具差异性。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Genetic and environmental contributions to adult attachment styles: Evidence from the Minnesota Twin Registry.","authors":"Keely A Dugan, Jacob J Kunkel, R Chris Fraley, D A Briley, Matt McGue, Robert F Krueger, Glenn I Roisman","doi":"10.1037/pspp0000516","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/pspp0000516","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Attachment theory, as originally outlined by Bowlby (1973, 1980, 1969/1982), suggests that the ways people think, feel, and behave in close relationships are shaped by the dynamic interplay between their genes and their social environment. Research on adult attachment, however, has largely focused on the latter, providing only a partial picture of how attachment styles emerge and develop throughout life. The present research leveraged data from the Minnesota Twin Registry, a large sample of older adult twins (<i>N</i> = 1,377 twins; 678 pairs; <i>M</i><sub>age</sub> = 70.40 years, <i>SD</i> = 5.42), to examine the relative contributions of genetic and environmental factors to adult attachment styles. Participants reported on both their <i>general</i> attachment styles and <i>relationship-specific</i> attachments to their mothers, fathers, partners, and best friends. The results suggest that attachment styles are partly heritable (∼36%) and partly attributable to environmental factors that are not shared between twins (∼64%). Heritability estimates were somewhat higher for parent-specific attachment styles (∼51%), whereas nonshared environmental factors accounted for larger proportions of the variance in partner- and best friend-specific attachment styles. Using multivariate biometric models, we also examined the genetic and environmental factors underlying the covariation among people's relationship-specific attachment styles. The findings indicate that the similarities among people's avoidant tendencies in different relationships can be explained by a single, higher order latent factor (e.g., global avoidance). In contrast, the genetic and environmental factors underlying attachment anxiety appear to be more differentiated across specific close relationships. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":16691,"journal":{"name":"Journal of personality and social psychology","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":6.4,"publicationDate":"2024-10-31","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142546067","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Institutions and cooperation: A meta-analysis of structural features in social dilemmas. 制度与合作:社会困境中的结构特征元分析。
IF 7.6 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, SOCIAL Pub Date : 2024-10-24 DOI: 10.1037/pspi0000474
Shuxian Jin,Giuliana Spadaro,Daniel Balliet
Cooperation underlies the ability of groups to realize collective benefits (e.g., creation of public goods). Yet, cooperation can be difficult to achieve when people face situations with conflicting interests between what is best for individuals versus the collective (i.e., social dilemmas). To address this challenge, groups can implement rules about structural changes in a situation. But what institutional rules can best facilitate cooperation? Theoretically, rules can be made to affect structural features of a social dilemma, such as the possible actions, outcomes, and people involved. We derived 13 preregistered hypotheses from existing work and collected 6 decades of empirical research to test how nine structural features influence cooperation within prisoner's dilemmas and public goods dilemmas. We do this by meta-analyzing mean levels of cooperation across studies (Study 1, k = 2,340, N = 229,528), and also examining how manipulations of these structural features in social dilemmas affect cooperation within studies (Study 2, k = 909). Results indicated that lower conflict of interests was associated with higher cooperation and that (a) the implementation of sanctions (i.e., reward and punishment of behaviors) and (b) allowing for communication most strongly enhanced cooperation. However, we found inconsistent support for the hypotheses that group size and matching design affect cooperation. Other structural features (e.g., symmetry of dilemmas, sequential decision making, payment) were not associated with cooperation. Overall, these findings inform institutions that can (or not) facilitate cooperation. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).
合作是群体实现集体利益(如创造公共产品)的基础。然而,当人们面临个人利益与集体利益发生冲突时(即社会困境),合作就很难实现。为了应对这一挑战,群体可以实施有关结构性变化的规则。但什么样的制度规则最能促进合作呢?从理论上讲,规则可以影响社会困境的结构特征,如可能的行动、结果和参与人员。我们从现有研究中推导出 13 个预先登记的假设,并收集了 60 年来的实证研究,以检验九个结构特征如何影响囚徒困境和公共物品困境中的合作。为此,我们对不同研究的平均合作水平进行了元分析(研究 1,k = 2,340, N = 229,528),同时还考察了在社会两难中操纵这些结构特征如何影响研究内部的合作(研究 2,k = 909)。结果表明,较低的利益冲突与较高的合作度相关,(a) 实施制裁(即对行为的奖励和惩罚)和(b) 允许交流最能促进合作。然而,我们发现小组规模和匹配设计影响合作的假设并不一致。其他结构特征(如困境的对称性、顺序决策、支付)与合作无关。总之,这些发现为能够(或不能)促进合作的机构提供了信息。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved)。
{"title":"Institutions and cooperation: A meta-analysis of structural features in social dilemmas.","authors":"Shuxian Jin,Giuliana Spadaro,Daniel Balliet","doi":"10.1037/pspi0000474","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/pspi0000474","url":null,"abstract":"Cooperation underlies the ability of groups to realize collective benefits (e.g., creation of public goods). Yet, cooperation can be difficult to achieve when people face situations with conflicting interests between what is best for individuals versus the collective (i.e., social dilemmas). To address this challenge, groups can implement rules about structural changes in a situation. But what institutional rules can best facilitate cooperation? Theoretically, rules can be made to affect structural features of a social dilemma, such as the possible actions, outcomes, and people involved. We derived 13 preregistered hypotheses from existing work and collected 6 decades of empirical research to test how nine structural features influence cooperation within prisoner's dilemmas and public goods dilemmas. We do this by meta-analyzing mean levels of cooperation across studies (Study 1, k = 2,340, N = 229,528), and also examining how manipulations of these structural features in social dilemmas affect cooperation within studies (Study 2, k = 909). Results indicated that lower conflict of interests was associated with higher cooperation and that (a) the implementation of sanctions (i.e., reward and punishment of behaviors) and (b) allowing for communication most strongly enhanced cooperation. However, we found inconsistent support for the hypotheses that group size and matching design affect cooperation. Other structural features (e.g., symmetry of dilemmas, sequential decision making, payment) were not associated with cooperation. Overall, these findings inform institutions that can (or not) facilitate cooperation. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).","PeriodicalId":16691,"journal":{"name":"Journal of personality and social psychology","volume":"15 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":7.6,"publicationDate":"2024-10-24","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142490830","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The bigger the problem the littler: When the scope of a problem makes it seem less dangerous. 问题越大越小:问题的范围越大,危险性就越小。
IF 7.6 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, SOCIAL Pub Date : 2024-10-24 DOI: 10.1037/pspa0000409
Lauren Eskreis-Winkler,Luiza Tanoue Troncoso Peres,Ayelet Fishbach
Across 15 studies (N = 2,636), people who considered the prevalence of a problem (e.g., 4.2 million people drive drunk each month) inferred it caused less harm, a phenomenon we dub the big problem paradox. People believed dire problems-ranging from poverty to drunk driving-were less problematic upon learning the number of people they affect (Studies 1-2). Prevalence information caused medical experts to infer medication nonadherence was less dangerous, just as it led women to underestimate their true risk of contracting cancer. The big problem paradox results from an optimistic view of the world. When people believe the world is good, they assume widespread problems have been addressed and, thus, cause less harm (Studies 3-4). The big problem paradox has key implications for motivation and helping behavior (Studies 5-6). Learning the prevalence of medical conditions (i.e., chest pain, suicidal ideation) led people to think a symptomatic individual was less sick and, as a result, to help less-in violation of clinical guidelines. The finding that scale warps judgments and de-motivates action is of particular relevance in the globalized 21st century. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).
在 15 项研究(N = 2,636)中,考虑到问题的普遍性(例如,每月有 420 万人酒后驾车),人们推断该问题造成的伤害较小,我们将这种现象称为 "大问题悖论"。从贫困到酒后驾车,人们在了解了这些问题的影响人数后,就会认为它们的危害较小(研究 1-2)。患病率信息导致医学专家推断不遵医嘱用药的危险性较低,就像它导致妇女低估了自己患癌症的真实风险一样。大问题悖论源于对世界的乐观看法。当人们认为世界是美好的,他们就会认为普遍存在的问题已经得到解决,因此造成的伤害也会减少(研究 3-4)。大问题悖论对动机和助人行为有重要影响(研究 5-6)。了解医疗状况(如胸痛、自杀倾向)的普遍性会让人们认为有症状的人病情较轻,从而减少帮助,这违反了临床指南。在全球化的 21 世纪,量表会扭曲判断并降低行动的积极性,这一发现具有特别重要的现实意义。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"The bigger the problem the littler: When the scope of a problem makes it seem less dangerous.","authors":"Lauren Eskreis-Winkler,Luiza Tanoue Troncoso Peres,Ayelet Fishbach","doi":"10.1037/pspa0000409","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/pspa0000409","url":null,"abstract":"Across 15 studies (N = 2,636), people who considered the prevalence of a problem (e.g., 4.2 million people drive drunk each month) inferred it caused less harm, a phenomenon we dub the big problem paradox. People believed dire problems-ranging from poverty to drunk driving-were less problematic upon learning the number of people they affect (Studies 1-2). Prevalence information caused medical experts to infer medication nonadherence was less dangerous, just as it led women to underestimate their true risk of contracting cancer. The big problem paradox results from an optimistic view of the world. When people believe the world is good, they assume widespread problems have been addressed and, thus, cause less harm (Studies 3-4). The big problem paradox has key implications for motivation and helping behavior (Studies 5-6). Learning the prevalence of medical conditions (i.e., chest pain, suicidal ideation) led people to think a symptomatic individual was less sick and, as a result, to help less-in violation of clinical guidelines. The finding that scale warps judgments and de-motivates action is of particular relevance in the globalized 21st century. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).","PeriodicalId":16691,"journal":{"name":"Journal of personality and social psychology","volume":"59 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":7.6,"publicationDate":"2024-10-24","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142490834","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Socioeconomic status differences in agentic and communal self-concepts: Insights from 6 million people across 133 nations. 代理人和社区自我概念的社会经济地位差异:来自 133 个国家 600 万人的见解。
IF 7.6 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, SOCIAL Pub Date : 2024-10-17 DOI: 10.1037/pspp0000517
Lucia L-A Boileau,Jochen E Gebauer,Wiebke Bleidorn,P Jason Rentfrow,Jeff Potter,Samuel D Gosling
Do people of different socioeconomic status (SES) differ in how they see themselves on the Big Two self-concept dimensions of agency and communion? Existent research relevant to this theoretically and socially important question has generally been indirect: It has relied on distant proxies for agentic and communal self-concepts, narrow operationalizations of SES, comparatively small samples, and data from few nations/world regions. By contrast, the present research directly examines the associations between SES and agentic and communal self-concepts, relies on well-validated measures of agency and communion, examines three complementary measures of SES, and uses data from 6 million people (years of age: M = 26.12, SD = 11.50) across 133 nations. Overall, people of higher status saw themselves as somewhat more agentic and as slightly less (or negligibly less) communal. Crucially, those associations varied considerably across nations. We sought to explain that variation with 11 national characteristics and found only three of them to be robustly relevant: National religiosity and pathogen load curbed status differences in agentic self-concepts, and income inequality amplified status differences in communal self-concepts. Our discussion develops theory to explain the importance of national religiosity, pathogen load, and income inequality for socioeconomic status differences in agentic and communal self-concepts and it also describes the substantial societal implications of those differences. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).
不同社会经济地位(SES)的人在如何看待自己的 "代理 "和 "共融 "两大自我概念维度上是否存在差异?与这一具有重要理论和社会意义的问题相关的现有研究一般都是间接的:这些研究依赖于代理和共通自我概念的遥远替代物、狭隘的 SES 操作方法、相对较小的样本以及来自少数国家/世界地区的数据。相比之下,本研究直接考察了社会经济地位与代理型和社区型自我概念之间的关系,采用了经过充分验证的代理型和社区型测量方法,考察了三种互补的社会经济地位测量方法,并使用了来自 133 个国家的 600 万人的数据(年龄:M = 26.12,SD = 11.50)。总体而言,地位越高的人认为自己的能动性越高,共融性越低(或低得可以忽略不计)。重要的是,这些关联在不同国家之间存在很大差异。我们试图用 11 个国家的特征来解释这种差异,结果发现其中只有三个国家的特征具有很强的相关性:民族宗教信仰和病原体负荷抑制了代理型自我概念的地位差异,而收入不平等扩大了社区型自我概念的地位差异。我们的讨论提出了一些理论来解释民族宗教信仰、病原体负荷和收入不平等对代理人和社区自我概念的社会经济地位差异的重要性,同时也描述了这些差异的重大社会影响。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved)。
{"title":"Socioeconomic status differences in agentic and communal self-concepts: Insights from 6 million people across 133 nations.","authors":"Lucia L-A Boileau,Jochen E Gebauer,Wiebke Bleidorn,P Jason Rentfrow,Jeff Potter,Samuel D Gosling","doi":"10.1037/pspp0000517","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/pspp0000517","url":null,"abstract":"Do people of different socioeconomic status (SES) differ in how they see themselves on the Big Two self-concept dimensions of agency and communion? Existent research relevant to this theoretically and socially important question has generally been indirect: It has relied on distant proxies for agentic and communal self-concepts, narrow operationalizations of SES, comparatively small samples, and data from few nations/world regions. By contrast, the present research directly examines the associations between SES and agentic and communal self-concepts, relies on well-validated measures of agency and communion, examines three complementary measures of SES, and uses data from 6 million people (years of age: M = 26.12, SD = 11.50) across 133 nations. Overall, people of higher status saw themselves as somewhat more agentic and as slightly less (or negligibly less) communal. Crucially, those associations varied considerably across nations. We sought to explain that variation with 11 national characteristics and found only three of them to be robustly relevant: National religiosity and pathogen load curbed status differences in agentic self-concepts, and income inequality amplified status differences in communal self-concepts. Our discussion develops theory to explain the importance of national religiosity, pathogen load, and income inequality for socioeconomic status differences in agentic and communal self-concepts and it also describes the substantial societal implications of those differences. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).","PeriodicalId":16691,"journal":{"name":"Journal of personality and social psychology","volume":"38 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":7.6,"publicationDate":"2024-10-17","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142449376","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Journal of personality and social psychology
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1