首页 > 最新文献

Journal of personality and social psychology最新文献

英文 中文
Does the first letter of one's name affect life decisions? A natural language processing examination of nominative determinism. 一个人名字的第一个字母会影响人生的决定吗?主格决定论的自然语言处理检验。
IF 7.6 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, SOCIAL Pub Date : 2023-11-01 Epub Date: 2023-05-25 DOI: 10.1037/pspa0000347
Promothesh Chatterjee, Himanshu Mishra, Arul Mishra

This research examines whether the phenomenon of nominative determinism (a name-driven outcome) exists in the real world. Nominative determinism manifests as a preference for a profession or city to live in that begins with the same letter as a person's own name. The literature presents opposing views on this phenomenon, with one stream of research documenting the influence and another stream questioning the existence and generalizability of the effect, as well as the proposed underlying process. To examine whether the effect occurs in the real world, we use large language models trained on Common Crawl, Twitter, Google News, and Google Books using two natural language processing word-embedding algorithms (word2vec and GloVe). After controlling for relevant variables, we find consistent evidence of the relationship between people's names and a preference for major life choices starting with the same letter as their first name. Our theoretical framework of identity expression builds on the implicit egotism explanation. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2023 APA, all rights reserved).

这项研究考察了主格决定论(一种名称驱动的结果)现象是否存在于现实世界中。提名决定论表现为对一个职业或城市的偏好,该偏好以与个人名字相同的字母开头。文献对这一现象提出了相反的观点,一股研究记录了这种影响,另一股研究质疑这种影响的存在性和普遍性,以及拟议的潜在过程。为了检验这种影响是否发生在现实世界中,我们使用在Common Crawl、Twitter、Google News和Google Books上训练的大型语言模型,使用两种自然语言处理单词嵌入算法(word2vec和GloVe)。在控制了相关变量后,我们发现了一致的证据,证明人们的名字与对主要生活选择的偏好之间存在关系,这些选择以与他们的名字相同的字母开头。我们的身份表达理论框架建立在隐含的利己主义解释之上。(PsycInfo数据库记录(c)2023 APA,保留所有权利)。
{"title":"Does the first letter of one's name affect life decisions? A natural language processing examination of nominative determinism.","authors":"Promothesh Chatterjee, Himanshu Mishra, Arul Mishra","doi":"10.1037/pspa0000347","DOIUrl":"10.1037/pspa0000347","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>This research examines whether the phenomenon of nominative determinism (a name-driven outcome) exists in the real world. Nominative determinism manifests as a preference for a profession or city to live in that begins with the same letter as a person's own name. The literature presents opposing views on this phenomenon, with one stream of research documenting the influence and another stream questioning the existence and generalizability of the effect, as well as the proposed underlying process. To examine whether the effect occurs in the real world, we use large language models trained on Common Crawl, Twitter, Google News, and Google Books using two natural language processing word-embedding algorithms (word2vec and GloVe). After controlling for relevant variables, we find consistent evidence of the relationship between people's names and a preference for major life choices starting with the same letter as their first name. Our theoretical framework of identity expression builds on the implicit egotism explanation. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2023 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":16691,"journal":{"name":"Journal of personality and social psychology","volume":" ","pages":"943-968"},"PeriodicalIF":7.6,"publicationDate":"2023-11-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9522379","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Morality beyond the WEIRD: How the nomological network of morality varies across cultures. 超越怪异的道德:道德的法理学网络如何在不同文化中变化。
IF 7.6 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, SOCIAL Pub Date : 2023-11-01 Epub Date: 2023-08-17 DOI: 10.1037/pspp0000470
Mohammad Atari, Jonathan Haidt, Jesse Graham, Sena Koleva, Sean T Stevens, Morteza Dehghani

Moral foundations theory has been a generative framework in moral psychology in the last 2 decades. Here, we revisit the theory and develop a new measurement tool, the Moral Foundations Questionnaire-2 (MFQ-2), based on data from 25 populations. We demonstrate empirically that equality and proportionality are distinct moral foundations while retaining the other four existing foundations of care, loyalty, authority, and purity. Three studies were conducted to develop the MFQ-2 and to examine how the nomological network of moral foundations varies across 25 populations. Study 1 (N = 3,360, five populations) specified a refined top-down approach for measurement of moral foundations. Study 2 (N = 3,902, 19 populations) used a variety of methods (e.g., factor analysis, exploratory structural equations model, network psychometrics, alignment measurement equivalence) to provide evidence that the MFQ-2 fares well in terms of reliability and validity across cultural contexts. We also examined population-level, religious, ideological, and gender differences using the new measure. Study 3 (N = 1,410, three populations) provided evidence for convergent validity of the MFQ-2 scores, expanded the nomological network of the six moral foundations, and demonstrated the improved predictive power of the measure compared with the original MFQ. Importantly, our results showed how the nomological network of moral foundations varied across cultural contexts: consistent with a pluralistic view of morality, different foundations were influential in the network of moral foundations depending on cultural context. These studies sharpen the theoretical and methodological resolution of moral foundations theory and provide the field of moral psychology a more accurate instrument for investigating the many ways that moral conflicts and divisions are shaping the modern world. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2023 APA, all rights reserved).

道德基础理论是近二十年来道德心理学的一个生成框架。在这里,我们重新审视了这一理论,并开发了一个新的测量工具,道德基础问卷-2 (MFQ-2),基于25个人群的数据。我们的经验证明,平等和比例性是独特的道德基础,同时保留了其他四个现有的基础,即关心,忠诚,权威和纯洁。研究人员进行了三项研究,以开发MFQ-2,并检查道德基础的法理学网络如何在25个人群中变化。研究1 (N = 3,360, 5个群体)指定了一种完善的自上而下的方法来衡量道德基础。研究2 (N = 3,902, 19个人群)使用多种方法(如因子分析、探索性结构方程模型、网络心理测量学、校准测量等效性)提供证据,证明MFQ-2在跨文化背景的信度和效度方面表现良好。我们还使用新方法检查了人口水平、宗教、意识形态和性别差异。研究3 (N = 1410, 3个人群)为MFQ-2评分的收敛效度提供了证据,扩展了6个道德基础的法则网络,并证明了与原始MFQ相比,该测量的预测能力有所提高。重要的是,我们的研究结果显示了道德基础的法理网络在不同的文化背景下是如何变化的:与道德的多元观一致,不同的基础在不同的文化背景下对道德基础网络有影响。这些研究强化了道德基础理论的理论和方法解决方案,并为道德心理学领域提供了一个更准确的工具,用于调查道德冲突和分歧塑造现代世界的多种方式。(PsycInfo数据库记录(c) 2023 APA,版权所有)。
{"title":"Morality beyond the WEIRD: How the nomological network of morality varies across cultures.","authors":"Mohammad Atari, Jonathan Haidt, Jesse Graham, Sena Koleva, Sean T Stevens, Morteza Dehghani","doi":"10.1037/pspp0000470","DOIUrl":"10.1037/pspp0000470","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Moral foundations theory has been a generative framework in moral psychology in the last 2 decades. Here, we revisit the theory and develop a new measurement tool, the Moral Foundations Questionnaire-2 (MFQ-2), based on data from 25 populations. We demonstrate empirically that equality and proportionality are distinct moral foundations while retaining the other four existing foundations of care, loyalty, authority, and purity. Three studies were conducted to develop the MFQ-2 and to examine how the nomological network of moral foundations varies across 25 populations. Study 1 (<i>N</i> = 3,360, five populations) specified a refined top-down approach for measurement of moral foundations. Study 2 (<i>N</i> = 3,902, 19 populations) used a variety of methods (e.g., factor analysis, exploratory structural equations model, network psychometrics, alignment measurement equivalence) to provide evidence that the MFQ-2 fares well in terms of reliability and validity across cultural contexts. We also examined population-level, religious, ideological, and gender differences using the new measure. Study 3 (<i>N</i> = 1,410, three populations) provided evidence for convergent validity of the MFQ-2 scores, expanded the nomological network of the six moral foundations, and demonstrated the improved predictive power of the measure compared with the original MFQ. Importantly, our results showed how the nomological network of moral foundations varied across cultural contexts: consistent with a pluralistic view of morality, different foundations were influential in the network of moral foundations depending on cultural context. These studies sharpen the theoretical and methodological resolution of moral foundations theory and provide the field of moral psychology a more accurate instrument for investigating the many ways that moral conflicts and divisions are shaping the modern world. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2023 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":16691,"journal":{"name":"Journal of personality and social psychology","volume":" ","pages":"1157-1188"},"PeriodicalIF":7.6,"publicationDate":"2023-11-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10012279","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Correction to Elnakouri et al. (2023). 对Elnakouri等人(2023)的更正。
IF 7.6 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, SOCIAL Pub Date : 2023-11-01 Epub Date: 2023-09-07 DOI: 10.1037/pspi0000436
<p><p>Reports an error in "In it together: Shared reality with instrumental others is linked to goal success" by Abdo Elnakouri, Maya Rossignac-Milon, Kori L. Krueger, Amanda L. Forest, E. Tory Higgins and Abigail A. Scholer (<i>Journal of Personality and Social Psychology</i>, Advanced Online Publication, Jul 13, 2023, np). In the original article, the abstract was revised. Specifically, there were errors in the the second and third sentences of the fifth paragraph of the Shared Reality section, fifth sentence of the Present Research section, An updated Figure 1 now appears in the erratum. NIO counterpart and and the specific note in Table 3, the first parenthetical text in the Procedure and Materials section in Study 2c, the phrase its NIO counterpart in the Discussion section of Study 2c, last sentence of the second paragraph of Study 3, third sentence in the third paragraph of Study 3, first sentence in the third paragraph of the Results section, the phrase their NIO counterparts in both the Self-Reported Goal Success and GPA sections of Study 4c, NIO counterpart and the specific note in Table 9, last phrase in the second paragraph in the Discussion section of Study 4, and the in-text citation of Footnote 9 in the Contribution to Understanding the Interpersonal Influences on Goal Success. (The following abstract of the original article appeared in record 2023-89842-001). Why are some people more successful than others? In addition to individual factors (e.g. self-control), research has recently suggested that the quality of people's interpersonal relationships is crucial for success. Successful people do not just like and feel close to instrumental objects (e.g., study material, the gym), they also like and feel close to instrumental others (IOs; people who make goal success more likely). Yet instrumental people have one crucially distinct feature that instrumental objects do not: A mind of their own. We propose that while a growing body of work suggests that the sense of closeness to IOs (others who make goal success more likely) is crucial for goal success, prior work has not examined how the sense of the quality of people's relationships with IOs, and therefore goal success, likely depends on their ability to "merge minds" with them, experiencing both the goal and the world at large (i.e., shared reality) in the same way as one's IO contributes to goal success. Specifically, the present research (<i>N</i> = 1,326) explored (a) whether people experience shared reality-the perception of shared attitudes and judgments about the world-with IOs and (b) whether those who do so achieve greater goal success. Participants perceiving their romantic partner as instrumental for their goals experienced more shared reality with them (Study 1); participants also reported greater shared reality with IOs relative to noninstrumental others (NIO; Study 2). Higher shared reality with IOs was linked to more goal success initially, (Studies 2-4), 3-4 weeks late
报告了Abdo Elnakouri, Maya Rossignac-Milon, Kori L. Krueger, Amanda L. Forest, E. Tory Higgins和Abigail A. Scholer(人格与社会心理学杂志,高级在线出版,2023年7月13日,p)所写的“在一起:与有用的他人共享现实与目标成功有关”中的错误。在原文中,对摘要进行了修改。具体来说,在共享现实部分的第五段的第二句和第三句中出现了错误,在当前研究部分的第五句中出现了更新的图1。表3中的NIO对应物和具体注释,研究2c的程序和材料部分的第一个括号文本,研究2c的讨论部分的NIO对应物,研究3第二段的最后一句话,研究3第三段的第三句,结果部分的第三段的第一句话,研究4c的自我报告目标成功和GPA部分的NIO对应物,NIO的对应和表9中的具体注释,研究4的Discussion部分第二段的最后一个短语,以及《Contribution to Understanding人际关系对目标成功的影响》中脚注9的文本引用。(原文摘要见记录2023-89842-001)为什么有些人比其他人更成功?除了个人因素(如自我控制),最近的研究表明,人际关系的质量对成功至关重要。成功人士不仅喜欢并感觉接近工具性对象(例如,学习材料,健身房),他们也喜欢并感觉接近工具性对象(IOs;更有可能实现目标的人)。然而,工具型人有一个工具型物体所没有的重要特征:他们有自己的思想。我们建议,尽管越来越多的研究表明,亲密的感觉,IOs(使目标的人更容易获得成功)为目标的成功是至关重要的,之前没有检查工作的人与IOs的关系的质量,因此目标成功,可能取决于他们的能力“合并思想”,经历了目标和世界的(例如,共享现实)以同样的方式作为一个成功的IO有助于目标。具体来说,目前的研究(N = 1326)探讨了(a)人们是否体验到与IOs共享的现实——对世界的共同态度和判断的感知,以及(b)这样做的人是否实现了更大的目标成功。参与者认为他们的浪漫伴侣有助于实现他们的目标,与他们分享更多的现实(研究1);参与者还报告说,与非工具性的其他人相比,他们与IOs分享的现实更多(NIO;研究2)。IOs共享现实越高,最初的目标成功率越高(研究2-4),3-4周后(研究2c),平均成绩越高(研究4)。在控制IO喜欢度、亲密度、认知信任和NIO共享现实的情况下,这些影响仍然存在。自我效能感在IO共享现实对目标成功的影响中起到中介作用,表明IO共享现实可能增强人们对自己能力的认知信心。总的来说,研究结果表明,与IOs共享现实可能在目标成功中发挥重要作用。(PsycInfo数据库记录(c) 2023 APA,版权所有)。
{"title":"Correction to Elnakouri et al. (2023).","authors":"","doi":"10.1037/pspi0000436","DOIUrl":"10.1037/pspi0000436","url":null,"abstract":"&lt;p&gt;&lt;p&gt;Reports an error in \"In it together: Shared reality with instrumental others is linked to goal success\" by Abdo Elnakouri, Maya Rossignac-Milon, Kori L. Krueger, Amanda L. Forest, E. Tory Higgins and Abigail A. Scholer (&lt;i&gt;Journal of Personality and Social Psychology&lt;/i&gt;, Advanced Online Publication, Jul 13, 2023, np). In the original article, the abstract was revised. Specifically, there were errors in the the second and third sentences of the fifth paragraph of the Shared Reality section, fifth sentence of the Present Research section, An updated Figure 1 now appears in the erratum. NIO counterpart and and the specific note in Table 3, the first parenthetical text in the Procedure and Materials section in Study 2c, the phrase its NIO counterpart in the Discussion section of Study 2c, last sentence of the second paragraph of Study 3, third sentence in the third paragraph of Study 3, first sentence in the third paragraph of the Results section, the phrase their NIO counterparts in both the Self-Reported Goal Success and GPA sections of Study 4c, NIO counterpart and the specific note in Table 9, last phrase in the second paragraph in the Discussion section of Study 4, and the in-text citation of Footnote 9 in the Contribution to Understanding the Interpersonal Influences on Goal Success. (The following abstract of the original article appeared in record 2023-89842-001). Why are some people more successful than others? In addition to individual factors (e.g. self-control), research has recently suggested that the quality of people's interpersonal relationships is crucial for success. Successful people do not just like and feel close to instrumental objects (e.g., study material, the gym), they also like and feel close to instrumental others (IOs; people who make goal success more likely). Yet instrumental people have one crucially distinct feature that instrumental objects do not: A mind of their own. We propose that while a growing body of work suggests that the sense of closeness to IOs (others who make goal success more likely) is crucial for goal success, prior work has not examined how the sense of the quality of people's relationships with IOs, and therefore goal success, likely depends on their ability to \"merge minds\" with them, experiencing both the goal and the world at large (i.e., shared reality) in the same way as one's IO contributes to goal success. Specifically, the present research (&lt;i&gt;N&lt;/i&gt; = 1,326) explored (a) whether people experience shared reality-the perception of shared attitudes and judgments about the world-with IOs and (b) whether those who do so achieve greater goal success. Participants perceiving their romantic partner as instrumental for their goals experienced more shared reality with them (Study 1); participants also reported greater shared reality with IOs relative to noninstrumental others (NIO; Study 2). Higher shared reality with IOs was linked to more goal success initially, (Studies 2-4), 3-4 weeks late","PeriodicalId":16691,"journal":{"name":"Journal of personality and social psychology","volume":" ","pages":"1095"},"PeriodicalIF":7.6,"publicationDate":"2023-11-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10233226","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Examining individual differences in metaperceptive accuracy using the social meta-accuracy model. 使用社会元准确性模型检验元知觉准确性的个体差异。
IF 7.6 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, SOCIAL Pub Date : 2023-11-01 Epub Date: 2023-09-18 DOI: 10.1037/pspp0000479
Leonie Hater, Norhan Elsaadawy, Jeremy C Biesanz, Simon M Breil, Lauren J Human, Lisa M Niemeyer, Hasagani Tissera, Mitja D Back, Erika N Carlson

To what extent do individuals differ in understanding how others see them and who is particularly good at it? Answering these questions about the "good metaperceiver" is relevant given the beneficial outcomes of meta-accuracy. However, there likely is more than one type of the good metaperceiver: One who knows the specific impressions they make more than others do (dyadic meta-accuracy) and one who knows their reputation more than others do (generalized meta-accuracy). To identify and understand these good metaperceivers, we introduce the social meta-accuracy model (SMAM) as a statistical and conceptual framework and apply the SMAM to four samples of first impression interactions. As part of our demonstration, we also investigated the routes to and the correlates of both types of good metaperceivers. Results from SMAM show that, overall, people were able to detect the unique and general first impressions they made, but there was little evidence for individual differences in dyadic meta-accuracy in a first impression. In contrast, there were substantial individual differences in generalized meta-accuracy, and this ability was largely explained by being transparent (i.e., good metaperceivers were seen as they saw themselves). We also observed some evidence that good generalized metaperceivers in a first impression tend to be extraverted and popular. This work demonstrated that the SMAM is a useful tool for identifying and understanding both types of good metaperceivers and paves the way for future work on individual differences in meta-accuracy in other contexts. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2023 APA, all rights reserved).

在理解别人对自己的看法方面,个人之间有多大程度的差异?考虑到元准确性的有益结果,回答这些关于“好的元感知者”的问题是相关的。然而,优秀的元感知者可能不止一种:一种比其他人更了解自己留下的具体印象(二元元准确性),另一种比其他人更了解自己的声誉(广义元准确性)。为了识别和理解这些优秀的元感知者,我们引入了社会元准确性模型(SMAM)作为一个统计和概念框架,并将SMAM应用于四个第一印象互动样本。作为我们演示的一部分,我们还研究了两种良好的元感知器的路径和相关关系。SMAM的结果表明,总的来说,人们能够察觉到他们所留下的独特和一般的第一印象,但几乎没有证据表明第一印象的二元元准确性存在个体差异。相比之下,广义元准确性存在显著的个体差异,这种能力在很大程度上可以通过透明来解释(即,优秀的元感知者被视为他们看待自己的方式)。我们还观察到一些证据表明,良好的广义元知觉者在第一印象中往往是外向和受欢迎的。这项工作表明,SMAM是识别和理解这两种类型的优秀元感知者的有用工具,并为未来研究其他背景下元准确性的个体差异铺平了道路。(PsycInfo数据库记录(c) 2023 APA,版权所有)。
{"title":"Examining individual differences in metaperceptive accuracy using the social meta-accuracy model.","authors":"Leonie Hater, Norhan Elsaadawy, Jeremy C Biesanz, Simon M Breil, Lauren J Human, Lisa M Niemeyer, Hasagani Tissera, Mitja D Back, Erika N Carlson","doi":"10.1037/pspp0000479","DOIUrl":"10.1037/pspp0000479","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>To what extent do individuals differ in understanding how others see them and who is particularly good at it? Answering these questions about the \"good metaperceiver\" is relevant given the beneficial outcomes of meta-accuracy. However, there likely is more than one type of the good metaperceiver: One who knows the specific impressions they make more than others do (<i>dyadic meta-accuracy</i>) and one who knows their reputation more than others do <i>(generalized meta-accuracy</i>). To identify and understand these good metaperceivers, we introduce the social meta-accuracy model (SMAM) as a statistical and conceptual framework and apply the SMAM to four samples of first impression interactions. As part of our demonstration, we also investigated the routes to and the correlates of both types of good metaperceivers. Results from SMAM show that, overall, people were able to detect the unique and general first impressions they made, but there was little evidence for individual differences in dyadic meta-accuracy in a first impression. In contrast, there were substantial individual differences in generalized meta-accuracy, and this ability was largely explained by being transparent (i.e., good metaperceivers were seen as they saw themselves). We also observed some evidence that good generalized metaperceivers in a first impression tend to be extraverted and popular. This work demonstrated that the SMAM is a useful tool for identifying and understanding both types of good metaperceivers and paves the way for future work on individual differences in meta-accuracy in other contexts. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2023 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":16691,"journal":{"name":"Journal of personality and social psychology","volume":" ","pages":"1119-1135"},"PeriodicalIF":7.6,"publicationDate":"2023-11-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10287132","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
What limitations are reported in short articles in social and personality psychology? 社会心理学和人格心理学的短文有哪些局限性?
IF 7.6 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, SOCIAL Pub Date : 2023-10-01 Epub Date: 2023-03-30 DOI: 10.1037/pspp0000458
Beth Clarke, Sarah Schiavone, Simine Vazire

Every research project has limitations. The limitations that authors acknowledge in their articles offer a glimpse into some of the concerns that occupy a field's attention. We examine the types of limitations authors discuss in their published articles by categorizing them according to the four validities framework and investigate whether the field's attention to each of the four validities has shifted from 2010 to 2020. We selected one journal in social and personality psychology (Social Psychological and Personality Science; SPPS), the subfield most in the crosshairs of psychology's replication crisis. We sampled 440 articles (with half of those articles containing a subsection explicitly addressing limitations), and we identified and categorized 831 limitations across the 440 articles. Articles with limitations sections reported more limitations than those without (avg. 2.6 vs. 1.2 limitations per article). Threats to external validity were the most common type of reported limitation (est. 52% of articles), and threats to statistical conclusion validity were the least common (est. 17% of articles). Authors reported slightly more limitations over time. Despite the extensive attention paid to statistical conclusion validity in the scientific discourse throughout psychology's credibility revolution, our results suggest that concerns about statistics-related issues were not reflected in social and personality psychologists' reported limitations. The high prevalence of limitations concerning external validity might suggest it is time that we improve our practices in this area, rather than apologizing for these limitations after the fact. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2023 APA, all rights reserved).

每个研究项目都有局限性。作者在文章中承认的局限性让我们得以一窥占据某个领域注意力的一些问题。我们根据四种有效性框架对作者在发表的文章中讨论的限制类型进行了分类,并调查了该领域对四种有效期中每一种的关注是否从2010年转移到了2020年。我们选择了一本社会与人格心理学杂志(《社会心理与人格科学》;SPPS),这是心理学复制危机最受关注的子领域。我们对440篇文章进行了抽样(其中一半的文章包含明确说明限制的小节),并在440篇文章中确定和分类了831个限制。有限制部分的文章比没有限制的文章报告了更多的限制(平均每篇文章2.6个限制,而1.2个限制)。外部有效性威胁是最常见的报告限制类型(约52%的文章),统计结论有效性威胁最不常见(约17%的文章)。作者报告说,随着时间的推移,局限性略有增加。尽管在心理学的可信度革命中,科学话语中对统计结论的有效性给予了广泛关注,但我们的研究结果表明,对统计相关问题的担忧并没有反映在社会和人格心理学家报告的局限性中。关于外部有效性的限制的高流行率可能表明,我们是时候改进我们在这一领域的做法了,而不是事后为这些限制道歉。(PsycInfo数据库记录(c)2023 APA,保留所有权利)。
{"title":"What limitations are reported in short articles in social and personality psychology?","authors":"Beth Clarke, Sarah Schiavone, Simine Vazire","doi":"10.1037/pspp0000458","DOIUrl":"10.1037/pspp0000458","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Every research project has limitations. The limitations that authors acknowledge in their articles offer a glimpse into some of the concerns that occupy a field's attention. We examine the types of limitations authors discuss in their published articles by categorizing them according to the four validities framework and investigate whether the field's attention to each of the four validities has shifted from 2010 to 2020. We selected one journal in social and personality psychology (<i>Social Psychological and Personality Science; SPPS</i>), the subfield most in the crosshairs of psychology's replication crisis. We sampled 440 articles (with half of those articles containing a subsection explicitly addressing limitations), and we identified and categorized 831 limitations across the 440 articles. Articles with limitations sections reported more limitations than those without (avg. 2.6 vs. 1.2 limitations per article). Threats to external validity were the most common type of reported limitation (est. 52% of articles), and threats to statistical conclusion validity were the least common (est. 17% of articles). Authors reported slightly more limitations over time. Despite the extensive attention paid to statistical conclusion validity in the scientific discourse throughout psychology's credibility revolution, our results suggest that concerns about statistics-related issues were not reflected in social and personality psychologists' reported limitations. The high prevalence of limitations concerning external validity might suggest it is time that we improve our practices in this area, rather than apologizing for these limitations after the fact. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2023 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":16691,"journal":{"name":"Journal of personality and social psychology","volume":"125 4","pages":"874-901"},"PeriodicalIF":7.6,"publicationDate":"2023-10-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10296799","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
When and why we ostracize others: Motivated social exclusion in group contexts. 我们排斥他人的时间和原因:群体背景下的社会排斥动机。
IF 7.6 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, SOCIAL Pub Date : 2023-10-01 Epub Date: 2023-03-09 DOI: 10.1037/pspi0000423
Selma C Rudert, J N Rasmus Möring, Christoph Kenntemich, Christiane M Büttner

Research on ostracism has mostly focused on ostracized targets' reactions to being excluded and ignored. In contrast, the ostracizing sources' perspective and reasons for why individuals decide to ostracize others are still a largely unexplored frontier for empirical research. We propose two fundamental motives situated in the target's behavior that drive motivated ostracism decisions for the benefit of one's group: A perceived norm violation of the target and perceived expendability of the target for achieving group goals. Two survey studies and five experiments (total N = 2,394, all preregistered) support our predictions: When asked to recall a recent ostracism decision and the motives for it, participants reported both perceived norm violations and/or expendability of the target as motives (Study 1). Switching to the target perspective, the frequency of experienced ostracism was associated with both self-perceived norm violations and expendability (Study 2). In five experiments (Studies 3-7), participants consistently choose to ostracize targets more often when they perceived them to be either norm-violating, or inept in a skill important for the group and thus expendable. Additionally, Studies 5-7 show that strategic considerations about the requirements of the situational context influence ostracism decisions: Participants were more likely to ostracize norm-violating targets in cooperative contexts, and more likely to ostracize inept targets in performance contexts. Results have strong theoretical implications for research on ostracism and group dynamics, as well as for interventions targeting ostracism behavior: Particularly, adjusting the requirements of the situational context might be a viable option to decrease ostracism and promote inclusion in groups. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2023 APA, all rights reserved).

关于排斥的研究主要集中在被排斥的目标对被排斥和忽视的反应上。相比之下,排斥来源的观点和个人决定排斥他人的原因在很大程度上仍然是实证研究的前沿。我们提出了目标行为中的两个基本动机,这两个动机驱动了出于群体利益的动机排斥决策:感知到的对目标的规范违反和感知到的目标对实现群体目标的可扩展性。两项调查研究和五项实验(总共N=2394,均为预登记)支持了我们的预测:当被要求回忆最近的排斥决定及其动机时,参与者报告了感知到的违反规范和/或目标的可消费性作为动机(研究1)。转换到目标视角,经历排斥的频率与自我感知的违反规范和可扩展性有关(研究2)。在五个实验中(研究3-7),当参与者认为目标违反规范,或者在对团队重要的技能上不称职,因此可以牺牲时,他们总是选择更频繁地排斥目标。此外,研究5-7表明,对情境环境要求的战略考虑会影响排斥决策:参与者在合作环境中更有可能排斥违反规范的目标,而在绩效环境中更可能排斥无能的目标。研究结果对排斥和群体动力学的研究以及针对排斥行为的干预措施具有很强的理论意义:特别是,调整情境背景的要求可能是减少排斥和促进群体包容的可行选择。(PsycInfo数据库记录(c)2023 APA,保留所有权利)。
{"title":"When and why we ostracize others: Motivated social exclusion in group contexts.","authors":"Selma C Rudert, J N Rasmus Möring, Christoph Kenntemich, Christiane M Büttner","doi":"10.1037/pspi0000423","DOIUrl":"10.1037/pspi0000423","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Research on ostracism has mostly focused on ostracized targets' reactions to being excluded and ignored. In contrast, the ostracizing sources' perspective and reasons for why individuals decide to ostracize others are still a largely unexplored frontier for empirical research. We propose two fundamental motives situated in the target's behavior that drive motivated ostracism decisions for the benefit of one's group: A perceived norm violation of the target and perceived expendability of the target for achieving group goals. Two survey studies and five experiments (total <i>N</i> = 2,394, all preregistered) support our predictions: When asked to recall a recent ostracism decision and the motives for it, participants reported both perceived norm violations and/or expendability of the target as motives (Study 1). Switching to the target perspective, the frequency of experienced ostracism was associated with both self-perceived norm violations and expendability (Study 2). In five experiments (Studies 3-7), participants consistently choose to ostracize targets more often when they perceived them to be either norm-violating, or inept in a skill important for the group and thus expendable. Additionally, Studies 5-7 show that strategic considerations about the requirements of the situational context influence ostracism decisions: Participants were more likely to ostracize norm-violating targets in cooperative contexts, and more likely to ostracize inept targets in performance contexts. Results have strong theoretical implications for research on ostracism and group dynamics, as well as for interventions targeting ostracism behavior: Particularly, adjusting the requirements of the situational context might be a viable option to decrease ostracism and promote inclusion in groups. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2023 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":16691,"journal":{"name":"Journal of personality and social psychology","volume":"125 4","pages":"803-826"},"PeriodicalIF":7.6,"publicationDate":"2023-10-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10295811","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Field-specific ability beliefs as an explanation for gender differences in academics' career trajectories: Evidence from public profiles on ORCID.Org. 特定领域能力信念对学者职业轨迹性别差异的解释:来自ORCID.Org公开档案的证据。
IF 7.6 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, SOCIAL Pub Date : 2023-10-01 Epub Date: 2023-06-22 DOI: 10.1037/pspa0000348
Aniko Hannak, Kenneth Joseph, Daniel B Larremore, Andrei Cimpian

Academic fields exhibit substantial levels of gender segregation. Here, we investigated differences in field-specific ability beliefs (FABs) as an explanation for this phenomenon. FABs may contribute to gender segregation to the extent that they portray success as depending on "brilliance" (i.e., exceptional intellectual ability), which is a trait culturally associated with men more than women. Although prior work has documented a relation between academic fields' FABs and their gender composition, it is still unclear what the underlying dynamics are that give rise to gender imbalances across academia as a function of FABs. To provide insight into this issue, we custom-built a new data set by combining information from the author-tracking service Open Researcher and Contributor ID (ORCID) with information from a survey of U.S. academics across 30 fields. Using this expansive longitudinal data set (Ns = 86,879-364,355), we found that women were underrepresented among those who enter fields with brilliance-oriented FABs and overrepresented among those who exit these fields. We also found that FABs' association with women's transitions across academic fields was substantially stronger than their association with men's transitions. With respect to mechanisms, FABs' association with gender segregation was partially explained by the fact that women encounter more prejudice in fields with brilliance-oriented FABs. With its focus on the dynamic patterns shaping segregation and its broad scope in terms of geography, career stage, and historical time, this research makes an important contribution toward understanding the factors driving gender segregation in academia. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2023 APA, all rights reserved).

学术领域表现出相当程度的性别隔离。在这里,我们调查了特定领域能力信念(FABs)的差异,作为对这一现象的解释。FAB可能会导致性别隔离,因为它们将成功描述为取决于“才华”(即非凡的智力),这是一种在文化上与男性而非女性相关的特质。尽管先前的工作已经记录了学术领域的FAB与其性别构成之间的关系,但仍不清楚FAB导致学术界性别失衡的潜在动力是什么。为了深入了解这个问题,我们将作者跟踪服务开放研究人员和贡献者ID(ORCID)的信息与美国30个领域学者的调查信息相结合,定制了一个新的数据集。使用这一扩展的纵向数据集(Ns=86879-464355),我们发现,在那些进入以才华为导向的FAB领域的人中,女性的代表性不足,而在那些退出这些领域的人当中,女性的比例过高。我们还发现,FAB与女性在各个学术领域的转变之间的联系远强于与男性转变之间的关系。在机制方面,FAB与性别隔离的联系部分解释为,妇女在FAB注重才华的领域中遇到了更多的偏见。本研究关注的是形成种族隔离的动态模式,以及在地理、职业阶段和历史时间方面的广泛范围,为理解学术界推动性别隔离的因素做出了重要贡献。(PsycInfo数据库记录(c)2023 APA,保留所有权利)。
{"title":"Field-specific ability beliefs as an explanation for gender differences in academics' career trajectories: Evidence from public profiles on ORCID.Org.","authors":"Aniko Hannak, Kenneth Joseph, Daniel B Larremore, Andrei Cimpian","doi":"10.1037/pspa0000348","DOIUrl":"10.1037/pspa0000348","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Academic fields exhibit substantial levels of gender segregation. Here, we investigated differences in <i>field-specific ability beliefs</i> (FABs) as an explanation for this phenomenon. FABs may contribute to gender segregation to the extent that they portray success as depending on \"brilliance\" (i.e., exceptional intellectual ability), which is a trait culturally associated with men more than women. Although prior work has documented a relation between academic fields' FABs and their gender composition, it is still unclear what the underlying dynamics are that give rise to gender imbalances across academia as a function of FABs. To provide insight into this issue, we custom-built a new data set by combining information from the author-tracking service Open Researcher and Contributor ID (ORCID) with information from a survey of U.S. academics across 30 fields. Using this expansive longitudinal data set (<i>N</i>s = 86,879-364,355), we found that women were underrepresented among those who <i>enter</i> fields with brilliance-oriented FABs and overrepresented among those who <i>exit</i> these fields. We also found that FABs' association with women's transitions across academic fields was substantially stronger than their association with men's transitions. With respect to mechanisms, FABs' association with gender segregation was partially explained by the fact that women encounter more prejudice in fields with brilliance-oriented FABs. With its focus on the dynamic patterns shaping segregation and its broad scope in terms of geography, career stage, and historical time, this research makes an important contribution toward understanding the factors driving gender segregation in academia. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2023 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":16691,"journal":{"name":"Journal of personality and social psychology","volume":"125 4","pages":"681-698"},"PeriodicalIF":7.6,"publicationDate":"2023-10-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10299426","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Power dynamics and the reciprocation of trust and distrust. 权力动态以及信任和不信任的相互作用。
IF 7.6 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, SOCIAL Pub Date : 2023-10-01 Epub Date: 2023-04-10 DOI: 10.1037/pspi0000424
Marlon Mooijman

Because trust is essential in relationships, scholars have sought to determine what causes people to trust each other. A burgeoning area of research on trust has focused on power dynamics. Yet, although successful trust development in relationships is a function of one individual initiating trust and another individual reciprocating this trust, research has focused exclusively on the impact of power on trust initiation and left unaddressed the impact of power on trust reciprocation. In the current research, I examine the impact of power dynamics on trust reciprocation-people trusting someone who first trusts them. Across five preregistered experiments, I demonstrate that people are more likely to trust high-power individuals than low-power individuals when these individuals first trust them. I also demonstrate that people are more likely to distrust high-power individuals than low-power individuals when these individuals first distrust them. Power dynamics amplify trust and distrust reciprocation because people believe that having power means making decisions based on dispositions rather than the situation. This belief makes people think that-compared to low-power individuals-high-power individuals view them as more trustworthy when trusting them, but more untrustworthy when distrusting them, which motivates reciprocation. Taken together, these findings show that power helps and hurts trust development and highlight when and why this occurs. This provides some clarity to a literature dominated by inconclusive findings on the relationship between power and trust, provides a blueprint for understanding the impact of power on longer term reciprocal trust development, and provides important practical implications for power holders. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2023 APA, all rights reserved).

由于信任在人际关系中至关重要,学者们试图确定是什么原因导致人们相互信任。信任研究的一个新兴领域集中在权力动态上。然而,尽管关系中成功的信任发展是一个人发起信任和另一个人回报信任的函数,但研究只关注权力对信任发起的影响,而没有解决权力对信任回报的影响。在目前的研究中,我考察了权力动态对信任回报的影响——人们信任最先信任他们的人。在五个预先注册的实验中,我证明,当高权力的人第一次信任他们时,他们比低权力的人更有可能信任他们。我还证明,当有权势的人第一次不信任他们时,他们更有可能不信任有权势的个人,而不是有权势的个体。权力动态放大了信任和不信任的相互作用,因为人们认为拥有权力意味着根据性格而不是情况做出决定。这种信念使人们认为,与低权力的人相比,高权力的人在信任他们时认为他们更值得信赖,但在不信任他们时则认为他们更不值得信赖,这激发了回报。总之,这些发现表明,权力有助于和损害信任的发展,并强调了这种情况发生的时间和原因。这为关于权力与信任之间关系的非结论性研究结果所主导的文献提供了一些清晰度,为理解权力对长期互惠信任发展的影响提供了蓝图,并为权力持有者提供了重要的现实意义。(PsycInfo数据库记录(c)2023 APA,保留所有权利)。
{"title":"Power dynamics and the reciprocation of trust and distrust.","authors":"Marlon Mooijman","doi":"10.1037/pspi0000424","DOIUrl":"10.1037/pspi0000424","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Because trust is essential in relationships, scholars have sought to determine what causes people to trust each other. A burgeoning area of research on trust has focused on power dynamics. Yet, although successful trust development in relationships is a function of one individual initiating trust and another individual reciprocating this trust, research has focused exclusively on the impact of power on trust initiation and left unaddressed the impact of power on trust reciprocation. In the current research, I examine the impact of power dynamics on trust reciprocation-people trusting someone who first trusts them. Across five preregistered experiments, I demonstrate that people are more likely to trust high-power individuals than low-power individuals when these individuals first trust them. I also demonstrate that people are more likely to <i>dis</i>trust high-power individuals than low-power individuals when these individuals first <i>dis</i>trust them. Power dynamics amplify trust and distrust reciprocation because people believe that having power means making decisions based on dispositions rather than the situation. This belief makes people think that-compared to low-power individuals-high-power individuals view them as more trustworthy when trusting them, but more untrustworthy when distrusting them, which motivates reciprocation. Taken together, these findings show that power helps <i>and</i> hurts trust development and highlight when and why this occurs. This provides some clarity to a literature dominated by inconclusive findings on the relationship between power and trust, provides a blueprint for understanding the impact of power on longer term reciprocal trust development, and provides important practical implications for power holders. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2023 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":16691,"journal":{"name":"Journal of personality and social psychology","volume":"125 4","pages":"779-802"},"PeriodicalIF":7.6,"publicationDate":"2023-10-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10296832","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The effects of a personality intervention on satisfaction in 10 domains of life: Evidence for increases and correlated change with personality traits. 人格干预对生活10个领域满意度的影响:人格特征增加和相关变化的证据。
IF 7.6 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, SOCIAL Pub Date : 2023-10-01 Epub Date: 2023-07-27 DOI: 10.1037/pspp0000474
Gabriel Olaru, Manon A van Scheppingen, Mirjam Stieger, Tobias Kowatsch, Christoph Flückiger, Mathias Allemand

The desire to change one's personality traits has been shown to be stronger if people are dissatisfied with associated aspects of their life. While evidence for the effects of interventions on personality trait change is increasing, it is unclear whether these lead to subsequent improvements in the satisfaction with various domains of life. In this study, we examined the effects of a 3-month digital-coaching personality change intervention study on 10 domains of satisfaction. We focused on the three largest intervention groups of the study (N = 418), which included participants who wanted to increase their Emotional Stability, Conscientiousness, or Extraversion. Bivariate latent change score models were used to examine correlated change between the targeted personality traits and satisfaction domains. We found that global life satisfaction and satisfaction with oneself as a person increased in all three intervention groups. In addition, increases in specific satisfaction domains were reported for the Conscientiousness (e.g., work/school, health, friendships) and Emotional Stability (e.g., family, sexual relationships, emotions) group. Increases were stable up to the 3-month follow-up. In contrast, the waitlist control group did not report any changes in global or domain-specific life satisfaction. Changes in the satisfaction domains were positively correlated with self-reported personality trait change to a similar degree as the cross-sectional associations, but not to observer-reported personality trait change. The personality intervention thus seemed to have a positive effect on satisfaction with various domains of life, which was associated with the degree of self-reported personality trait change. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2023 APA, all rights reserved).

研究表明,如果人们对生活中的相关方面感到不满,那么改变个人性格特征的愿望就会更强。尽管干预措施对人格特质变化影响的证据越来越多,但尚不清楚这些措施是否会导致对生活各个领域的满意度随后提高。在这项研究中,我们检验了为期3个月的数字辅导人格改变干预研究对10个满意度领域的影响。我们重点关注了该研究中最大的三个干预组(N=418),其中包括希望提高情绪稳定性、责任感或外向性的参与者。使用双变量潜在变化评分模型来检验目标人格特征和满意度领域之间的相关变化。我们发现,在所有三个干预组中,全球生活满意度和对自身的满意度都有所提高。此外,据报道,尽责性(如工作/学校、健康、友谊)和情绪稳定性(如家庭、性关系、情绪)组的特定满意度领域有所增加。在3个月的随访中,增长是稳定的。相比之下,等待名单对照组没有报告全球或特定领域生活满意度的任何变化。满意度领域的变化与自我报告的人格特征变化呈正相关,其程度与横断面关联相似,但与观察者报告的人格特质变化无关。因此,人格干预似乎对生活各个领域的满意度有积极影响,这与自我报告的人格特质变化程度有关。(PsycInfo数据库记录(c)2023 APA,保留所有权利)。
{"title":"The effects of a personality intervention on satisfaction in 10 domains of life: Evidence for increases and correlated change with personality traits.","authors":"Gabriel Olaru, Manon A van Scheppingen, Mirjam Stieger, Tobias Kowatsch, Christoph Flückiger, Mathias Allemand","doi":"10.1037/pspp0000474","DOIUrl":"10.1037/pspp0000474","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The desire to change one's personality traits has been shown to be stronger if people are dissatisfied with associated aspects of their life. While evidence for the effects of interventions on personality trait change is increasing, it is unclear whether these lead to subsequent improvements in the satisfaction with various domains of life. In this study, we examined the effects of a 3-month digital-coaching personality change intervention study on 10 domains of satisfaction. We focused on the three largest intervention groups of the study (<i>N</i> = 418), which included participants who wanted to increase their Emotional Stability, Conscientiousness, or Extraversion. Bivariate latent change score models were used to examine correlated change between the targeted personality traits and satisfaction domains. We found that global life satisfaction and satisfaction with oneself as a person increased in all three intervention groups. In addition, increases in specific satisfaction domains were reported for the Conscientiousness (e.g., work/school, health, friendships) and Emotional Stability (e.g., family, sexual relationships, emotions) group. Increases were stable up to the 3-month follow-up. In contrast, the waitlist control group did not report any changes in global or domain-specific life satisfaction. Changes in the satisfaction domains were positively correlated with self-reported personality trait change to a similar degree as the cross-sectional associations, but not to observer-reported personality trait change. The personality intervention thus seemed to have a positive effect on satisfaction with various domains of life, which was associated with the degree of self-reported personality trait change. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2023 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":16691,"journal":{"name":"Journal of personality and social psychology","volume":"125 4","pages":"902-924"},"PeriodicalIF":7.6,"publicationDate":"2023-10-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10301936","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Correction to Wurm and Schäfer (2022). 修正Wurm和Schäfer(2022)。
IF 7.6 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, SOCIAL Pub Date : 2023-10-01 DOI: 10.1037/pspp0000486
<p><p>Reports an error in "Gain- but not loss-related self-perceptions of aging predict mortality over a period of 23 years: A multidimensional approach" by Susanne Wurm and Sarah K. Schäfer (<i>Journal of Personality and Social Psychology</i>, 2022[Sep], Vol 123[3], 636-653). The last sentence of the second paragraph of the Prediction of Mortality Based on Single Gain- or Loss-Related SPA Dimensions section now appears as Figure 3 shows a Kaplan-Meier curve as schematic illustration of the relationship between gain-related SPA (ongoing development) and mortality over 23 years. The title of Figure 3 now appears as Illustration of the Unadjusted Effect of Gain-Related Self-Perceptions of Aging (SPA) on Mortality, and the second to last sentence of the Figure 3 note now appears as the figure depicts the findings of Model 1.1 (cf. Table 2); in addition, this table contains findings adjusted for various covariates. All versions of this article have been corrected. (The following abstract of the original article appeared in record 2022-31793-001.) [Correction Notice: An Erratum for this article was reported online in <i>Journal of Personality and Social Psychology</i> on Apr 25 2022 (see record 2022-56187-001). In the original article, there was an error in the third sentence in the Combined Model Comprising SPA Dimensions and SA section. The corrected sentence should read as: Again, an increase of gain-related SPA by 1 SD was related to a decrease in risk of death by 12%. All versions of this article have been corrected.] Some 2 decades have passed since Levy et al. (2002) published their seminal study on the impact of self-perceptions of aging (SPA) on mortality over a period of 23 years in this journal; we aimed at replicating and extending these findings against the background of recent discussions in the research on subjective aging. Based on a large German nationwide population-based sample of individuals aged 40 and older (<i>N</i> = 2,400), for whom mortality was also documented over a period of 23 years (1996-2019), the present study is the first to investigate the unique impact of gain- and loss-related SPA and subjective age (SA) as components of subjective aging on mortality. Data were analyzed with hierarchical Cox proportional hazard regressions. The study pointed to the prominent role of gain-related SPA. For individuals who perceived aging as associated with ongoing development risk of death was half that of individuals with less gain-related SPA. Viewing aging as associated with physical or social losses could not predict mortality after controlling for covariates such as age, gender, education, health-related variables, and psychological variables known to predict mortality. Neither could SA predict mortality. When SA and gain- and loss-related SPA were analyzed in a combined model, gain-related SPA remained a significant predictor of mortality. The findings support previous studies on the importance of SPA for mortality. In addition
Susanne Wurm和Sarah K.Schäfer在“与衰老相关但非损失的自我认知预测23年内的死亡率:一种多维方法”中报告了一个错误(《个性与社会心理学杂志》,2022年9月,第123卷[3],636-653)。基于单一增益或损失相关SPA维度的死亡率预测部分第二段的最后一句现在出现在图3中,图3显示了Kaplan-Meier曲线,作为23年来增益相关SPA(正在进行的开发)与死亡率之间关系的示意图。图3的标题现在显示为增益相关的衰老自我感知(SPA)对死亡率的未经调整影响的说明,图3注释的倒数第二句现在显示为该图描述了模型1.1的发现(参见表2);此外,该表包含针对各种协变量进行调整的结果。这篇文章的所有版本都已更正。(以下原文摘要出现在记录2022-31793-001中。)[更正通知:本文的勘误表于2022年4月25日在线报道在《个性与社会心理学杂志》上(见记录2022-56187-001)在原文章中,“包含SPA维度和SA的组合模型”部分的第三句出现错误。更正后的句子应为:同样,增益相关SPA增加1 SD与死亡风险降低12%有关。这篇文章的所有版本都已更正。]自从Levy等人(2002)在本杂志上发表了他们关于衰老自我认知(SPA)对23年死亡率影响的开创性研究以来,大约20年过去了;我们的目的是在最近关于主观衰老研究的讨论背景下复制和扩展这些发现。本研究基于德国全国范围内40岁及以上人群的大样本(N=2400),在23年(1996-2019年)的时间里,这些人群的死亡率也有记录,本研究首次调查了作为主观衰老组成部分的损益相关SPA和主观年龄(SA)对死亡率的独特影响。采用层次Cox比例风险回归分析数据。该研究指出了收益相关SPA的突出作用。对于那些认为衰老与持续发展相关的个体,死亡风险是增益相关SPA较少的个体的一半。在控制了年龄、性别、教育、健康相关变量和已知可预测死亡率的心理变量等协变量后,将衰老视为与身体或社会损失相关无法预测死亡率。SA也不能预测死亡率。当在联合模型中分析SA和增益和损失相关的SPA时,增益相关的SPA仍然是死亡率的重要预测因素。这些发现支持了先前关于SPA对死亡率重要性的研究。此外,研究结果表明,主要与增益相关的SPA(而不是与损失相关的SPA和SA)可以解释死亡率的差异,因此应在干预研究中加以解决。(PsycInfo数据库记录(c)2023 APA,保留所有权利)。
{"title":"Correction to Wurm and Schäfer (2022).","authors":"","doi":"10.1037/pspp0000486","DOIUrl":"10.1037/pspp0000486","url":null,"abstract":"&lt;p&gt;&lt;p&gt;Reports an error in \"Gain- but not loss-related self-perceptions of aging predict mortality over a period of 23 years: A multidimensional approach\" by Susanne Wurm and Sarah K. Schäfer (&lt;i&gt;Journal of Personality and Social Psychology&lt;/i&gt;, 2022[Sep], Vol 123[3], 636-653). The last sentence of the second paragraph of the Prediction of Mortality Based on Single Gain- or Loss-Related SPA Dimensions section now appears as Figure 3 shows a Kaplan-Meier curve as schematic illustration of the relationship between gain-related SPA (ongoing development) and mortality over 23 years. The title of Figure 3 now appears as Illustration of the Unadjusted Effect of Gain-Related Self-Perceptions of Aging (SPA) on Mortality, and the second to last sentence of the Figure 3 note now appears as the figure depicts the findings of Model 1.1 (cf. Table 2); in addition, this table contains findings adjusted for various covariates. All versions of this article have been corrected. (The following abstract of the original article appeared in record 2022-31793-001.) [Correction Notice: An Erratum for this article was reported online in &lt;i&gt;Journal of Personality and Social Psychology&lt;/i&gt; on Apr 25 2022 (see record 2022-56187-001). In the original article, there was an error in the third sentence in the Combined Model Comprising SPA Dimensions and SA section. The corrected sentence should read as: Again, an increase of gain-related SPA by 1 SD was related to a decrease in risk of death by 12%. All versions of this article have been corrected.] Some 2 decades have passed since Levy et al. (2002) published their seminal study on the impact of self-perceptions of aging (SPA) on mortality over a period of 23 years in this journal; we aimed at replicating and extending these findings against the background of recent discussions in the research on subjective aging. Based on a large German nationwide population-based sample of individuals aged 40 and older (&lt;i&gt;N&lt;/i&gt; = 2,400), for whom mortality was also documented over a period of 23 years (1996-2019), the present study is the first to investigate the unique impact of gain- and loss-related SPA and subjective age (SA) as components of subjective aging on mortality. Data were analyzed with hierarchical Cox proportional hazard regressions. The study pointed to the prominent role of gain-related SPA. For individuals who perceived aging as associated with ongoing development risk of death was half that of individuals with less gain-related SPA. Viewing aging as associated with physical or social losses could not predict mortality after controlling for covariates such as age, gender, education, health-related variables, and psychological variables known to predict mortality. Neither could SA predict mortality. When SA and gain- and loss-related SPA were analyzed in a combined model, gain-related SPA remained a significant predictor of mortality. The findings support previous studies on the importance of SPA for mortality. In addition","PeriodicalId":16691,"journal":{"name":"Journal of personality and social psychology","volume":"125 4","pages":"719"},"PeriodicalIF":7.6,"publicationDate":"2023-10-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10310558","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Journal of personality and social psychology
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1