首页 > 最新文献

Journal of personality and social psychology最新文献

英文 中文
Going beyond the "self" in self-control: Interpersonal consequences of commitment strategies. 在自我控制中超越 "自我":承诺策略的人际后果
IF 7.6 1区 心理学 Q1 Social Sciences Pub Date : 2024-04-11 DOI: 10.1037/pspa0000385
A. Kristal, Julian J. Zlatev
Commitment strategies are effective mechanisms individuals can use to overcome self-control problems. Across seven studies (and two supplemental studies), we explore the negative interpersonal consequences of commitment strategy choice and use. In Study 1, using an incentivized trust game, we demonstrate that individuals trust people who choose to use a commitment strategy less than those who choose to use willpower to achieve their goals. Study 2 shows this relationship holds across four domains and for integrity-based trust in particular. Study 3 provides evidence that it is the choice to use the strategy rather than strategy use itself that incurs this integrity penalty. In Studies 4-5b, we demonstrate that this effect is driven, at least in part, by the fact that people infer past performance from strategy choice. Finally, Study 6 provides evidence that people select commitment strategies more in private than in public, which is consistent with the notion that people anticipate the negative consequences of commitment strategy choice. Thus, we establish the role of willpower as a positive signal in impression formation as well as the negative interpersonal consequences of choosing to rely on external aides when faced with temptation. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).
承诺策略是个人用来克服自我控制问题的有效机制。通过七项研究(以及两项补充研究),我们探讨了选择和使用承诺策略的负面人际后果。在研究 1 中,我们利用激励信任游戏证明,与选择使用意志力来实现目标的人相比,个人对选择使用承诺策略的人信任度较低。研究 2 表明,这种关系在四个领域都成立,尤其是在基于诚信的信任方面。研究 3 提供的证据表明,是选择使用策略而不是使用策略本身导致了这种诚信惩罚。在研究 4-5b 中,我们证明了这种效应至少部分是由人们从策略选择中推断出过去的表现这一事实所驱动的。最后,研究 6 提供的证据表明,人们在私下比在公开场合更倾向于选择承诺策略,这与人们预期承诺策略选择的负面影响这一观点是一致的。因此,我们确定了意志力在印象形成中作为积极信号的作用,以及在面对诱惑时选择依赖外部辅助的负面人际后果。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Going beyond the \"self\" in self-control: Interpersonal consequences of commitment strategies.","authors":"A. Kristal, Julian J. Zlatev","doi":"10.1037/pspa0000385","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/pspa0000385","url":null,"abstract":"Commitment strategies are effective mechanisms individuals can use to overcome self-control problems. Across seven studies (and two supplemental studies), we explore the negative interpersonal consequences of commitment strategy choice and use. In Study 1, using an incentivized trust game, we demonstrate that individuals trust people who choose to use a commitment strategy less than those who choose to use willpower to achieve their goals. Study 2 shows this relationship holds across four domains and for integrity-based trust in particular. Study 3 provides evidence that it is the choice to use the strategy rather than strategy use itself that incurs this integrity penalty. In Studies 4-5b, we demonstrate that this effect is driven, at least in part, by the fact that people infer past performance from strategy choice. Finally, Study 6 provides evidence that people select commitment strategies more in private than in public, which is consistent with the notion that people anticipate the negative consequences of commitment strategy choice. Thus, we establish the role of willpower as a positive signal in impression formation as well as the negative interpersonal consequences of choosing to rely on external aides when faced with temptation. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).","PeriodicalId":16691,"journal":{"name":"Journal of personality and social psychology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":7.6,"publicationDate":"2024-04-11","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140715784","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Supplemental Material for Interpersonal Supports for Basic Psychological Needs and Their Relations With Motivation, Well-Being, and Performance: A Meta-Analysis 基本心理需求的人际支持及其与动机、幸福和绩效的关系》补充材料:元分析
IF 7.6 1区 心理学 Q1 Social Sciences Pub Date : 2024-04-11 DOI: 10.1037/pspi0000459.supp
{"title":"Supplemental Material for Interpersonal Supports for Basic Psychological Needs and Their Relations With Motivation, Well-Being, and Performance: A Meta-Analysis","authors":"","doi":"10.1037/pspi0000459.supp","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/pspi0000459.supp","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":16691,"journal":{"name":"Journal of personality and social psychology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":7.6,"publicationDate":"2024-04-11","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140715269","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The allure of consensus: People (over)seek consensus in selecting group persuasion strategies. 共识的诱惑:人们在选择团体说服策略时(过度)寻求共识。
IF 6.4 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, SOCIAL Pub Date : 2024-04-01 Epub Date: 2024-02-15 DOI: 10.1037/pspa0000382
Derek D Rucker, Jesse D'Agostino, Mark Dyer, Zakary L Tormala

How do people select targets when tasked with persuading a group of people? One approach would be to prioritize getting people in support of the persuader's position to hold relatively extreme attitudes-an extremity strategy. An alternative approach would be to prioritize getting as many people as possible to support the persuader's position, regardless of how extreme they are-a consensus strategy. Although some situations might allow persuaders to combine these strategies, the present work examines how people select targets and strategies when a natural trade-off exists between acquiring fewer people with more extreme attitudes versus more people with less extreme attitudes. Prior work suggests that potential advantages exist for both extremity and consensus strategies. However, the current research finds that people exhibit a strong preference for a consensus strategy when tasked with selecting targets in group persuasion contexts. Further, this preference prevails even when consensus strategies would backfire and cause one's persuasive efforts to fail. This allure of consensus is demonstrated across eight primary experiments, which also reveal why people are drawn to a consensus strategy as well as explore potential boundary conditions for this preference. This work has implications for understanding how persuaders select targets and strategies in the context of group persuasion. In addition, the paradigms and results invite a host of new avenues for future research. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

当需要说服一群人时,人们如何选择目标?一种方法是优先让支持说服者立场的人持有相对极端的态度--极端策略。另一种方法是优先让尽可能多的人支持说服者的立场,无论他们有多极端--共识策略。虽然在某些情况下,说服者可以将这些策略结合起来,但本研究探讨的是,当人们在获取较少态度极端的人与较多态度不极端的人之间自然权衡时,如何选择目标和策略。之前的研究表明,极端策略和共识策略都存在潜在优势。然而,目前的研究发现,在群体说服情境中,人们在选择目标时会表现出对共识策略的强烈偏好。此外,即使共识策略会适得其反,导致说服工作失败,这种偏好也会占上风。八项主要实验证明了共识的诱惑力,这些实验还揭示了人们被共识策略吸引的原因,并探索了这种偏好的潜在边界条件。这项研究对于理解说服者如何在群体说服中选择目标和策略具有重要意义。此外,研究范式和结果还为今后的研究开辟了许多新途径。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved)。
{"title":"The allure of consensus: People (over)seek consensus in selecting group persuasion strategies.","authors":"Derek D Rucker, Jesse D'Agostino, Mark Dyer, Zakary L Tormala","doi":"10.1037/pspa0000382","DOIUrl":"10.1037/pspa0000382","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>How do people select targets when tasked with persuading a group of people? One approach would be to prioritize getting people in support of the persuader's position to hold relatively extreme attitudes-an <i>extremity strategy.</i> An alternative approach would be to prioritize getting as many people as possible to support the persuader's position, regardless of how extreme they are-a <i>consensus</i> <i>strategy</i>. Although some situations might allow persuaders to combine these strategies, the present work examines how people select targets and strategies when a natural trade-off exists between acquiring fewer people with more extreme attitudes versus more people with less extreme attitudes. Prior work suggests that potential advantages exist for both extremity and consensus strategies. However, the current research finds that people exhibit a strong preference for a consensus strategy when tasked with selecting targets in group persuasion contexts. Further, this preference prevails even when consensus strategies would backfire and cause one's persuasive efforts to fail. This <i>allure of consensus</i> is demonstrated across eight primary experiments, which also reveal why people are drawn to a consensus strategy as well as explore potential boundary conditions for this preference. This work has implications for understanding how persuaders select targets and strategies in the context of group persuasion. In addition, the paradigms and results invite a host of new avenues for future research. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":16691,"journal":{"name":"Journal of personality and social psychology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":6.4,"publicationDate":"2024-04-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139735481","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Getting under the skin? Influences of work-family experiences on personality trait adaptation and reciprocal relationships. 深入人心?工作与家庭经历对人格特质适应和互惠关系的影响。
IF 6.4 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, SOCIAL Pub Date : 2024-04-01 Epub Date: 2023-08-10 DOI: 10.1037/pspp0000476
Wen-Dong Li, Jiexin Wang, Tammy Allen, Xin Zhang, Kaili Yu, Hong Zhang, Jason L Huang, Mengqiao Liu, Andrew Li

The literature on personality trait development has mainly focused on influences of life experiences in one single life domain (e.g., work or family) separate from one another and has primarily examined personality development in early life stages. Thus, less attention has been devoted to influences from interplays across different life domains and personality development in middle and late adulthood. Synthesizing the literature on personality science and organizational research, we built a theoretical model and investigated what, how, and why the interplay between two central life domains-work and family-may be related to personality trait development of people at their middle and late life stages, and more important, change-related reciprocal relationships between personality traits and work-family experiences. Generally, convergent findings with data from two longitudinal studies (National Survey of Midlife in the United States, maximum N = 3,192, three waves; and Health and Retirement Study, maximum N = 1,133, three waves except anxiety) revealed that work-to-family conflict, family-to-work conflict, work-to-family facilitation, and family-to-work facilitation mostly had lagged effects on changes of Conscientiousness, Extraversion, and Neuroticism, and the influences were generally channeled through changes of anxiety. Personality traits also had lagged influences on changes of work-family experiences, with some influences deteriorating over time. Change-related reciprocal relationships were recorded mainly between Neuroticism and Extraversion with work-family experiences. Some selection effects were larger than socialization effects. Our research contributes to the personality and the work-family literature and represents a useful example of cross-fertilization of research in different areas of psychology to advance personality research. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

有关人格特质发展的文献主要集中于单个生活领域(如工作或家庭)的生活经历对人格特质发展的影响,并且主要研究生命早期阶段的人格发展。因此,人们较少关注不同生活领域的相互影响以及成年中期和晚期的人格发展。综合人格科学和组织研究方面的文献,我们建立了一个理论模型,并研究了两个核心生活领域--工作和家庭--之间的相互作用与中晚年人的人格特质发展之间的关系,以及更重要的人格特质与工作和家庭经历之间与变化相关的互惠关系。总体而言,与两项纵向研究(美国全国中年调查,最大样本数=3 192,共三波;健康与退休研究,最大样本数=1 133,共三波,焦虑除外)数据的趋同结果显示,工作与家庭的冲突、家庭与工作的冲突、工作对家庭的促进作用以及家庭对工作的促进作用大多对自觉性、外向性和神经质的变化具有滞后效应,而且这些影响一般通过焦虑的变化传导。人格特质对工作-家庭体验变化的影响也是滞后的,有些影响会随着时间的推移而减弱。与变化相关的互惠关系主要存在于神经质和外向性与工作-家庭经历之间。一些选择效应大于社会化效应。我们的研究为人格和工作-家庭文献做出了贡献,也是心理学不同领域研究相互促进以推动人格研究的一个有益范例。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Getting under the skin? Influences of work-family experiences on personality trait adaptation and reciprocal relationships.","authors":"Wen-Dong Li, Jiexin Wang, Tammy Allen, Xin Zhang, Kaili Yu, Hong Zhang, Jason L Huang, Mengqiao Liu, Andrew Li","doi":"10.1037/pspp0000476","DOIUrl":"10.1037/pspp0000476","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The literature on personality trait development has mainly focused on influences of life experiences in one single life domain (e.g., work or family) separate from one another and has primarily examined personality development in early life stages. Thus, less attention has been devoted to influences from interplays across different life domains and personality development in middle and late adulthood. Synthesizing the literature on personality science and organizational research, we built a theoretical model and investigated what, how, and why the interplay between two central life domains-work and family-may be related to personality trait development of people at their middle and late life stages, and more important, change-related reciprocal relationships between personality traits and work-family experiences. Generally, convergent findings with data from two longitudinal studies (National Survey of Midlife in the United States, maximum <i>N</i> = 3,192, three waves; and Health and Retirement Study, maximum <i>N</i> = 1,133, three waves except anxiety) revealed that work-to-family conflict, family-to-work conflict, work-to-family facilitation, and family-to-work facilitation mostly had lagged effects on changes of Conscientiousness, Extraversion, and Neuroticism, and the influences were generally channeled through changes of anxiety. Personality traits also had lagged influences on changes of work-family experiences, with some influences deteriorating over time. Change-related reciprocal relationships were recorded mainly between Neuroticism and Extraversion with work-family experiences. Some selection effects were larger than socialization effects. Our research contributes to the personality and the work-family literature and represents a useful example of cross-fertilization of research in different areas of psychology to advance personality research. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":16691,"journal":{"name":"Journal of personality and social psychology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":6.4,"publicationDate":"2024-04-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9958389","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Dual-promotion: Bragging better by promoting peers. 双重晋升:通过提拔同行更好地吹嘘自己。
IF 6.4 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, SOCIAL Pub Date : 2024-04-01 Epub Date: 2023-08-10 DOI: 10.1037/pspi0000431
Eric M VanEpps, Einav Hart, Maurice E Schweitzer

To create favorable impressions and receive credit, individuals need to share information about their past accomplishments. Broadcasting one's past accomplishments or claiming credit to demonstrate competence, however, can harm perceptions of warmth and likability. In fact, prior work has conceptualized self-promotion as a hydraulic challenge: tactics that boost perceptions along one dimension (e.g., competence) harm perceptions along other dimensions (e.g., warmth). In this work, we identify a novel approach to self-promotion: We show that by combining self-promotion with other-promotion (complimenting or giving credit to others), which we term "dual-promotion," individuals can project both warmth and competence to make better impressions on observers than they do by only self-promoting. In seven preregistered studies, including analyses of annual reports from members of Congress and experiments using social network, workplace, and political contexts (total N = 1,448), we show that individuals who engage in dual-promotion create more favorable impressions of warmth and competence than those who only engage in self-promotion. The beneficial effects of dual-promotion are robust to both competitive and noncompetitive contexts and extend to behavioral intentions. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

为了给人留下好印象并获得荣誉,个人需要分享有关其过去成就的信息。然而,宣传自己过去的成就或声称自己有能力,会损害人们对其热情和可亲程度的感知。事实上,先前的研究已将自我推销概念化为一种水力挑战:在一个维度(如能力)上提升感知的策略会损害在其他维度(如温暖)上的感知。在这项研究中,我们发现了一种新的自我推销方法:我们的研究表明,通过将自我推销与他人推销(赞美他人或给予他人荣誉)相结合(我们称之为 "双重推销"),个体可以同时表现出热情和能力,从而给观察者留下比只进行自我推销更好的印象。在七项预先登记的研究中,包括对国会议员年度报告的分析,以及利用社交网络、工作场所和政治背景进行的实验(总人数 = 1,448),我们表明,与只进行自我推销的人相比,进行双重推销的人更能给人留下热情和能力兼备的好印象。双重宣传的有利效应在竞争性和非竞争性情境中都是稳健的,并能延伸到行为意向中。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Dual-promotion: Bragging better by promoting peers.","authors":"Eric M VanEpps, Einav Hart, Maurice E Schweitzer","doi":"10.1037/pspi0000431","DOIUrl":"10.1037/pspi0000431","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>To create favorable impressions and receive credit, individuals need to share information about their past accomplishments. Broadcasting one's past accomplishments or claiming credit to demonstrate competence, however, can harm perceptions of warmth and likability. In fact, prior work has conceptualized self-promotion as a hydraulic challenge: tactics that boost perceptions along one dimension (e.g., competence) harm perceptions along other dimensions (e.g., warmth). In this work, we identify a novel approach to self-promotion: We show that by combining self-promotion with other-promotion (complimenting or giving credit to others), which we term \"dual-promotion,\" individuals can project both warmth and competence to make better impressions on observers than they do by only self-promoting. In seven preregistered studies, including analyses of annual reports from members of Congress and experiments using social network, workplace, and political contexts (total <i>N</i> = 1,448), we show that individuals who engage in dual-promotion create more favorable impressions of warmth and competence than those who only engage in self-promotion. The beneficial effects of dual-promotion are robust to both competitive and noncompetitive contexts and extend to behavioral intentions. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":16691,"journal":{"name":"Journal of personality and social psychology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":6.4,"publicationDate":"2024-04-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10108877","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Anger has benefits for attaining goals. 愤怒有利于实现目标。
IF 6.4 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, SOCIAL Pub Date : 2024-04-01 Epub Date: 2023-10-30 DOI: 10.1037/pspa0000350
Heather C Lench, Noah T Reed, Tiffany George, Kaitlyn A Kaiser, Sophia G North

[Correction Notice: An Erratum for this article was reported in Vol 126(4) of Journal of Personality and Social Psychology (see record 2024-93961-001). In the article (https://doi.org/10.1037/pspa 0000350), the first paragraph in the Method section of Study 4 has been revised. All paragraphs in the Results section of Study 4 have been revised. Table 1 and Figure 5 have been updated. All versions of this article have been corrected.] Functional accounts of emotion have guided research for decades, with the core assumption that emotions are functional-they improve outcomes for people. Based on functional accounts of emotion, we theorized that anger should improve goal attainment in the presence of challenges. In seven studies, goal attainment was assessed in situations that involved varying levels of challenges to goal attainment. Across studies, anger compared to a neutral condition resulted in behavior that facilitated greater goal attainment on tasks that involved challenges. With a goal to solve difficult puzzles, anger resulted in more puzzles correctly solved (Study 1). With a goal to attain prizes, anger increased cheating rates and numbers of unearned prizes (Study 2). With a goal to do well in a video game, anger increased scores on a game with challenges to be avoided, but not other scores (Study 3). In two studies, examining the consequences of anger in response to the challenging task that was the focus of that anger, anger decreased reaction time with goals to win trials (Study 4), and predicted making the effort to vote in two contentious elections (Study 5). With a goal to protect financial resources, anger increased action taken to prevent loss compared to a physiological arousal condition (Study 6). (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

几十年来,情绪的功能性描述一直指导着研究,其核心假设是情绪是功能性的——它们可以改善人们的结果。基于对情绪的功能描述,我们推断,在存在挑战的情况下,愤怒应该能提高目标的实现。在七项研究中,在涉及不同程度的目标实现挑战的情况下,对目标实现进行了评估。在所有研究中,与中性状态相比,愤怒会导致在涉及挑战的任务中有助于实现更大目标的行为。以解决难题为目标,愤怒会导致更多的难题得到正确解决(研究1)。为了获得奖品,愤怒会增加作弊率和未获得奖品的数量(研究2)。有了在电子游戏中表现出色的目标,愤怒会在需要避免挑战的游戏中增加分数,但不会增加其他分数(研究3)。在两项研究中,研究了愤怒对作为愤怒焦点的挑战性任务的反应结果,愤怒减少了对赢得审判目标的反应时间(研究4),并预测了在两次有争议的选举中投票的努力(研究5)。为了保护经济资源,与生理唤醒条件相比,愤怒增加了为防止损失而采取的行动(研究6)。(PsycInfo数据库记录(c)2023 APA,保留所有权利)。
{"title":"Anger has benefits for attaining goals.","authors":"Heather C Lench, Noah T Reed, Tiffany George, Kaitlyn A Kaiser, Sophia G North","doi":"10.1037/pspa0000350","DOIUrl":"10.1037/pspa0000350","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>[Correction Notice: An Erratum for this article was reported in Vol 126(4) of <i>Journal of Personality and Social Psychology</i> (see record 2024-93961-001). In the article (https://doi.org/10.1037/pspa 0000350), the first paragraph in the Method section of Study 4 has been revised. All paragraphs in the Results section of Study 4 have been revised. Table 1 and Figure 5 have been updated. All versions of this article have been corrected.] Functional accounts of emotion have guided research for decades, with the core assumption that emotions are functional-they improve outcomes for people. Based on functional accounts of emotion, we theorized that anger should improve goal attainment in the presence of challenges. In seven studies, goal attainment was assessed in situations that involved varying levels of challenges to goal attainment. Across studies, anger compared to a neutral condition resulted in behavior that facilitated greater goal attainment on tasks that involved challenges. With a goal to solve difficult puzzles, anger resulted in more puzzles correctly solved (Study 1). With a goal to attain prizes, anger increased cheating rates and numbers of unearned prizes (Study 2). With a goal to do well in a video game, anger increased scores on a game with challenges to be avoided, but not other scores (Study 3). In two studies, examining the consequences of anger in response to the challenging task that was the focus of that anger, anger decreased reaction time with goals to win trials (Study 4), and predicted making the effort to vote in two contentious elections (Study 5). With a goal to protect financial resources, anger increased action taken to prevent loss compared to a physiological arousal condition (Study 6). (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":16691,"journal":{"name":"Journal of personality and social psychology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":6.4,"publicationDate":"2024-04-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"71412678","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Correction to "Anger has benefits for attaining goals" by Lench et al. (2023). 更正 Lench 等人(2023 年)的文章 "愤怒有利于实现目标"。
IF 6.4 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, SOCIAL Pub Date : 2024-04-01 DOI: 10.1037/pspa0000400

Reports an error in "Anger has benefits for attaining goals" by Heather C. Lench, Noah T. Reed, Tiffany George, Kaitlyn A. Kaiser and Sophia G. North (Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, Advanced Online Publication, Oct 30, 2023, np). In the article (https://doi.org/10.1037/pspa 0000350), the first paragraph in the Method section of Study 4 has been revised. All paragraphs in the Results section of Study 4 have been revised. Table 1 and Figure 5 have been updated. All versions of this article have been corrected. (The following abstract of the original article appeared in record 2024-17814-001.) Functional accounts of emotion have guided research for decades, with the core assumption that emotions are functional-they improve outcomes for people. Based on functional accounts of emotion, we theorized that anger should improve goal attainment in the presence of challenges. In seven studies, goal attainment was assessed in situations that involved varying levels of challenges to goal attainment. Across studies, anger compared to a neutral condition resulted in behavior that facilitated greater goal attainment on tasks that involved challenges. With a goal to solve difficult puzzles, anger resulted in more puzzles correctly solved (Study 1). With a goal to attain prizes, anger increased cheating rates and numbers of unearned prizes (Study 2). With a goal to do well in a video game, anger increased scores on a game with challenges to be avoided, but not other scores (Study 3). In two studies, examining the consequences of anger in response to the challenging task that was the focus of that anger, anger decreased reaction time with goals to win trials (Study 4), and predicted making the effort to vote in two contentious elections (Study 5). With a goal to protect financial resources, anger increased action taken to prevent loss compared to a physiological arousal condition (Study 6). (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

报告了 Heather C. Lench、Noah T. Reed、Tiffany George、Kaitlyn A. Kaiser 和 Sophia G. North 所著《愤怒有利于实现目标》(《人格与社会心理学杂志》,高级在线出版,2023 年 10 月 30 日,np)中的一处错误。在文章(https://doi.org/10.1037/pspa 0000350)中,研究 4 的方法部分的第一段已经修改。修改了研究 4 "结果 "部分的所有段落。表 1 和图 5 已更新。本文所有版本均已更正。(原文摘要如下,载于 2024-17814-001 号记录)。数十年来,情绪的功能性描述一直指导着相关研究,其核心假设是情绪具有功能性--它们能改善人们的结果。基于情绪的功能性描述,我们推测,在面临挑战时,愤怒应能改善目标的实现。在七项研究中,我们在目标实现过程中遇到不同程度挑战的情况下对目标实现情况进行了评估。在所有研究中,与中性条件相比,愤怒导致的行为有利于在涉及挑战的任务中更大程度地实现目标。在以解决困难谜题为目标的情况下,愤怒会导致更多谜题被正确解决(研究 1)。如果目标是获得奖品,愤怒会增加作弊率和未获得奖品的数量(研究 2)。如果目标是在电子游戏中取得好成绩,那么愤怒会提高游戏中需要避免的挑战的得分,但不会提高其他得分(研究 3)。在两项研究中,研究人员对愤怒所带来的后果进行了调查,结果显示,愤怒会减少以赢得比赛为目标的反应时间(研究 4),并预测在两次有争议的选举中投票的努力(研究 5)。与生理唤醒条件相比,在保护财务资源的目标下,愤怒会增加为防止损失而采取的行动(研究 6)。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Correction to \"Anger has benefits for attaining goals\" by Lench et al. (2023).","authors":"","doi":"10.1037/pspa0000400","DOIUrl":"10.1037/pspa0000400","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Reports an error in \"Anger has benefits for attaining goals\" by Heather C. Lench, Noah T. Reed, Tiffany George, Kaitlyn A. Kaiser and Sophia G. North (<i>Journal of Personality and Social Psychology</i>, Advanced Online Publication, Oct 30, 2023, np). In the article (https://doi.org/10.1037/pspa 0000350), the first paragraph in the Method section of Study 4 has been revised. All paragraphs in the Results section of Study 4 have been revised. Table 1 and Figure 5 have been updated. All versions of this article have been corrected. (The following abstract of the original article appeared in record 2024-17814-001.) Functional accounts of emotion have guided research for decades, with the core assumption that emotions are functional-they improve outcomes for people. Based on functional accounts of emotion, we theorized that anger should improve goal attainment in the presence of challenges. In seven studies, goal attainment was assessed in situations that involved varying levels of challenges to goal attainment. Across studies, anger compared to a neutral condition resulted in behavior that facilitated greater goal attainment on tasks that involved challenges. With a goal to solve difficult puzzles, anger resulted in more puzzles correctly solved (Study 1). With a goal to attain prizes, anger increased cheating rates and numbers of unearned prizes (Study 2). With a goal to do well in a video game, anger increased scores on a game with challenges to be avoided, but not other scores (Study 3). In two studies, examining the consequences of anger in response to the challenging task that was the focus of that anger, anger decreased reaction time with goals to win trials (Study 4), and predicted making the effort to vote in two contentious elections (Study 5). With a goal to protect financial resources, anger increased action taken to prevent loss compared to a physiological arousal condition (Study 6). (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":16691,"journal":{"name":"Journal of personality and social psychology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":6.4,"publicationDate":"2024-04-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141310938","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
How perceived polarization predicts attitude moralization (and vice versa): A four-wave longitudinal study during the 2020 U.S. election. 感知到的极化如何预测态度道德化(反之亦然):2020 年美国大选期间的四波纵向研究。
IF 6.4 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, SOCIAL Pub Date : 2024-04-01 Epub Date: 2024-02-22 DOI: 10.1037/pspi0000454
Chantal D'Amore, Martijn van Zomeren, Namkje Koudenburg

Within structurally polarized and dynamic contexts, such as the U.S. 2020 presidential elections, the moralization of individuals' attitudes on a specific topic (e.g., climate policy) can dangerously escalate disagreements between groups into zero-sum conflict. However, limited knowledge exists regarding the factors that influence individuals' tendency to moralize their attitudes over time, and what the role of structural polarization is in this psychological process. Our objective is to test a theoretically integrative model of when and how perceived polarization is related to attitude moralization over time within the polarized context of the U.S. 2020 presidential elections and explore reciprocal feedback loops to understand the dynamic relationship between polarization and moralization over time. Our model predicts that, when repeatedly faced with outgroup expressions in the news, individuals' perceptions of polarization will predict within-person attitude moralization over time via strengthening their value-protective responses to these expressions (i.e., perceiving dyadic harm and experiencing negative moral emotions toward the outgroup). To test our model, we conducted a four-wave, 4-month longitudinal study among Biden supporters (N = 1,236) and Trump supporters (N = 617). The results of the within-person analyses generally supported the model's hypotheses across both samples and various attitude topics. Furthermore, cross-lagged structural equation models explored reciprocal influences, revealing positive feedback loops between structural polarization and attitude moralization over time. Our findings thus indicate that perceived polarization strengthens attitude moralization (and vice versa) over time-a dynamic process that helps to explain how nonmoralized conflict between groups can evolve into zero-sum conflict during periods of intense polarization. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

在结构极化和动态的背景下,如美国 2020 年总统大选,个人对特定话题(如气候政策)态度的道德化可能会将群体间的分歧升级为零和冲突,这是很危险的。然而,人们对影响个人态度道德化倾向的因素以及结构性极化在这一心理过程中的作用了解有限。我们的目标是在美国 2020 年总统大选的两极分化背景下,检验感知到的两极分化与态度道德化之间的理论整合模型,并探索相互反馈回路,以了解两极分化与道德化之间随着时间推移的动态关系。我们的模型预测,当反复面对新闻中的外群体表达时,个体对两极分化的感知将通过加强对这些表达的价值保护反应(即感知二元伤害和体验对外群体的负面道德情绪)来预测随着时间推移的人内态度道德化。为了检验我们的模型,我们在拜登支持者(人数=1,236)和特朗普支持者(人数=617)中进行了为期四个波次、四个月的纵向研究。在两个样本和不同的态度主题中,人内分析的结果普遍支持模型的假设。此外,交叉滞后结构方程模型探讨了相互影响,揭示了结构极化和态度道德化之间随着时间推移的正反馈循环。因此,我们的研究结果表明,随着时间的推移,感知到的极化会加强态度道德化(反之亦然)--这一动态过程有助于解释在激烈的极化时期,群体间的非道德化冲突是如何演变成零和冲突的。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"How perceived polarization predicts attitude moralization (and vice versa): A four-wave longitudinal study during the 2020 U.S. election.","authors":"Chantal D'Amore, Martijn van Zomeren, Namkje Koudenburg","doi":"10.1037/pspi0000454","DOIUrl":"10.1037/pspi0000454","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Within structurally polarized and dynamic contexts, such as the U.S. 2020 presidential elections, the moralization of individuals' attitudes on a specific topic (e.g., climate policy) can dangerously escalate disagreements between groups into zero-sum conflict. However, limited knowledge exists regarding the factors that influence individuals' tendency to moralize their attitudes over time, and what the role of structural polarization is in this psychological process. Our objective is to test a theoretically integrative model of when and how perceived polarization is related to attitude moralization over time within the polarized context of the U.S. 2020 presidential elections and explore reciprocal feedback loops to understand the dynamic relationship between polarization and moralization over time. Our model predicts that, when repeatedly faced with outgroup expressions in the news, individuals' perceptions of polarization will predict within-person attitude moralization over time via strengthening their value-protective responses to these expressions (i.e., perceiving dyadic harm and experiencing negative moral emotions toward the outgroup). To test our model, we conducted a four-wave, 4-month longitudinal study among Biden supporters (<i>N</i> = 1,236) and Trump supporters (<i>N</i> = 617). The results of the within-person analyses generally supported the model's hypotheses across both samples and various attitude topics. Furthermore, cross-lagged structural equation models explored reciprocal influences, revealing positive feedback loops between structural polarization and attitude moralization over time. Our findings thus indicate that perceived polarization strengthens attitude moralization (and vice versa) over time-a dynamic process that helps to explain how nonmoralized conflict between groups can evolve into zero-sum conflict during periods of intense polarization. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":16691,"journal":{"name":"Journal of personality and social psychology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":6.4,"publicationDate":"2024-04-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139931589","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Bridging temperament and the Big Five in children: A genetically informative study. 连接儿童的气质和五大特征:基因信息研究
IF 6.4 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, SOCIAL Pub Date : 2024-04-01 DOI: 10.1037/pspp0000500
Margarete E Vollrath, Espen Moen Eilertsen, Svenn Torgersen, Line C Gjerde, Eivind Ystrom

Early temperament precedes children's emerging Big Five personality, but shared models of temperament and personality are scarce. We wanted to estimate the genetic factor structure underlying both temperament and the Big Five in children, employing a genetically informed study. Within the Norwegian Mother and Child Cohort Study, we selected 26,354 twins, siblings, and cousins. Mothers rated their children's temperament three times between the ages of 1.5 and 5 years, and the children's Big Five personality at the age of 8. We analyzed the data using biometric modeling. The mean heritability of single-time temperamental traits and Big Five personality traits was .48 and .45, respectively. The mean genetic correlations of temperament across time were .80. The genetic correlations of temperament at 5 years and the Big Five at 8 years revealed two factors, the first comprising reversed Big Five Neuroticism, Agreeableness, Conscientiousness, and reversed EAS Emotionality, the second comprising Big Five Extraversion, Openness to Experience, EAS Activity, Sociability, and reversed Shyness. A confirmatory factor analysis estimated the two factors showing heritabilities of .96 and .72, respectively. The two factors mirrored the metatraits Stability and Plasticity by John M. Digman. Temperament and personality in childhood can be meaningfully bridged using just two metafactors. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

早期气质先于儿童的大五人格出现,但气质和人格的共享模型却很少。我们希望通过一项遗传学研究来估测儿童气质和大五人格的遗传因子结构。在挪威母子队列研究(Norwegian Mother and Child Cohort Study)中,我们选取了26354对双胞胎、兄弟姐妹和表兄弟姐妹。母亲在孩子1.5岁至5岁期间对其脾气进行了三次评分,并在孩子8岁时对其大五人格进行了评分。单次性情特征和大五人格特征的平均遗传率分别为 0.48 和 0.45。不同时期气质的平均遗传相关性为 0.80。5 岁时的气质与 8 岁时的大五人格的遗传相关性显示出两个因子,第一个因子由反向大五人格神经质、宜人性、自觉性和反向 EAS 情绪性组成,第二个因子由大五人格外向性、经验开放性、EAS 活动性、社交性和反向害羞性组成。通过确认性因子分析估计,这两个因子的遗传率分别为 0.96 和 0.72。这两个因子反映了约翰-M-迪格曼(John M. Digman)所著的《稳定性与可塑性》(Stability and Plasticity)元特质。只需使用两个元因子,就能将童年时期的气质和个性有意义地联系起来。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Bridging temperament and the Big Five in children: A genetically informative study.","authors":"Margarete E Vollrath, Espen Moen Eilertsen, Svenn Torgersen, Line C Gjerde, Eivind Ystrom","doi":"10.1037/pspp0000500","DOIUrl":"10.1037/pspp0000500","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Early temperament precedes children's emerging Big Five personality, but shared models of temperament and personality are scarce. We wanted to estimate the genetic factor structure underlying both temperament and the Big Five in children, employing a genetically informed study. Within the Norwegian Mother and Child Cohort Study, we selected 26,354 twins, siblings, and cousins. Mothers rated their children's temperament three times between the ages of 1.5 and 5 years, and the children's Big Five personality at the age of 8. We analyzed the data using biometric modeling. The mean heritability of single-time temperamental traits and Big Five personality traits was .48 and .45, respectively. The mean genetic correlations of temperament across time were .80. The genetic correlations of temperament at 5 years and the Big Five at 8 years revealed two factors, the first comprising reversed Big Five Neuroticism, Agreeableness, Conscientiousness, and reversed EAS Emotionality, the second comprising Big Five Extraversion, Openness to Experience, EAS Activity, Sociability, and reversed Shyness. A confirmatory factor analysis estimated the two factors showing heritabilities of .96 and .72, respectively. The two factors mirrored the metatraits Stability and Plasticity by John M. Digman. Temperament and personality in childhood can be meaningfully bridged using just two metafactors. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":16691,"journal":{"name":"Journal of personality and social psychology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":6.4,"publicationDate":"2024-04-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141310937","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Most people's life satisfaction matches their personality traits: True correlations in multitrait, multirater, multisample data. 大多数人的生活满意度与其人格特质相匹配:多特征、多参数、多样本数据中的真实相关性。
IF 6.4 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, SOCIAL Pub Date : 2024-04-01 DOI: 10.1037/pspp0000501
René Mõttus, Anu Realo, Jüri Allik, Liisi Ausmees, Samuel Henry, Robert R McCrae, Uku Vainik

Despite numerous meta-analyses, the true extent to which life satisfaction reflects personality traits has remained unclear due to overreliance on a single method to assess both and insufficient attention to construct overlaps. Using data from three samples tested in different languages (Estonian, N = 20,886; Russian, N = 768; English, N = 600), we combined self- and informant-reports to estimate personality domains' and nuances' true correlations (rtrue) with general life satisfaction (LS) and satisfactions with eight life domains (DSs), while controlling for single-method and occasion-specific biases and random error, and avoiding direct construct overlaps. The associations replicated well across samples. The Big Five domains and nuances allowed predicting LS with accuracies up to rtrue ≈ .80-.90 in independent (sub)samples. Emotional stability, extraversion, and conscientiousness correlated rtrue ≈ .30-.50 with LS, while its correlations with openness and agreeableness were small. At the nuances level, low LS was most strongly associated with feeling misunderstood, unexcited, indecisive, envious, bored, used, unable, and unrewarded (rtrue ≈ .40-.70). Supporting LS's construct validity, DSs had similar personality correlates among themselves and with LS, and an aggregated DS correlated rtrue ≈ .90 with LS. LS's approximately 10-year stability was rtrue = .70 and its longitudinal associations with personality traits mirrored cross-sectional ones. We conclude that without common measurement limitations, most people's life satisfaction is highly consistent with their personality traits, even across many years. So, satisfaction is usually shaped by these same relatively stable factors that shape personality traits more broadly. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

尽管进行了大量的荟萃分析,但生活满意度反映人格特质的真实程度仍不明确,原因是过度依赖单一方法来评估二者,以及对建构重叠关注不够。利用三个不同语言测试样本(爱沙尼亚语,N = 20,886;俄语,N = 768;英语,N = 600)的数据,我们将自我报告和信息提供者报告结合起来,估计了人格领域和细微差别与一般生活满意度(LS)和八个生活领域满意度(DSs)的真实相关性(rtrue),同时控制了单一方法和特定场合的偏差和随机误差,并避免了直接的结构重叠。这些关联在不同样本中得到了很好的复制。在独立(子)样本中,大五领域和细微差别预测 LS 的准确率可达 rtrue ≈.80-.90。情绪稳定性、外向性和自觉性与 LS 的相关性 rtrue ≈ .30-.50,而与开放性和宜人性的相关性较小。在细微差别层面上,低 LS 与感觉被误解、不兴奋、优柔寡断、嫉妒、无聊、被利用、无能和无回报(rtrue ≈ .40-.70)的相关性最强。为支持LS的建构有效性,DSs之间以及DSs与LS之间具有相似的人格相关性,DS与LS的综合相关性rtrue≈.90。LS约10年的稳定性为rtrue = .70,其与人格特质的纵向关联反映了横截面关联。我们的结论是,如果没有常见的测量限制,大多数人的生活满意度与他们的人格特质高度一致,甚至可以跨越许多年。因此,满意度通常也是由这些相对稳定的因素形成的,而这些因素更广泛地影响着人格特质。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Most people's life satisfaction matches their personality traits: True correlations in multitrait, multirater, multisample data.","authors":"René Mõttus, Anu Realo, Jüri Allik, Liisi Ausmees, Samuel Henry, Robert R McCrae, Uku Vainik","doi":"10.1037/pspp0000501","DOIUrl":"10.1037/pspp0000501","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Despite numerous meta-analyses, the true extent to which life satisfaction reflects personality traits has remained unclear due to overreliance on a single method to assess both and insufficient attention to construct overlaps. Using data from three samples tested in different languages (Estonian, N = 20,886; Russian, N = 768; English, N = 600), we combined self- and informant-reports to estimate personality domains' and nuances' true correlations (r<sub>true</sub>) with general life satisfaction (LS) and satisfactions with eight life domains (DSs), while controlling for single-method and occasion-specific biases and random error, and avoiding direct construct overlaps. The associations replicated well across samples. The Big Five domains and nuances allowed predicting LS with accuracies up to r<sub>true</sub> ≈ .80-.90 in independent (sub)samples. Emotional stability, extraversion, and conscientiousness correlated r<sub>true</sub> ≈ .30-.50 with LS, while its correlations with openness and agreeableness were small. At the nuances level, low LS was most strongly associated with feeling misunderstood, unexcited, indecisive, envious, bored, used, unable, and unrewarded (r<sub>true</sub> ≈ .40-.70). Supporting LS's construct validity, DSs had similar personality correlates among themselves and with LS, and an aggregated DS correlated r<sub>true</sub> ≈ .90 with LS. LS's approximately 10-year stability was r<sub>true</sub> = .70 and its longitudinal associations with personality traits mirrored cross-sectional ones. We conclude that without common measurement limitations, most people's life satisfaction is highly consistent with their personality traits, even across many years. So, satisfaction is usually shaped by these same relatively stable factors that shape personality traits more broadly. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":16691,"journal":{"name":"Journal of personality and social psychology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":6.4,"publicationDate":"2024-04-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141310939","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Journal of personality and social psychology
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1