首页 > 最新文献

Journal of personality and social psychology最新文献

英文 中文
"A model of (often mixed) stereotype content: Competence and warmth respectively follow from perceived status and competition": Correction to Fiske et al. (2002). "刻板印象内容(往往是混合的)模型:能力和热情分别来自于所感知的地位和竞争":对 Fiske 等人(2002 年)的更正。
IF 7.6 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, SOCIAL Pub Date : 2024-03-01 Epub Date: 2019-04-25 DOI: 10.1037/pspa0000163
Susan T Fiske, Amy J Cuddy, Glick Peter, Jun Xu

Reports an error in "A model of (often mixed) stereotype content: Competence and warmth respectively follow from perceived status and competition" by Susan T. Fiske, Amy J. C. Cuddy, Peter Glick and Jun Xu (Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 2002[Jun], Vol 82[6], 878-902). In the fourth paragraph of the Status Predicts Competence, and Competition Predicts Warmth section, the results are worded in a confusing way, and some values are wrong. In the fourth paragraph's first sentence, all correlation coefficients mistakenly omitted the negative sign implied in the text ("negatively correlated") and shown in the correct values reported in Table 6. The text should appear instead as follows: Perceived competition negatively correlated with perceived warmth for the student sample, group-level r(21) .68, p < .001; individual-level r(71) .22, p < .1, and the nonstudent sample, group-level r(21) .53, p < .001; individual-level r(36) .11, ns. (The following abstract of the original article appeared in record 2002-02942-002.) Stereotype research emphasizes systematic processes over seemingly arbitrary contents, but content also may prove systematic. On the basis of stereotypes' intergroup functions, the stereotype content model hypothesizes that (1) 2 primary dimensions are competence and warmth, (2) frequent mixed clusters combine high warmth with low competence (paternalistic) or high competence with low warmth (envious), and (3) distinct emotions (pity, envy, admiration, contempt) differentiate the 4 competence-warmth combinations. Stereotypically, (4) status predicts high competence, and competition predicts low warmth. Nine varied samples rated gender, ethnicity, race, class, age, and disability out-groups. Contrary to antipathy models, 2 dimensions mattered, and many stereotypes were mixed, either pitying (low competence, high warmth subordinates) or envying (high competence, low warmth competitors). Stereotypically, status predicted competence, and competition predicted low warmth. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

报告了 Susan T. Fiske、Amy J. C. Cuddy、Peter Glick 和 Jun Xu 合著的 "刻板印象内容(通常是混合的)模型:Susan T. Fiske、Amy J. C. Cuddy、Peter Glick 和 Jun Xu 合著的 "A model of stereotype content:Competence and warmth respectively follow from perceived status and competition"(《人格与社会心理学杂志》,2002 年 6 月,第 82 卷[6],878-902 页)中的错误。在 "地位预测能力 "和 "竞争预测温暖 "部分的第四段中,结果的措辞比较混乱,有些数值是错误的。在第四段的第一句中,所有相关系数都错误地省略了文本中暗示的负号("负相关"),表 6 中报告的正确值也显示了负号。应改为在学生样本中,感知竞争与感知温暖负相关,群体水平 r(21) .68,p < .001;个体水平 r(71) .22,p < .1;在非学生样本中,群体水平 r(21) .53,p < .001;个体水平 r(36) .11,ns。(以下为 2002-02942-002 号记录中的原文摘要)。刻板印象研究强调的是系统过程,而不是看似任意的内容,但内容也可能被证明是系统的。根据刻板印象的群体间功能,刻板印象内容模型假设:(1) 能力和温暖是两个主要维度;(2) 高温暖与低能力(家长式)或高能力与低温暖(妒忌)经常混合成群;(3) 不同的情绪(怜悯、妒忌、钦佩、蔑视)区分了 4 种能力-温暖组合。在刻板印象中,(4) 地位预示着高能力,竞争预示着低热情。九个不同的样本对性别、民族、种族、阶级、年龄和残疾外群体进行了评级。与反感模型相反,有两个维度是重要的,而且许多刻板印象是混合的,要么是同情(低能力、高热情的下属),要么是羡慕(高能力、低热情的竞争者)。在刻板印象中,地位预示着能力,竞争预示着低热情。(PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2019 APA, all rights reserved)。
{"title":"\"A model of (often mixed) stereotype content: Competence and warmth respectively follow from perceived status and competition\": Correction to Fiske et al. (2002).","authors":"Susan T Fiske, Amy J Cuddy, Glick Peter, Jun Xu","doi":"10.1037/pspa0000163","DOIUrl":"10.1037/pspa0000163","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Reports an error in \"A model of (often mixed) stereotype content: Competence and warmth respectively follow from perceived status and competition\" by Susan T. Fiske, Amy J. C. Cuddy, Peter Glick and Jun Xu (<i>Journal of Personality and Social Psychology</i>, 2002[Jun], Vol 82[6], 878-902). In the fourth paragraph of the Status Predicts Competence, and Competition Predicts Warmth section, the results are worded in a confusing way, and some values are wrong. In the fourth paragraph's first sentence, all correlation coefficients mistakenly omitted the negative sign implied in the text (\"negatively correlated\") and shown in the correct values reported in Table 6. The text should appear instead as follows: Perceived competition negatively correlated with perceived warmth for the student sample, group-level <i>r</i>(21) .68, <i>p</i> < .001; individual-level <i>r</i>(71) .22, <i>p</i> < .1, and the nonstudent sample, group-level <i>r</i>(21) .53, <i>p</i> < .001; individual-level <i>r</i>(36) .11, <i>ns</i>. (The following abstract of the original article appeared in record 2002-02942-002.) Stereotype research emphasizes systematic processes over seemingly arbitrary contents, but content also may prove systematic. On the basis of stereotypes' intergroup functions, the stereotype content model hypothesizes that (1) 2 primary dimensions are competence and warmth, (2) frequent mixed clusters combine high warmth with low competence (paternalistic) or high competence with low warmth (envious), and (3) distinct emotions (pity, envy, admiration, contempt) differentiate the 4 competence-warmth combinations. Stereotypically, (4) status predicts high competence, and competition predicts low warmth. Nine varied samples rated gender, ethnicity, race, class, age, and disability out-groups. Contrary to antipathy models, 2 dimensions mattered, and many stereotypes were mixed, either pitying (low competence, high warmth subordinates) or envying (high competence, low warmth competitors). Stereotypically, status predicted competence, and competition predicted low warmth. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":16691,"journal":{"name":"Journal of personality and social psychology","volume":" ","pages":"412"},"PeriodicalIF":7.6,"publicationDate":"2024-03-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"37183300","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Codevelopment of life goals and the Big Five personality traits across adulthood and old age. 生活目标和五大人格特质在成年期和老年期的编码发展。
IF 6.4 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, SOCIAL Pub Date : 2024-02-01 Epub Date: 2023-07-27 DOI: 10.1037/pspp0000477
Laura Buchinger, Theresa M Entringer, David Richter, Gert G Wagner, Denis Gerstorf, Wiebke Bleidorn

Since the new millennium, research in the field of personality development has focused on the stability and change of basic personality traits. Motivational aspects of personality and their longitudinal association with basic traits have received comparably little attention. In this preregistered study, we applied bivariate latent growth curve model to investigated the codevelopment of nine life goals and the Big Five traits. We tested age, perceived control, gender, educational background, and regional socialization as potential moderators of codevelopment. Data came from the German Socio-Economic Panel study (N = 55,040, age range: 18-103 years) and span a study period of 13 years. During this period, the Big Five traits and life goals were assessed four times. Our findings suggest that development in broader life goal domains (e.g., self-fulfillment) is more strongly connected to personality development across the life span, whereas changes in specific goals (e.g., having children) are more closely tied to trait changes during young and middle adulthood. The strongest codevelopment was found between Openness and agentic goals with a focus on personal growth followed by codevelopment between Agreeableness and communal goals. Developmental stage and educational background moderated the codevelopment of Conscientiousness and economic achievement as well as family-related goals. Contrary to the previous research, we found that Neuroticism codeveloped with communal life goals (i.e., having a happy relationship/marriage). Our findings reinforce theoretical frameworks that highlight the role of changing opportunities, constraints, and developmental tasks across adulthood. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

新千年以来,人格发展领域的研究主要集中在基本人格特质的稳定性和变化上。相对而言,人格的动机方面及其与基本特质的纵向关联却很少受到关注。在这项预先登记的研究中,我们采用了双变量潜成长曲线模型来研究九个人生目标和五大特质的编码发展。我们测试了年龄、感知控制、性别、教育背景和地区社会化等因素对编码发展的潜在调节作用。数据来自德国社会经济小组研究(N = 55,040 人,年龄范围:18-103 岁),研究时间跨度为 13 年。在此期间,对五大特质和生活目标进行了四次评估。我们的研究结果表明,更广泛的生活目标领域(如自我实现)的发展与整个生命周期的人格发展联系更为紧密,而特定目标(如生儿育女)的变化与青年和中年时期的特质变化联系更为紧密。在开放性和以个人成长为重点的行动目标之间发现了最强烈的编码发展,其次是宜人性和公共目标之间的编码发展。发展阶段和教育背景调节了 "自觉性 "与经济成就以及家庭相关目标之间的编码发展。与以往的研究相反,我们发现神经质与社区生活目标(即拥有幸福的关系/婚姻)之间存在着编码发展关系。我们的研究结果强化了强调机会、限制和发展任务在整个成年期不断变化的作用的理论框架。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Codevelopment of life goals and the Big Five personality traits across adulthood and old age.","authors":"Laura Buchinger, Theresa M Entringer, David Richter, Gert G Wagner, Denis Gerstorf, Wiebke Bleidorn","doi":"10.1037/pspp0000477","DOIUrl":"10.1037/pspp0000477","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Since the new millennium, research in the field of personality development has focused on the stability and change of basic personality traits. Motivational aspects of personality and their longitudinal association with basic traits have received comparably little attention. In this preregistered study, we applied bivariate latent growth curve model to investigated the codevelopment of nine life goals and the Big Five traits. We tested age, perceived control, gender, educational background, and regional socialization as potential moderators of codevelopment. Data came from the German Socio-Economic Panel study (<i>N</i> = 55,040, age range: 18-103 years) and span a study period of 13 years. During this period, the Big Five traits and life goals were assessed four times. Our findings suggest that development in broader life goal domains (e.g., self-fulfillment) is more strongly connected to personality development across the life span, whereas changes in specific goals (e.g., having children) are more closely tied to trait changes during young and middle adulthood. The strongest codevelopment was found between Openness and agentic goals with a focus on personal growth followed by codevelopment between Agreeableness and communal goals. Developmental stage and educational background moderated the codevelopment of Conscientiousness and economic achievement as well as family-related goals. Contrary to the previous research, we found that Neuroticism codeveloped with communal life goals (i.e., having a happy relationship/marriage). Our findings reinforce theoretical frameworks that highlight the role of changing opportunities, constraints, and developmental tasks across adulthood. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":16691,"journal":{"name":"Journal of personality and social psychology","volume":" ","pages":"346-368"},"PeriodicalIF":6.4,"publicationDate":"2024-02-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9882824","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
A deep learning approach to personality assessment: Generalizing across items and expanding the reach of survey-based research. 人格评估的深度学习方法:对不同项目进行归纳,扩大基于调查的研究范围。
IF 6.4 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, SOCIAL Pub Date : 2024-02-01 Epub Date: 2023-09-07 DOI: 10.1037/pspp0000480
Suhaib Abdurahman, Huy Vu, Wanling Zou, Lyle Ungar, Sudeep Bhatia

Traditional methods of personality assessment, and survey-based research in general, cannot make inferences about new items that have not been surveyed previously. This limits the amount of information that can be obtained from a given survey. In this article, we tackle this problem by leveraging recent advances in statistical natural language processing. Specifically, we extract "embedding" representations of questionnaire items from deep neural networks, trained on large-scale English language data. These embeddings allow us to construct a high-dimensional space of items, in which linguistically similar items are located near each other. We combine item embeddings with machine learning algorithms to extrapolate participant ratings of personality items to completely new items that have not been rated by any participants. The accuracy of our approach is on par with incentivized human judges given an identical task, indicating that it predicts ratings of new personality items as accurately as people do. Our approach is also capable of identifying psychological constructs associated with questionnaire items and can accurately cluster items into their constructs based only on their language content. Overall, our results show how representations of linguistic personality descriptors obtained from deep language models can be used to model and predict a large variety of traits, scales, and constructs. In doing so, they showcase a new scalable and cost-effective method for psychological measurement. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

传统的人格评估方法,以及基于调查的一般研究,无法对以前未调查过的新项目做出推断。这就限制了从特定调查中获取的信息量。在本文中,我们利用统计自然语言处理的最新进展来解决这一问题。具体来说,我们从深度神经网络中提取问卷项目的 "嵌入 "表征,并在大规模英语语言数据中进行了训练。通过这些嵌入,我们可以构建一个高维度的项目空间,在这个空间中,语言相似的项目彼此靠近。我们将项目嵌入与机器学习算法相结合,将参与者对个性项目的评分推断到未被任何参与者评分的全新项目上。我们方法的准确性与接受相同任务的受激励人类评委不相上下,表明它预测新个性项目评分的准确性不亚于人类。我们的方法还能识别与问卷项目相关的心理结构,并能仅根据语言内容就准确地将项目归类到其结构中。总之,我们的研究结果表明,从深度语言模型中获得的语言人格描述符表征可用于模拟和预测大量特质、量表和构造。在此过程中,它们展示了一种新的可扩展且具有成本效益的心理测量方法。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA,保留所有权利)。
{"title":"A deep learning approach to personality assessment: Generalizing across items and expanding the reach of survey-based research.","authors":"Suhaib Abdurahman, Huy Vu, Wanling Zou, Lyle Ungar, Sudeep Bhatia","doi":"10.1037/pspp0000480","DOIUrl":"10.1037/pspp0000480","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Traditional methods of personality assessment, and survey-based research in general, cannot make inferences about new items that have not been surveyed previously. This limits the amount of information that can be obtained from a given survey. In this article, we tackle this problem by leveraging recent advances in statistical natural language processing. Specifically, we extract \"embedding\" representations of questionnaire items from deep neural networks, trained on large-scale English language data. These embeddings allow us to construct a high-dimensional space of items, in which linguistically similar items are located near each other. We combine item embeddings with machine learning algorithms to extrapolate participant ratings of personality items to completely new items that have not been rated by any participants. The accuracy of our approach is on par with incentivized human judges given an identical task, indicating that it predicts ratings of new personality items as accurately as people do. Our approach is also capable of identifying psychological constructs associated with questionnaire items and can accurately cluster items into their constructs based only on their language content. Overall, our results show how representations of linguistic personality descriptors obtained from deep language models can be used to model and predict a large variety of traits, scales, and constructs. In doing so, they showcase a new scalable and cost-effective method for psychological measurement. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":16691,"journal":{"name":"Journal of personality and social psychology","volume":" ","pages":"312-331"},"PeriodicalIF":6.4,"publicationDate":"2024-02-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10168495","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Boosting yourself? Associations between momentary self-esteem, daily social interactions, and self-esteem development in late adolescence and late adulthood. 鼓舞自己?青少年后期和成年后期的瞬时自尊、日常社交互动和自尊发展之间的关系。
IF 6.4 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, SOCIAL Pub Date : 2024-02-01 Epub Date: 2023-09-04 DOI: 10.1037/pspp0000481
Jenny Wagner, Larissa L Wieczorek, Naemi D Brandt

Research over the past 2 decades has repeatedly shown that the evaluation of one's own worth-trait self-esteem-is closely linked to the quality of social relationships and perceptions of social inclusion. However, there is limited evidence on the dynamics between momentary self-esteem and perceptions of social inclusion in everyday life, as well as on their possible long-term (bottom-up) effects on the development of trait self-esteem. We addressed this research gap using longitudinal data from a German multimethodological study (N = 324) in which N = 235 late adolescents (Mage = 17.7; 76% female) and N = 89 older adults (Mage = 63.8; 64% female) were followed over 1 year. Based on three trait questionnaires with 6-month intervals and a 7-day experience-sampling burst at the first measurement point, we investigated momentary dynamics in self-esteem and longitudinal change by using multilevel and latent growth modeling. Results confirmed the positive association between momentary self-esteem and perceptions of social inclusion in everyday life, that is, self-esteem reactivity in both age groups. In addition, both self- and other-reports showed a consistent increase in trait self-esteem over 1 year. However, because the slope parameters did not indicate substantial interindividual variance, we were unable to test for bottom-up effects of self-esteem reactivity. We discuss the importance of daily social experiences for momentary self-esteem in late adolescence and late adulthood but also point to the need for further multimethodological research. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

过去20年的研究一再表明,对自身价值特质自尊的评估与社会关系的质量和社会包容的感知密切相关。然而,关于日常生活中瞬时自尊和社会包容感之间的动态,以及它们对特质自尊发展可能产生的长期(自下而上)影响,证据有限。我们使用德国一项多方法研究(N=324)的纵向数据来解决这一研究差距,该研究对N=235名晚期青少年(Mage=17.7;76%为女性)和N=89名老年人(Mage=63.8;64%为女性)进行了一年多的随访。基于三份间隔6个月的特质问卷和在第一个测量点的7天经验抽样,我们使用多层次和潜在增长模型研究了自尊和纵向变化的瞬时动态。结果证实了瞬间自尊与日常生活中的社会包容感之间的正相关,即两个年龄组的自尊反应。此外,自我和其他报告都显示,在一年多的时间里,特质自尊持续增加。然而,由于斜率参数没有显示出显著的个体间差异,我们无法测试自尊反应的自下而上效应。我们讨论了日常社会经历对青春期晚期和成年晚期短暂自尊的重要性,但也指出了进一步多方法研究的必要性。(PsycInfo数据库记录(c)2023 APA,保留所有权利)。
{"title":"Boosting yourself? Associations between momentary self-esteem, daily social interactions, and self-esteem development in late adolescence and late adulthood.","authors":"Jenny Wagner, Larissa L Wieczorek, Naemi D Brandt","doi":"10.1037/pspp0000481","DOIUrl":"10.1037/pspp0000481","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Research over the past 2 decades has repeatedly shown that the evaluation of one's own worth-trait self-esteem-is closely linked to the quality of social relationships and perceptions of social inclusion. However, there is limited evidence on the dynamics between momentary self-esteem and perceptions of social inclusion in everyday life, as well as on their possible long-term (bottom-up) effects on the development of trait self-esteem. We addressed this research gap using longitudinal data from a German multimethodological study (<i>N</i> = 324) in which <i>N</i> = 235 late adolescents (<i>M</i><sub>age</sub> = 17.7; 76% female) and <i>N</i> = 89 older adults (<i>M</i><sub>age</sub> = 63.8; 64% female) were followed over 1 year. Based on three trait questionnaires with 6-month intervals and a 7-day experience-sampling burst at the first measurement point, we investigated momentary dynamics in self-esteem and longitudinal change by using multilevel and latent growth modeling. Results confirmed the positive association between momentary self-esteem and perceptions of social inclusion in everyday life, that is, self-esteem reactivity in both age groups. In addition, both self- and other-reports showed a consistent increase in trait self-esteem over 1 year. However, because the slope parameters did not indicate substantial interindividual variance, we were unable to test for bottom-up effects of self-esteem reactivity. We discuss the importance of daily social experiences for momentary self-esteem in late adolescence and late adulthood but also point to the need for further multimethodological research. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":16691,"journal":{"name":"Journal of personality and social psychology","volume":" ","pages":"332-345"},"PeriodicalIF":6.4,"publicationDate":"2024-02-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10152528","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The role of trust in reducing confrontation-related social costs. 信任在降低与对抗相关的社会成本中的作用。
IF 6.4 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, SOCIAL Pub Date : 2024-02-01 Epub Date: 2023-08-03 DOI: 10.1037/pspi0000429
Laura K Hildebrand, Margo J Monteith, Ximena B Arriaga

Confronting, or calling out people for prejudiced remarks, reduces subsequent expressions of prejudice. However, people who confront others incur social costs: Confronters are disliked, derogated, and avoided relative to others who have not confronted. These social costs hurt the confronter and reduce the likelihood of future confrontation. The present studies (N = 1,019) integrate the close relationships and prejudice reduction literatures to examine whether people who are confronted assign fewer social costs when they trust the confronter. Study 1 provided correlational evidence that people who were confronted for making a sexist remark experienced less irritation and annoyance (i.e., negative other-directed affect) if they trusted the confronter, which, in turn, reduced social costs. Manipulation of trust in Study 2 with non-Black participants provided causal evidence that trust buffers against social costs. Being confronted predictably led to more negative other-directed affect and social costs, relative to not-confronted participants; however, these effects were mitigated among participants who underwent a trust-building exercise with the confronter. Study 3 used an ecologically valid context in which non-Black participants who made a stereotypic remark were confronted by an actual friend or stranger. They assigned fewer social costs when confronted by their friend (vs. stranger), and this effect was serially mediated by trust and negative other-directed affect. Importantly, confrontation reduced subsequent stereotyping in all studies. Practically, these studies reveal that when confronters establish trust, they experience fewer social costs. Theoretically, these studies provide a new direction for confrontation research that accounts for interpersonal dynamics. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

与人对质,或指出他人的偏见言论,可以减少随后的偏见表达。然而,与他人对峙的人会付出社会代价:相对于其他没有对抗的人,对抗者会被厌恶、贬低和回避。这些社会成本伤害了对抗者,并降低了未来对抗的可能性。目前的研究(N = 1,019)整合了亲密关系和减少偏见的文献,以考察被对抗者在信任对抗者时是否会付出更少的社会成本。研究 1 提供了相关证据,证明因性别歧视言论而被对质的人如果信任对质者,就会减少恼怒和烦恼(即他人导向的负面情绪),进而降低社会成本。在 "研究 2 "中,对非黑人参与者的信任操纵提供了因果证据,证明信任可以缓冲社会成本。相对于未与之对质的参与者,与之对质可预见会导致更多的负面他人导向情绪和社会成本;然而,这些影响在与对质者建立信任的参与者中得到了缓解。研究 3 采用了一种生态学上有效的情境,让发表刻板言论的非黑人参与者与真实的朋友或陌生人面对面。在与朋友(相对于陌生人)对峙时,他们付出的社会成本更少,而且这种效应通过信任和他人导向的消极情绪进行串联。重要的是,在所有研究中,面对面都会减少随后的刻板印象。实际上,这些研究揭示了当对抗者建立起信任时,他们所经历的社会成本就会减少。从理论上讲,这些研究为考虑人际动态的对抗研究提供了一个新的方向。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"The role of trust in reducing confrontation-related social costs.","authors":"Laura K Hildebrand, Margo J Monteith, Ximena B Arriaga","doi":"10.1037/pspi0000429","DOIUrl":"10.1037/pspi0000429","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Confronting, or calling out people for prejudiced remarks, reduces subsequent expressions of prejudice. However, people who confront others incur social costs: Confronters are disliked, derogated, and avoided relative to others who have not confronted. These social costs hurt the confronter and reduce the likelihood of future confrontation. The present studies (<i>N</i> = 1,019) integrate the close relationships and prejudice reduction literatures to examine whether people who are confronted assign fewer social costs when they trust the confronter. Study 1 provided correlational evidence that people who were confronted for making a sexist remark experienced less irritation and annoyance (i.e., negative other-directed affect) if they trusted the confronter, which, in turn, reduced social costs. Manipulation of trust in Study 2 with non-Black participants provided causal evidence that trust buffers against social costs. Being confronted predictably led to more negative other-directed affect and social costs, relative to not-confronted participants; however, these effects were mitigated among participants who underwent a trust-building exercise with the confronter. Study 3 used an ecologically valid context in which non-Black participants who made a stereotypic remark were confronted by an actual friend or stranger. They assigned fewer social costs when confronted by their friend (vs. stranger), and this effect was serially mediated by trust and negative other-directed affect. Importantly, confrontation reduced subsequent stereotyping in all studies. Practically, these studies reveal that when confronters establish trust, they experience fewer social costs. Theoretically, these studies provide a new direction for confrontation research that accounts for interpersonal dynamics. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":16691,"journal":{"name":"Journal of personality and social psychology","volume":" ","pages":"240-261"},"PeriodicalIF":6.4,"publicationDate":"2024-02-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9988100","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Distinguishing four types of Person × Situation interactions: An integrative framework and empirical examination. 区分 "人×情境 "互动的四种类型:综合框架与实证研究。
IF 6.4 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, SOCIAL Pub Date : 2024-02-01 Epub Date: 2023-09-04 DOI: 10.1037/pspp0000473
Niclas Kuper, Alina S von Garrel, Brenton M Wiernik, Le Vy Phan, Nick Modersitzki, John F Rauthmann

People differ in their reaction to situations, resulting in Person × Situation interactions. These interactions have been emphasized by many theoretical accounts of personality. Nevertheless, empirical progress on Person × Situation interactions has been slow. This is in part attributable to an insufficient distinction of person and situation variables and of different types of interaction effects. We propose a framework distinguishing four nested types of interaction effects varying in specificity: (a) P × S: broad Person × Situation interaction variance, (b) P × Sspec: between-person differences in situation variable-outcome associations, (c) Pspec × S: between-situation differences in person variable-outcome associations, and (d) Pspec × Sspec: specific Person Variable × Situation Variable interactions. We conducted two large online studies (N = 622 and N = 818) with standardized situation stimuli (N = 62 pictures and N = 62 first-person perspective videos) and assessed Big Five traits, DIAMONDS situation characteristics, and Big Five states. Using preregistered multilevel models, we systematically quantified the different interaction types. We found (a) large Person × Situation interaction variance in personality states, (b) sizable individual differences in situation characteristic-state contingencies, (c) consistent but smaller between-situation differences in trait-state associations, and (d) some significant but (very) small Personality Trait × Situation Characteristic interactions. Our findings highlight Person × Situation interactions as potentially important predictors of psychological states, although the explanation of these interactions through specific person variables remains difficult. Individual differences in the reaction to situations should be incorporated into our models of personality. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

人们对情境的反应各不相同,从而产生了 "人×情境 "的相互作用。许多人格理论都强调了这些相互作用。然而,关于 "人×情境 "相互作用的实证研究进展缓慢。这部分归因于对人和情境变量以及不同类型的互动效应区分不够。我们提出了一个框架,将四种嵌套类型的互动效应按特异性加以区分:(a) P × S:广泛的 "人×情境 "互动方差;(b) P × Sspec:情境变量-结果关联中的人际差异;(c) Pspec × S:人际变量-结果关联中的情境差异;(d) Pspec × Sspec:特定的 "人际变量×情境变量 "互动。我们进行了两项大型在线研究(N = 622 和 N = 818),使用标准化情境刺激(N = 62 张图片和 N = 62 个第一人称视角视频)评估了大五特质、DIAMONDS 情境特征和大五状态。利用预先注册的多层次模型,我们系统地量化了不同的交互作用类型。我们发现:(a)人格状态中存在较大的 "人×情境 "交互变异;(b)情境特征-状态或然率存在较大的个体差异;(c)特质-状态关联存在一致但较小的情境间差异;以及(d)人格特质×情境特征存在一些显著但(非常)微小的交互作用。我们的研究结果凸显了 "人×情境 "的相互作用可能是预测心理状态的重要因素,尽管通过具体的人的变量来解释这些相互作用仍然很困难。对情境反应的个体差异应纳入我们的人格模型中。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Distinguishing four types of Person × Situation interactions: An integrative framework and empirical examination.","authors":"Niclas Kuper, Alina S von Garrel, Brenton M Wiernik, Le Vy Phan, Nick Modersitzki, John F Rauthmann","doi":"10.1037/pspp0000473","DOIUrl":"10.1037/pspp0000473","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>People differ in their reaction to situations, resulting in Person × Situation interactions. These interactions have been emphasized by many theoretical accounts of personality. Nevertheless, empirical progress on Person × Situation interactions has been slow. This is in part attributable to an insufficient distinction of person and situation variables and of different types of interaction effects. We propose a framework distinguishing four nested types of interaction effects varying in specificity: (a) P × S: broad Person × Situation interaction variance, (b) P × S<sub>spec</sub>: between-person differences in situation variable-outcome associations, (c) P<sub>spec</sub> × S: between-situation differences in person variable-outcome associations, and (d) P<sub>spec</sub> × S<sub>spec</sub>: specific Person Variable × Situation Variable interactions. We conducted two large online studies (<i>N</i> = 622 and <i>N</i> = 818) with standardized situation stimuli (<i>N</i> = 62 pictures and <i>N</i> = 62 first-person perspective videos) and assessed Big Five traits, DIAMONDS situation characteristics, and Big Five states. Using preregistered multilevel models, we systematically quantified the different interaction types. We found (a) large Person × Situation interaction variance in personality states, (b) sizable individual differences in situation characteristic-state contingencies, (c) consistent but smaller between-situation differences in trait-state associations, and (d) some significant but (very) small Personality Trait × Situation Characteristic interactions. Our findings highlight Person × Situation interactions as potentially important predictors of psychological states, although the explanation of these interactions through specific person variables remains difficult. Individual differences in the reaction to situations should be incorporated into our models of personality. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":16691,"journal":{"name":"Journal of personality and social psychology","volume":" ","pages":"282-311"},"PeriodicalIF":6.4,"publicationDate":"2024-02-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10146069","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Do temperament trajectories from late childhood through adolescence predict success in school? Findings from a longitudinal study of Mexican-origin youth. 从童年晚期到青春期的气质轨迹能预测学业成功吗?墨西哥裔青少年纵向研究的结果。
IF 6.4 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, SOCIAL Pub Date : 2024-01-01 Epub Date: 2023-08-17 DOI: 10.1037/pspp0000475
Rongxin Cheng, Katherine M Lawson, Richard W Robins

School achievement has long-term consequences for occupational success, mental health, and overall psychological adjustment. The present study examined the association between temperament trajectories from late childhood through adolescence and academic outcomes during late adolescence and young adulthood. Data come from the California Families Project, a longitudinal study of 674 Mexican-origin youth assessed 12 times from Age 10 to 23, and from school records. Results from latent growth curve models indicate that higher levels of Effortful Control (EC) at Age 10 were associated with better academic achievement (i.e., higher high school grade point average and test scores, greater likelihood of high school graduation and college attendance) in late adolescence and young adulthood. Higher levels of Negative Emotionality (NEM) at Age 10 were associated with worse academic achievement, but this effect did not hold for all facets of NEM. Neither the levels nor slopes of Positive Emotionality (Surgency, Affiliation) consistently predicted school achievement. There were no main effects of the EC or NEM slopes; however, statistically significant interactions between these slopes and parental monitoring emerged. When parental monitoring was low, youth who experienced greater increases in EC (vs. flat or decreasing slopes) had better academic achievement, and youth who experienced greater increases in NEM had worse academic achievement; in contrast, when parents closely monitored their children, changes in EC and NEM were only weakly associated with achievement. Overall, these findings demonstrate that temperament in late childhood, and changes in temperament across adolescence, have important prospective effects on academic achievement. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

学业成绩对职业成功、心理健康和整体心理调整有着长期影响。本研究探讨了从童年晚期到青春期的气质轨迹与青春期晚期和青年期学业成绩之间的关系。数据来自加利福尼亚家庭项目,这是一项对 674 名墨西哥裔青少年进行的纵向研究,从 10 岁到 23 岁共进行了 12 次评估,数据也来自学校记录。潜在成长曲线模型的结果表明,10 岁时较高水平的努力控制(Effortful Control,EC)与青少年后期和青年期较好的学业成绩(即较高的高中平均成绩和考试分数、较高的高中毕业和上大学的可能性)相关。10 岁时消极情绪(NEM)水平越高,学业成绩越差,但这一效应并不在消极情绪的所有方面都有效。积极情绪(Surgency、Affiliation)的水平和斜率都不能一致地预测学业成绩。积极情感(EC)和非积极情感(NEM)斜率没有主效应;但是,这些斜率与父母监督之间出现了统计学意义上的显著交互作用。当父母的监督程度较低时,EC 增加较多(与斜率持平或下降相比)的青少年的学业成绩较好,而 NEM 增加较多的青少年的学业成绩较差;相反,当父母密切监督子女时,EC 和 NEM 的变化与学业成绩的关系较弱。总之,这些研究结果表明,儿童晚期的气质以及整个青春期的气质变化对学业成绩有着重要的前瞻性影响。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2023 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Do temperament trajectories from late childhood through adolescence predict success in school? Findings from a longitudinal study of Mexican-origin youth.","authors":"Rongxin Cheng, Katherine M Lawson, Richard W Robins","doi":"10.1037/pspp0000475","DOIUrl":"10.1037/pspp0000475","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>School achievement has long-term consequences for occupational success, mental health, and overall psychological adjustment. The present study examined the association between temperament trajectories from late childhood through adolescence and academic outcomes during late adolescence and young adulthood. Data come from the California Families Project, a longitudinal study of 674 Mexican-origin youth assessed 12 times from Age 10 to 23, and from school records. Results from latent growth curve models indicate that higher levels of Effortful Control (EC) at Age 10 were associated with better academic achievement (i.e., higher high school grade point average and test scores, greater likelihood of high school graduation and college attendance) in late adolescence and young adulthood. Higher levels of Negative Emotionality (NEM) at Age 10 were associated with worse academic achievement, but this effect did not hold for all facets of NEM. Neither the levels nor slopes of Positive Emotionality (Surgency, Affiliation) consistently predicted school achievement. There were no main effects of the EC or NEM slopes; however, statistically significant interactions between these slopes and parental monitoring emerged. When parental monitoring was low, youth who experienced greater increases in EC (vs. flat or decreasing slopes) had better academic achievement, and youth who experienced greater increases in NEM had worse academic achievement; in contrast, when parents closely monitored their children, changes in EC and NEM were only weakly associated with achievement. Overall, these findings demonstrate that temperament in late childhood, and changes in temperament across adolescence, have important prospective effects on academic achievement. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":16691,"journal":{"name":"Journal of personality and social psychology","volume":" ","pages":"128-149"},"PeriodicalIF":6.4,"publicationDate":"2024-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC10873481/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10123897","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The consequences of heroization for exploitation. 剥削英雄化的后果。
IF 6.4 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, SOCIAL Pub Date : 2024-01-01 Epub Date: 2023-10-26 DOI: 10.1037/pspa0000365
Matthew L Stanley, Aaron C Kay

The hero label has become a pervasive positive stereotype applied to many different groups and occupations, such as nurses, teachers, and members of the military. Although meant to show support, appreciation, and even admiration, we suggest that attaching this label to groups and occupations may actually have problematic consequences. Specifically, we theorize that the hero label may affect beliefs about the internal motivations of these group members that make them more vulnerable to exploitation. These ideas are tested and supported across nine preregistered studies using complementary materials and experimental paradigms. In these studies, we find that: (a) heroization strengthens expectations that teachers, nurses, and military personnel would willingly volunteer for their own exploitation; (b) the hero label and its consequences follow workers even after they transition to a new career (e.g., participants expected a military veteran-relative to a matched nonveteran-to be more willing to volunteer for his own exploitation at his subsequent civilian job, because the veteran was perceived to be more heroic than the matched nonveteran); and (c) occupational heroization-likely because of its impact on beliefs regarding what heroized workers would freely choose to do-reduces opposition to exploitative policies. In short, our studies show that heroization ultimately promotes worse treatment of the very groups that it is meant to venerate. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

英雄标签已经成为一种普遍的积极刻板印象,适用于许多不同的群体和职业,如护士、教师和军人。尽管这是为了表示支持、赞赏甚至钦佩,但我们认为,将这个标签贴在群体和职业上实际上可能会产生问题。具体来说,我们认为英雄标签可能会影响对这些群体成员内部动机的信念,使他们更容易受到剥削。这些想法在使用补充材料和实验范式的九项预先注册的研究中得到了测试和支持。在这些研究中,我们发现:(a)英雄化强化了人们的期望,即教师、护士和军事人员会自愿为自己的剥削服务;(b) 即使在工人过渡到新的职业生涯后,英雄标签及其后果也会随之而来(例如,参与者希望退伍军人相对于匹配的非自愿者更愿意在随后的文职工作中自愿为自己的剥削服务,因为退伍军人被认为比匹配的非志愿者更英雄);(c)职业英雄化可能是因为它影响了关于英雄化工人自由选择做什么的信念,从而减少了对剥削政策的反对。简言之,我们的研究表明,英雄化最终会导致其所崇拜的群体受到更恶劣的待遇。(PsycInfo数据库记录(c)2023 APA,保留所有权利)。
{"title":"The consequences of heroization for exploitation.","authors":"Matthew L Stanley, Aaron C Kay","doi":"10.1037/pspa0000365","DOIUrl":"10.1037/pspa0000365","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The hero label has become a pervasive positive stereotype applied to many different groups and occupations, such as nurses, teachers, and members of the military. Although meant to show support, appreciation, and even admiration, we suggest that attaching this label to groups and occupations may actually have problematic consequences. Specifically, we theorize that the hero label may affect beliefs about the internal motivations of these group members that make them more vulnerable to exploitation. These ideas are tested and supported across nine preregistered studies using complementary materials and experimental paradigms. In these studies, we find that: (a) heroization strengthens expectations that teachers, nurses, and military personnel would willingly volunteer for their own exploitation; (b) the hero label and its consequences follow workers even after they transition to a new career (e.g., participants expected a military veteran-relative to a matched nonveteran-to be more willing to volunteer for his own exploitation at his subsequent civilian job, because the veteran was perceived to be more heroic than the matched nonveteran); and (c) occupational heroization-likely because of its impact on beliefs regarding what heroized workers would freely choose to do-reduces opposition to exploitative policies. In short, our studies show that heroization ultimately promotes worse treatment of the very groups that it is meant to venerate. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":16691,"journal":{"name":"Journal of personality and social psychology","volume":" ","pages":"5-25"},"PeriodicalIF":6.4,"publicationDate":"2024-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"50161885","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
I like it because it hurts you: On the association of everyday sadism, sadistic pleasure, and victim blaming. 我喜欢它,因为它会伤害你:关于日常虐待狂、虐待狂快感和受害者自责的关联。
IF 6.4 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, SOCIAL Pub Date : 2024-01-01 Epub Date: 2023-04-20 DOI: 10.1037/pspp0000464
Claudia Sassenrath, Johannes Keller, Dominik Stöckle, Rebekka Kesberg, Yngwie Asbjørn Nielsen, Stefan Pfattheicher

Past research on determinants of victim blaming mainly concentrated on individuals' just-world beliefs as motivational process underlying this harsh reaction to others' suffering. The present work provides novel insights regarding underlying affective processes by showing how individuals prone to derive pleasure from others' suffering-individuals high in everyday sadism-engage in victim blaming due to increased sadistic pleasure and reduced empathic concern they experience. Results of three cross-sectional studies and one ambulatory assessment study applying online experience sampling method (ESM; overall N = 2,653) document this association. Importantly, the relation emerged over and above the honesty-humility, emotionality, extraversion, agreeableness, conscientiousness, and openness personality model (Study 1a), and other so-called dark traits (Study 1b), across different cultural backgrounds (Study 1c), and also when sampling from a population of individuals frequently confronted with victim-perpetrator constellations: police officers (Study 1d). Studies 2 and 3 highlight a significant behavioral correlate of victim blaming. Everyday sadism is related to reduced willingness to engage in effortful cognitive activity as individuals high (vs. low) in everyday sadism recall less information regarding victim-perpetrator constellations of sexual assault. Results obtained in the ESM study (Study 4) indicate that the relation of everyday sadism, sadistic pleasure, and victim blaming holds in everyday life and is not significantly moderated by interpersonal closeness to the blamed victim or impactfulness of the incident. Overall, the present article extends our understanding of what determines innocent victims' derogation and highlights emotional mechanisms, societal relevance, and generalizability of the observed associations beyond the laboratory. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

过去关于受害者指责的决定因素的研究主要集中在个人的正义世界信念,认为这是个人对他人痛苦做出严厉反应的动机过程。本研究通过展示容易从他人的痛苦中获得快感的个体--日常虐待狂程度较高的个体--是如何因为虐待狂快感的增加和移情关怀的减少而参与到指责受害者的行为中去的,从而提供了有关潜在情感过程的新见解。三项横断面研究和一项采用在线经验取样法(ESM,总人数=2,653)进行的流动评估研究的结果证明了这种关联。重要的是,这种关系的出现超越了诚实-谦逊、情绪化、外向性、合意性、自觉性和开放性人格模型(研究 1a)以及其他所谓的阴暗特质(研究 1b),跨越了不同的文化背景(研究 1c),而且还从经常面对受害者-施暴者组合的人群(警察)中进行了取样(研究 1d)。研究 2 和研究 3 强调了受害者自责的一个重要行为相关因素。日常虐待狂与参与努力认知活动的意愿降低有关,因为日常虐待狂程度高的人(与程度低的人)回忆起的有关性侵犯受害者-施害者组合的信息较少。ESM研究(研究4)的结果表明,日常虐待狂、虐待狂快感和受害者责备之间的关系在日常生活中是成立的,并且不会因为与被责备的受害者的人际关系亲密程度或事件的影响程度而发生显著的调节作用。总之,本文拓展了我们对无辜受害者受到贬损的决定性因素的理解,并强调了所观察到的关联的情感机制、社会相关性和超越实验室的普适性。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved)。
{"title":"I like it because it hurts you: On the association of everyday sadism, sadistic pleasure, and victim blaming.","authors":"Claudia Sassenrath, Johannes Keller, Dominik Stöckle, Rebekka Kesberg, Yngwie Asbjørn Nielsen, Stefan Pfattheicher","doi":"10.1037/pspp0000464","DOIUrl":"10.1037/pspp0000464","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Past research on determinants of victim blaming mainly concentrated on individuals' just-world beliefs as motivational process underlying this harsh reaction to others' suffering. The present work provides novel insights regarding underlying <i>affective</i> processes by showing how individuals prone to derive pleasure from others' suffering-individuals high in everyday sadism-engage in victim blaming due to increased sadistic pleasure and reduced empathic concern they experience. Results of three cross-sectional studies and one ambulatory assessment study applying online experience sampling method (ESM; overall <i>N</i> = 2,653) document this association. Importantly, the relation emerged over and above the honesty-humility, emotionality, extraversion, agreeableness, conscientiousness, and openness personality model (Study 1a), and other so-called dark traits (Study 1b), across different cultural backgrounds (Study 1c), and also when sampling from a population of individuals frequently confronted with victim-perpetrator constellations: police officers (Study 1d). Studies 2 and 3 highlight a significant behavioral correlate of victim blaming. Everyday sadism is related to reduced willingness to engage in effortful cognitive activity as individuals high (vs. low) in everyday sadism recall less information regarding victim-perpetrator constellations of sexual assault. Results obtained in the ESM study (Study 4) indicate that the relation of everyday sadism, sadistic pleasure, and victim blaming holds in everyday life and is not significantly moderated by interpersonal closeness to the blamed victim or impactfulness of the incident. Overall, the present article extends our understanding of what determines innocent victims' derogation and highlights emotional mechanisms, societal relevance, and generalizability of the observed associations beyond the laboratory. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":16691,"journal":{"name":"Journal of personality and social psychology","volume":" ","pages":"105-127"},"PeriodicalIF":6.4,"publicationDate":"2024-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9422732","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
"Thanks, but no thanks": Gratitude expression paradoxically signals distance. “谢谢,但不谢谢”:感恩的表达矛盾地表示距离。
IF 6.4 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, SOCIAL Pub Date : 2024-01-01 Epub Date: 2023-10-05 DOI: 10.1037/pspi0000435
Jiaqi Yu, Shereen J Chaudhry

Many studies have found that feelings and expressions of gratitude bring profound benefits to people and relationships. We complicate this view of gratitude. We examine two variables known to impact people's expectations for relationships: culture (collectivist vs. individualist) and relational distance (close vs. distant), and we find evidence that expressing gratitude conveys that relationship expectations have been exceeded, such that people view it as less desirable to give and receive gratitude for actions that are expected duties of a relationship. In both observational data and real behavior in an experiment, we found that people in a collectivist culture (China) are less likely than those in an individualist culture (America) to express gratitude to close others (Studies 1 and 2). Using hypothetical vignettes, we confirmed this pattern and further found there was no cultural difference for distant others (Study 3). These differences in expressing gratitude reflect differences in underlying feelings of gratitude, as well as differences in expectations of how the target would react to being thanked (Study 4). This cultural difference can be explained by cultural differences in the extent of duties placed on close others (Studies 5 and 6): People in China expect more of their close others. Perhaps as a result, people in China show a weaker preference than Americans for direct expressions of gratitude toward close others, but no difference for distant others (Study 7). Overall, our findings suggest that expressing gratitude may not always be good for close relationships. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

许多研究发现,感恩的情感和表达会给人和人际关系带来深远的好处。我们使这种感恩的观点复杂化了。我们研究了两个已知会影响人们对关系期望的变量:文化(集体主义者与个人主义者)和关系距离(亲密与遥远),我们发现有证据表明,表达感激之情表明关系期望已经被超越,以至于人们认为,对一段关系中预期职责的行为给予和接受感激是不太可取的。在观察数据和实验中的真实行为中,我们发现集体主义文化(中国)中的人比个人主义文化(美国)中的人们更不可能表达对亲密他人的感激之情(研究1和2)。使用假设的小插曲,我们证实了这种模式,并进一步发现远方的人没有文化差异(研究3)。在表达感激之情方面的这些差异反映了潜在感激之情的差异,以及对目标对被感激的反应的期望的差异(研究4)。这种文化差异可以用对亲密他人的责任程度的文化差异来解释(研究5和6):中国人对亲密他人期望更高。也许因此,与美国人相比,中国人对亲密的人直接表达感激之情的偏好较弱,但对疏远的人则没有差异(研究7)。总的来说,我们的研究结果表明,表达感激之情可能并不总是有利于亲密关系。(PsycInfo数据库记录(c)2023 APA,保留所有权利)。
{"title":"\"Thanks, but no thanks\": Gratitude expression paradoxically signals distance.","authors":"Jiaqi Yu, Shereen J Chaudhry","doi":"10.1037/pspi0000435","DOIUrl":"10.1037/pspi0000435","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Many studies have found that feelings and expressions of gratitude bring profound benefits to people and relationships. We complicate this view of gratitude. We examine two variables known to impact people's expectations for relationships: culture (collectivist vs. individualist) and relational distance (close vs. distant), and we find evidence that expressing gratitude conveys that relationship expectations have been exceeded, such that people view it as less desirable to give and receive gratitude for actions that are expected duties of a relationship. In both observational data and real behavior in an experiment, we found that people in a collectivist culture (China) are less likely than those in an individualist culture (America) to express gratitude to close others (Studies 1 and 2). Using hypothetical vignettes, we confirmed this pattern and further found there was no cultural difference for distant others (Study 3). These differences in expressing gratitude reflect differences in underlying feelings of gratitude, as well as differences in expectations of how the target would react to being thanked (Study 4). This cultural difference can be explained by cultural differences in the extent of duties placed on close others (Studies 5 and 6): People in China expect more of their close others. Perhaps as a result, people in China show a weaker preference than Americans for direct expressions of gratitude toward close others, but no difference for distant others (Study 7). Overall, our findings suggest that expressing gratitude may not always be good for close relationships. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":16691,"journal":{"name":"Journal of personality and social psychology","volume":" ","pages":"58-78"},"PeriodicalIF":6.4,"publicationDate":"2024-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"41124327","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Journal of personality and social psychology
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1