首页 > 最新文献

Nature Human Behaviour最新文献

英文 中文
Flexible social inference facilitates targeted social learning when rewards are not observable 当无法观察到奖励时,灵活的社会推理有助于有针对性的社会学习。
IF 29.9 1区 心理学 Q1 MULTIDISCIPLINARY SCIENCES Pub Date : 2023-08-17 DOI: 10.1038/s41562-023-01682-x
Robert D. Hawkins, Andrew M. Berdahl, Alex ‘Sandy’ Pentland, Joshua B. Tenenbaum, Noah D. Goodman, P. M. Krafft
Groups coordinate more effectively when individuals are able to learn from others’ successes. But acquiring such knowledge is not always easy, especially in real-world environments where success is hidden from public view. We suggest that social inference capacities may help bridge this gap, allowing individuals to update their beliefs about others’ underlying knowledge and success from observable trajectories of behaviour. We compared our social inference model against simpler heuristics in three studies of human behaviour in a collective-sensing task. Experiment 1 demonstrated that average performance improved as a function of group size at a rate greater than predicted by heuristic models. Experiment 2 introduced artificial agents to evaluate how individuals selectively rely on social information. Experiment 3 generalized these findings to a more complex reward landscape. Taken together, our findings provide insight into the relationship between individual social cognition and the flexibility of collective behaviour. Groups coordinate more effectively when individuals are able to learn from others’ successes. Hawkins et al. use a large-scale collective sensing paradigm to test how individual social inference abilities shape the emergent behaviour of human groups.
当个人能够从他人的成功中学习时,团队会更有效地协调。但获得这些知识并不总是那么容易,尤其是在现实世界中,成功是隐藏在公众视野之外的。我们认为,社会推理能力可能有助于弥合这一差距,使个人能够根据可观察的行为轨迹更新他们对他人潜在知识和成功的信念。我们在集体感知任务中对人类行为的三项研究中,将我们的社会推理模型与更简单的启发式方法进行了比较。实验1表明,作为组大小的函数,平均性能以比启发式模型预测的更高的速率提高。实验2引入了人工智能体来评估个体如何选择性地依赖社会信息。实验3将这些发现推广到更复杂的奖励景观中。总之,我们的研究结果深入了解了个体社会认知与集体行为灵活性之间的关系。
{"title":"Flexible social inference facilitates targeted social learning when rewards are not observable","authors":"Robert D. Hawkins, Andrew M. Berdahl, Alex ‘Sandy’ Pentland, Joshua B. Tenenbaum, Noah D. Goodman, P. M. Krafft","doi":"10.1038/s41562-023-01682-x","DOIUrl":"10.1038/s41562-023-01682-x","url":null,"abstract":"Groups coordinate more effectively when individuals are able to learn from others’ successes. But acquiring such knowledge is not always easy, especially in real-world environments where success is hidden from public view. We suggest that social inference capacities may help bridge this gap, allowing individuals to update their beliefs about others’ underlying knowledge and success from observable trajectories of behaviour. We compared our social inference model against simpler heuristics in three studies of human behaviour in a collective-sensing task. Experiment 1 demonstrated that average performance improved as a function of group size at a rate greater than predicted by heuristic models. Experiment 2 introduced artificial agents to evaluate how individuals selectively rely on social information. Experiment 3 generalized these findings to a more complex reward landscape. Taken together, our findings provide insight into the relationship between individual social cognition and the flexibility of collective behaviour. Groups coordinate more effectively when individuals are able to learn from others’ successes. Hawkins et al. use a large-scale collective sensing paradigm to test how individual social inference abilities shape the emergent behaviour of human groups.","PeriodicalId":19074,"journal":{"name":"Nature Human Behaviour","volume":"7 10","pages":"1767-1776"},"PeriodicalIF":29.9,"publicationDate":"2023-08-17","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10374681","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Collective cognition and behaviour 集体认知和行为。
IF 29.9 1区 心理学 Q1 MULTIDISCIPLINARY SCIENCES Pub Date : 2023-08-17 DOI: 10.1038/s41562-023-01683-w
Wataru Toyokawa
‘Metacognition’ refers to thinking about thinking, and its function in collective human behaviour remains largely unknown. Using a multiplayer online game and agent-based modelling, Hawkins et al. found distinctive patterns of collective intelligence that only emerge when using metacognitive social inference skills.
元认知 "指的是关于思维的思考,它在人类集体行为中的功能在很大程度上仍不为人所知。Hawkins 等人利用多人在线游戏和基于代理的建模,发现了只有在使用元认知社会推理技能时才会出现的独特的集体智慧模式。
{"title":"Collective cognition and behaviour","authors":"Wataru Toyokawa","doi":"10.1038/s41562-023-01683-w","DOIUrl":"10.1038/s41562-023-01683-w","url":null,"abstract":"‘Metacognition’ refers to thinking about thinking, and its function in collective human behaviour remains largely unknown. Using a multiplayer online game and agent-based modelling, Hawkins et al. found distinctive patterns of collective intelligence that only emerge when using metacognitive social inference skills.","PeriodicalId":19074,"journal":{"name":"Nature Human Behaviour","volume":"7 10","pages":"1612-1613"},"PeriodicalIF":29.9,"publicationDate":"2023-08-17","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10396164","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Developmental changes in exploration resemble stochastic optimization 勘探的发展变化类似于随机优化。
IF 29.9 1区 心理学 Q1 MULTIDISCIPLINARY SCIENCES Pub Date : 2023-08-17 DOI: 10.1038/s41562-023-01662-1
Anna P. Giron, Simon Ciranka, Eric Schulz, Wouter van den Bos, Azzurra Ruggeri, Björn Meder, Charley M. Wu
Human development is often described as a ‘cooling off’ process, analogous to stochastic optimization algorithms that implement a gradual reduction in randomness over time. Yet there is ambiguity in how to interpret this analogy, due to a lack of concrete empirical comparisons. Using data from n = 281 participants ages 5 to 55, we show that cooling off does not only apply to the single dimension of randomness. Rather, human development resembles an optimization process of multiple learning parameters, for example, reward generalization, uncertainty-directed exploration and random temperature. Rapid changes in parameters occur during childhood, but these changes plateau and converge to efficient values in adulthood. We show that while the developmental trajectory of human parameters is strikingly similar to several stochastic optimization algorithms, there are important differences in convergence. None of the optimization algorithms tested were able to discover reliably better regions of the strategy space than adult participants on this task. Giron et al. provide empirical evidence that human development has much in common with the algorithm of ‘stochastic optimization’ widely used in machine learning, resolving ambiguities around commonly used analogies in developmental psychology.
人类的发展通常被描述为一个“冷却”的过程,类似于随机优化算法,随着时间的推移逐步减少随机性。然而,由于缺乏具体的经验比较,如何解释这一类比存在歧义。使用n = 281名年龄在5岁至55岁之间的参与者的数据,我们表明冷却不仅适用于随机性的单一维度。相反,人类的发展类似于多个学习参数的优化过程,例如,奖励泛化、不确定性导向的探索和随机温度。这些参数在儿童期发生快速变化,但这些变化趋于平稳,并在成年期收敛为有效值。我们表明,虽然人类参数的发展轨迹与几种随机优化算法惊人地相似,但在收敛性方面存在重要差异。在这项任务中,测试的优化算法都不能可靠地发现比成人参与者更好的策略空间区域。
{"title":"Developmental changes in exploration resemble stochastic optimization","authors":"Anna P. Giron, Simon Ciranka, Eric Schulz, Wouter van den Bos, Azzurra Ruggeri, Björn Meder, Charley M. Wu","doi":"10.1038/s41562-023-01662-1","DOIUrl":"10.1038/s41562-023-01662-1","url":null,"abstract":"Human development is often described as a ‘cooling off’ process, analogous to stochastic optimization algorithms that implement a gradual reduction in randomness over time. Yet there is ambiguity in how to interpret this analogy, due to a lack of concrete empirical comparisons. Using data from n = 281 participants ages 5 to 55, we show that cooling off does not only apply to the single dimension of randomness. Rather, human development resembles an optimization process of multiple learning parameters, for example, reward generalization, uncertainty-directed exploration and random temperature. Rapid changes in parameters occur during childhood, but these changes plateau and converge to efficient values in adulthood. We show that while the developmental trajectory of human parameters is strikingly similar to several stochastic optimization algorithms, there are important differences in convergence. None of the optimization algorithms tested were able to discover reliably better regions of the strategy space than adult participants on this task. Giron et al. provide empirical evidence that human development has much in common with the algorithm of ‘stochastic optimization’ widely used in machine learning, resolving ambiguities around commonly used analogies in developmental psychology.","PeriodicalId":19074,"journal":{"name":"Nature Human Behaviour","volume":"7 11","pages":"1955-1967"},"PeriodicalIF":29.9,"publicationDate":"2023-08-17","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC10663152/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10374679","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Testing cognitive theories with multivariate pattern analysis of neuroimaging data 用神经影像学数据的多变量模式分析检验认知理论。
IF 29.9 1区 心理学 Q1 MULTIDISCIPLINARY SCIENCES Pub Date : 2023-08-17 DOI: 10.1038/s41562-023-01680-z
Marius V. Peelen, Paul E. Downing
Multivariate pattern analysis (MVPA) has emerged as a powerful method for the analysis of functional magnetic resonance imaging, electroencephalography and magnetoencephalography data. The new approaches to experimental design and hypothesis testing afforded by MVPA have made it possible to address theories that describe cognition at the functional level. Here we review a selection of studies that have used MVPA to test cognitive theories from a range of domains, including perception, attention, memory, navigation, emotion, social cognition and motor control. This broad view reveals properties of MVPA that make it suitable for understanding the ‘how’ of human cognition, such as the ability to test predictions expressed at the item or event level. It also reveals limitations and points to future directions. Peelen and Downing review the use of multivariate pattern analysis in cognitive neuroscience to study cognition at the functional level.
多变量模式分析(MVPA)已成为分析功能性磁共振成像、脑电图和脑磁图数据的一种强大方法。MVPA提供的实验设计和假设检验的新方法使在功能水平上描述认知的理论成为可能。在这里,我们回顾了一系列使用MVPA测试认知理论的研究,这些理论来自一系列领域,包括感知、注意力、记忆、导航、情绪、社会认知和运动控制。这一宽泛的观点揭示了MVPA的特性,使其适合于理解人类认知的“方式”,例如测试在项目或事件层面表达的预测的能力。它还揭示了局限性,并指出了未来的方向。
{"title":"Testing cognitive theories with multivariate pattern analysis of neuroimaging data","authors":"Marius V. Peelen, Paul E. Downing","doi":"10.1038/s41562-023-01680-z","DOIUrl":"10.1038/s41562-023-01680-z","url":null,"abstract":"Multivariate pattern analysis (MVPA) has emerged as a powerful method for the analysis of functional magnetic resonance imaging, electroencephalography and magnetoencephalography data. The new approaches to experimental design and hypothesis testing afforded by MVPA have made it possible to address theories that describe cognition at the functional level. Here we review a selection of studies that have used MVPA to test cognitive theories from a range of domains, including perception, attention, memory, navigation, emotion, social cognition and motor control. This broad view reveals properties of MVPA that make it suitable for understanding the ‘how’ of human cognition, such as the ability to test predictions expressed at the item or event level. It also reveals limitations and points to future directions. Peelen and Downing review the use of multivariate pattern analysis in cognitive neuroscience to study cognition at the functional level.","PeriodicalId":19074,"journal":{"name":"Nature Human Behaviour","volume":"7 9","pages":"1430-1441"},"PeriodicalIF":29.9,"publicationDate":"2023-08-17","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10374682","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
No evidence that Chinese playtime mandates reduced heavy gaming in one segment of the video games industry 没有证据表明中国的游戏时间规定减少了电子游戏行业某一领域的重度游戏。
IF 29.9 1区 心理学 Q1 MULTIDISCIPLINARY SCIENCES Pub Date : 2023-08-10 DOI: 10.1038/s41562-023-01669-8
David Zendle, Catherine Flick, Elena Gordon-Petrovskaya, Nick Ballou, Leon Y. Xiao, Anders Drachen
Governments around the world are considering regulatory measures to reduce young people’s time spent on digital devices, particularly video games. This raises the question of whether proposed regulatory measures would be effective. Since the early 2000s, the Chinese government has been enacting regulations to directly restrict young people’s playtime. In November 2019, it limited players aged under 18 to 1.5 hours of daily playtime and 3 hours on public holidays. Using telemetry data on over seven billion hours of playtime provided by a stakeholder from the video games industry, we found no credible evidence for overall reduction in the prevalence of heavy playtime following the implementation of regulations: individual accounts became 1.14 times more likely to play heavily in any given week (95% confidence interval 1.139–1.141). This falls below our preregistered smallest effect size of interest (2.0) and thus is not interpreted as a practically meaningful increase. Results remain robust across a variety of sensitivity analyses, including an analysis of more recent (2021) adjustments to playtime regulation. This casts doubt on the effectiveness of such state-controlled playtime mandates. The authors show that video game playtime restriction policies in China had no discernible influence on time spent gaming.
世界各国政府正在考虑采取监管措施,减少年轻人花在数字设备上的时间,尤其是电子游戏上的时间。这就提出了一个问题,即拟议的监管措施是否有效。自21世纪初以来,中国政府一直在制定法规,直接限制年轻人的游戏时间。2019年11月,它将18岁以下的玩家限制在1.5岁以内 每日游戏时间和3小时 公共假日的营业时间。使用视频游戏行业的利益相关者提供的超过70亿小时游戏时间的遥测数据,我们没有发现任何可信的证据表明,在实施规定后,大量游戏时间的普遍性总体上减少了:个人账户在任何一周内大量游戏的可能性增加了1.14倍(95%置信区间1.139-1.141)。这低于我们预先登记的最小兴趣效应大小(2.0),因此不被解释为实际有意义的增加。各种敏感性分析的结果仍然稳健,包括对最近(2021年)对游戏时间调节的调整的分析。这让人们对这种国家控制的游戏时间规定的有效性产生了怀疑。
{"title":"No evidence that Chinese playtime mandates reduced heavy gaming in one segment of the video games industry","authors":"David Zendle, Catherine Flick, Elena Gordon-Petrovskaya, Nick Ballou, Leon Y. Xiao, Anders Drachen","doi":"10.1038/s41562-023-01669-8","DOIUrl":"10.1038/s41562-023-01669-8","url":null,"abstract":"Governments around the world are considering regulatory measures to reduce young people’s time spent on digital devices, particularly video games. This raises the question of whether proposed regulatory measures would be effective. Since the early 2000s, the Chinese government has been enacting regulations to directly restrict young people’s playtime. In November 2019, it limited players aged under 18 to 1.5 hours of daily playtime and 3 hours on public holidays. Using telemetry data on over seven billion hours of playtime provided by a stakeholder from the video games industry, we found no credible evidence for overall reduction in the prevalence of heavy playtime following the implementation of regulations: individual accounts became 1.14 times more likely to play heavily in any given week (95% confidence interval 1.139–1.141). This falls below our preregistered smallest effect size of interest (2.0) and thus is not interpreted as a practically meaningful increase. Results remain robust across a variety of sensitivity analyses, including an analysis of more recent (2021) adjustments to playtime regulation. This casts doubt on the effectiveness of such state-controlled playtime mandates. The authors show that video game playtime restriction policies in China had no discernible influence on time spent gaming.","PeriodicalId":19074,"journal":{"name":"Nature Human Behaviour","volume":"7 10","pages":"1753-1766"},"PeriodicalIF":29.9,"publicationDate":"2023-08-10","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC10593605/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9965201","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
A systematic review and meta-analysis of the relationship between economic inequality and prosocial behaviour 经济不平等与亲社会行为关系的系统回顾与元分析。
IF 29.9 1区 心理学 Q1 MULTIDISCIPLINARY SCIENCES Pub Date : 2023-08-10 DOI: 10.1038/s41562-023-01681-y
Yongzheng Yang, Sara Konrath
How does economic inequality relate to prosocial behaviour? Existing theories and empirical studies from multiple disciplines have produced mixed results. Here we conduct a systematic review and meta-analysis to systematically synthesize empirical studies. Results from 192 effect sizes and over 2.5 million observations in 100 studies show that the relationship varies from being negative to positive depending upon the study (95% prediction interval −0.450 to 0.343). However, on average, there is a small, negative relationship between economic inequality and prosocial behaviour (r = −0.064, P = 0.004, 95% confidence interval −0.106 to −0.021). There is generally no evidence that results depend upon characteristics of the studies, participants, the way prosocial behaviour and inequality were assessed, and the publication discipline. Given the prevalence of economic inequality and the importance of prosocial behaviour, this systematic review and meta-analysis provides a timely study on the relationship between economic inequality and prosocial behaviour. This meta-analysis of the relationship between economic inequality and prosocial behaviour finds that the relationship varies from being negative to positive, but, on average, higher economic inequality is associated with lower prosocial behaviour.
经济不平等与亲社会行为有何关系?来自多个学科的现有理论和实证研究产生了不同的结果。本文通过系统综述和元分析,对实证研究进行系统综合。来自192个效应大小和100个研究中超过250万个观察结果的结果表明,根据研究的不同,这种关系从负向正变化(95%预测区间为-0.450至0.343)。然而,平均而言,经济不平等与亲社会行为之间存在较小的负相关(r = -0.064, P = 0.004, 95%置信区间为-0.106至-0.021)。一般来说,没有证据表明结果取决于研究的特征、参与者、评估亲社会行为和不平等的方式以及发表的学科。鉴于经济不平等的普遍存在和亲社会行为的重要性,本系统综述和荟萃分析为经济不平等与亲社会行为之间的关系提供了及时的研究。
{"title":"A systematic review and meta-analysis of the relationship between economic inequality and prosocial behaviour","authors":"Yongzheng Yang, Sara Konrath","doi":"10.1038/s41562-023-01681-y","DOIUrl":"10.1038/s41562-023-01681-y","url":null,"abstract":"How does economic inequality relate to prosocial behaviour? Existing theories and empirical studies from multiple disciplines have produced mixed results. Here we conduct a systematic review and meta-analysis to systematically synthesize empirical studies. Results from 192 effect sizes and over 2.5 million observations in 100 studies show that the relationship varies from being negative to positive depending upon the study (95% prediction interval −0.450 to 0.343). However, on average, there is a small, negative relationship between economic inequality and prosocial behaviour (r = −0.064, P = 0.004, 95% confidence interval −0.106 to −0.021). There is generally no evidence that results depend upon characteristics of the studies, participants, the way prosocial behaviour and inequality were assessed, and the publication discipline. Given the prevalence of economic inequality and the importance of prosocial behaviour, this systematic review and meta-analysis provides a timely study on the relationship between economic inequality and prosocial behaviour. This meta-analysis of the relationship between economic inequality and prosocial behaviour finds that the relationship varies from being negative to positive, but, on average, higher economic inequality is associated with lower prosocial behaviour.","PeriodicalId":19074,"journal":{"name":"Nature Human Behaviour","volume":"7 11","pages":"1899-1916"},"PeriodicalIF":29.9,"publicationDate":"2023-08-10","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9965207","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Big STEM collaborations should include humanities and social science 大型STEM合作应包括人文科学和社会科学。
IF 29.9 1区 心理学 Q1 MULTIDISCIPLINARY SCIENCES Pub Date : 2023-08-10 DOI: 10.1038/s41562-023-01674-x
Alexandru Marcoci, Ann C. Thresher, Niels C. M. Martens, Peter Galison, Sheperd S. Doeleman, Michael D. Johnson
{"title":"Big STEM collaborations should include humanities and social science","authors":"Alexandru Marcoci, Ann C. Thresher, Niels C. M. Martens, Peter Galison, Sheperd S. Doeleman, Michael D. Johnson","doi":"10.1038/s41562-023-01674-x","DOIUrl":"10.1038/s41562-023-01674-x","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":19074,"journal":{"name":"Nature Human Behaviour","volume":"7 8","pages":"1229-1230"},"PeriodicalIF":29.9,"publicationDate":"2023-08-10","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10432281","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
How to think about whether misinformation interventions work 如何思考错误信息干预是否有效。
IF 29.9 1区 心理学 Q1 MULTIDISCIPLINARY SCIENCES Pub Date : 2023-08-10 DOI: 10.1038/s41562-023-01667-w
Brian Guay, Adam J. Berinsky, Gordon Pennycook, David Rand
Progress in the burgeoning field of misinformation research requires some degree of consensus about what constitutes an effective intervention to combat misinformation. We differentiate between research designs that are used to evaluate interventions and recommend one that measures how well people discern between true and false content.
要想在蓬勃发展的误导信息研究领域取得进展,就必须在一定程度上就如何有效干预误导信息达成共识。我们对用于评估干预措施的研究设计进行了区分,并推荐了一种衡量人们辨别真假内容能力的设计。
{"title":"How to think about whether misinformation interventions work","authors":"Brian Guay, Adam J. Berinsky, Gordon Pennycook, David Rand","doi":"10.1038/s41562-023-01667-w","DOIUrl":"10.1038/s41562-023-01667-w","url":null,"abstract":"Progress in the burgeoning field of misinformation research requires some degree of consensus about what constitutes an effective intervention to combat misinformation. We differentiate between research designs that are used to evaluate interventions and recommend one that measures how well people discern between true and false content.","PeriodicalId":19074,"journal":{"name":"Nature Human Behaviour","volume":"7 8","pages":"1231-1233"},"PeriodicalIF":29.9,"publicationDate":"2023-08-10","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10056695","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 11
Empowering women facing gender-based violence amid COVID-19 through media campaigns 通过媒体宣传活动,增强新冠肺炎期间面临性别暴力的妇女的能力。
IF 29.9 1区 心理学 Q1 MULTIDISCIPLINARY SCIENCES Pub Date : 2023-08-07 DOI: 10.1038/s41562-023-01665-y
Fotini Christia, Horacio Larreguy, Elizabeth Parker-Magyar, Manuel Quintero
COVID-19 heightened women’s exposure to gender-based and intimate partner violence, especially in low-income and middle-income countries. We tested whether edutainment interventions shown to successfully combat gender-based and intimate partner violence when delivered in person can be effectively delivered using social (WhatsApp and Facebook) and traditional (TV) media. To do so, we randomized the mode of implementation of an intervention conducted by an Egyptian women’s rights organization seeking to support women amid COVID-19 social distancing. We found WhatsApp to be more effective in delivering the intervention than Facebook but no credible evidence of differences across outcomes between social media and TV dissemination. Our findings show little credible evidence that these campaigns affected women’s attitudes towards gender or marital equality or on the justifiability of violence. However, the campaign did increase women’s knowledge, hypothetical use and reported use of available resources. Christia et al. evaluate the delivery of content to empower women exposed to violence amid COVID-19. The recipients exhibited no credible evidence of a shift in attitudes but increased their knowledge and hypothetical and reported use of resources.
新冠肺炎增加了妇女遭受基于性别和亲密伴侣暴力的风险,特别是在低收入和中等收入国家。我们测试了社交媒体(WhatsApp和Facebook)和传统媒体(电视)是否可以有效地提供教育娱乐干预措施,这些干预措施在亲自实施时被证明可以成功打击基于性别和亲密伴侣的暴力。为此,我们随机选择了埃及一家妇女权利组织进行的干预措施的实施模式,该组织旨在支持新冠肺炎期间保持社交距离的妇女。我们发现WhatsApp在提供干预方面比Facebook更有效,但没有可信的证据表明社交媒体和电视传播之间的结果存在差异。我们的调查结果几乎没有可信的证据表明,这些运动影响了妇女对性别或婚姻平等或暴力正当性的态度。然而,这场运动确实增加了妇女对现有资源的了解、假设使用和报告使用情况。
{"title":"Empowering women facing gender-based violence amid COVID-19 through media campaigns","authors":"Fotini Christia, Horacio Larreguy, Elizabeth Parker-Magyar, Manuel Quintero","doi":"10.1038/s41562-023-01665-y","DOIUrl":"10.1038/s41562-023-01665-y","url":null,"abstract":"COVID-19 heightened women’s exposure to gender-based and intimate partner violence, especially in low-income and middle-income countries. We tested whether edutainment interventions shown to successfully combat gender-based and intimate partner violence when delivered in person can be effectively delivered using social (WhatsApp and Facebook) and traditional (TV) media. To do so, we randomized the mode of implementation of an intervention conducted by an Egyptian women’s rights organization seeking to support women amid COVID-19 social distancing. We found WhatsApp to be more effective in delivering the intervention than Facebook but no credible evidence of differences across outcomes between social media and TV dissemination. Our findings show little credible evidence that these campaigns affected women’s attitudes towards gender or marital equality or on the justifiability of violence. However, the campaign did increase women’s knowledge, hypothetical use and reported use of available resources. Christia et al. evaluate the delivery of content to empower women exposed to violence amid COVID-19. The recipients exhibited no credible evidence of a shift in attitudes but increased their knowledge and hypothetical and reported use of resources.","PeriodicalId":19074,"journal":{"name":"Nature Human Behaviour","volume":"7 10","pages":"1740-1752"},"PeriodicalIF":29.9,"publicationDate":"2023-08-07","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9954391","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Large-scale AI language systems display an emergent ability to reason by analogy 大规模的人工智能语言系统显示出一种通过类比推理的能力。
IF 29.9 1区 心理学 Q1 MULTIDISCIPLINARY SCIENCES Pub Date : 2023-08-04 DOI: 10.1038/s41562-023-01671-0
Analogical reasoning is a hallmark of human intelligence, as it enables us to flexibly solve new problems without extensive practice. By using a wide range of tests, we demonstrate that GPT-3, a large-scale artificial intelligence language model, is capable of solving difficult analogy problems at a level comparable to human performance.
类比推理是人类智能的一个标志,因为它使我们无需大量练习就能灵活地解决新问题。通过大量测试,我们证明大型人工智能语言模型 GPT-3 能够以与人类相当的水平解决困难的类比问题。
{"title":"Large-scale AI language systems display an emergent ability to reason by analogy","authors":"","doi":"10.1038/s41562-023-01671-0","DOIUrl":"10.1038/s41562-023-01671-0","url":null,"abstract":"Analogical reasoning is a hallmark of human intelligence, as it enables us to flexibly solve new problems without extensive practice. By using a wide range of tests, we demonstrate that GPT-3, a large-scale artificial intelligence language model, is capable of solving difficult analogy problems at a level comparable to human performance.","PeriodicalId":19074,"journal":{"name":"Nature Human Behaviour","volume":"7 9","pages":"1426-1427"},"PeriodicalIF":29.9,"publicationDate":"2023-08-04","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9930856","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Nature Human Behaviour
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1