首页 > 最新文献

Nature Human Behaviour最新文献

英文 中文
No phenotypic or genotypic evidence for a link between sleep duration and brain atrophy 没有表型或基因型证据表明睡眠时间与脑萎缩之间存在联系。
IF 29.9 1区 心理学 Q1 MULTIDISCIPLINARY SCIENCES Pub Date : 2023-10-05 DOI: 10.1038/s41562-023-01707-5
Anders M. Fjell, Øystein Sørensen, Yunpeng Wang, Inge K. Amlien, William F. C. Baaré, David Bartrés-Faz, Lars Bertram, Carl-Johan Boraxbekk, Andreas M. Brandmaier, Ilja Demuth, Christian A. Drevon, Klaus P. Ebmeier, Paolo Ghisletta, Rogier Kievit, Simone Kühn, Kathrine Skak Madsen, Athanasia M. Mowinckel, Lars Nyberg, Claire E. Sexton, Cristina Solé-Padullés, Didac Vidal-Piñeiro, Gerd Wagner, Leiv Otto Watne, Kristine B. Walhovd
Short sleep is held to cause poorer brain health, but is short sleep associated with higher rates of brain structural decline? Analysing 8,153 longitudinal MRIs from 3,893 healthy adults, we found no evidence for an association between sleep duration and brain atrophy. In contrast, cross-sectional analyses (51,295 observations) showed inverse U-shaped relationships, where a duration of 6.5 (95% confidence interval, (5.7, 7.3)) hours was associated with the thickest cortex and largest volumes relative to intracranial volume. This fits converging evidence from research on mortality, health and cognition that points to roughly seven hours being associated with good health. Genome-wide association analyses suggested that genes associated with longer sleep for below-average sleepers were linked to shorter sleep for above-average sleepers. Mendelian randomization did not yield evidence for causal impacts of sleep on brain structure. The combined results challenge the notion that habitual short sleep causes brain atrophy, suggesting that normal brains promote adequate sleep duration—which is shorter than current recommendations. Fjell et al. analysed multiple large-scale longitudinal MRI datasets and found no evidence for an association of sleep duration and brain atrophy, suggesting that normal brains promote adequate sleep.
睡眠时间短被认为会导致大脑健康状况不佳,但睡眠时间短是否与大脑结构衰退率较高有关?通过分析3893名健康成年人的8153例纵向核磁共振成像,我们没有发现睡眠时间与脑萎缩之间存在关联的证据。相反,横断面分析(51295个观察结果)显示出倒U型关系,其中6.5小时(95%置信区间,(5.7,7.3))的持续时间与最厚的皮层和相对于颅内体积的最大体积有关。这与死亡率、健康和认知研究的一致证据相吻合,这些证据表明,大约7小时与健康有关。全基因组关联分析表明,与低于平均水平的睡眠者睡眠时间更长有关的基因与高于平均水平的睡者睡眠时间更短有关。孟德尔随机化并没有产生睡眠对大脑结构的因果影响的证据。综合结果挑战了习惯性短睡眠会导致大脑萎缩的观点,表明正常大脑会促进充足的睡眠时间,这比目前的建议更短。
{"title":"No phenotypic or genotypic evidence for a link between sleep duration and brain atrophy","authors":"Anders M. Fjell, Øystein Sørensen, Yunpeng Wang, Inge K. Amlien, William F. C. Baaré, David Bartrés-Faz, Lars Bertram, Carl-Johan Boraxbekk, Andreas M. Brandmaier, Ilja Demuth, Christian A. Drevon, Klaus P. Ebmeier, Paolo Ghisletta, Rogier Kievit, Simone Kühn, Kathrine Skak Madsen, Athanasia M. Mowinckel, Lars Nyberg, Claire E. Sexton, Cristina Solé-Padullés, Didac Vidal-Piñeiro, Gerd Wagner, Leiv Otto Watne, Kristine B. Walhovd","doi":"10.1038/s41562-023-01707-5","DOIUrl":"10.1038/s41562-023-01707-5","url":null,"abstract":"Short sleep is held to cause poorer brain health, but is short sleep associated with higher rates of brain structural decline? Analysing 8,153 longitudinal MRIs from 3,893 healthy adults, we found no evidence for an association between sleep duration and brain atrophy. In contrast, cross-sectional analyses (51,295 observations) showed inverse U-shaped relationships, where a duration of 6.5 (95% confidence interval, (5.7, 7.3)) hours was associated with the thickest cortex and largest volumes relative to intracranial volume. This fits converging evidence from research on mortality, health and cognition that points to roughly seven hours being associated with good health. Genome-wide association analyses suggested that genes associated with longer sleep for below-average sleepers were linked to shorter sleep for above-average sleepers. Mendelian randomization did not yield evidence for causal impacts of sleep on brain structure. The combined results challenge the notion that habitual short sleep causes brain atrophy, suggesting that normal brains promote adequate sleep duration—which is shorter than current recommendations. Fjell et al. analysed multiple large-scale longitudinal MRI datasets and found no evidence for an association of sleep duration and brain atrophy, suggesting that normal brains promote adequate sleep.","PeriodicalId":19074,"journal":{"name":"Nature Human Behaviour","volume":"7 11","pages":"2008-2022"},"PeriodicalIF":29.9,"publicationDate":"2023-10-05","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC10663160/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"41133057","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Distinct neuronal representation of small and large numbers in the human medial temporal lobe 人类内侧颞叶中大量和少量神经元的不同表现。
IF 29.9 1区 心理学 Q1 MULTIDISCIPLINARY SCIENCES Pub Date : 2023-10-02 DOI: 10.1038/s41562-023-01709-3
Esther F. Kutter, Gert Dehnen, Valeri Borger, Rainer Surges, Florian Mormann, Andreas Nieder
Whether small numerical quantities are represented by a special subitizing system that is distinct from a large-number estimation system has been debated for over a century. Here we show that two separate neural mechanisms underlie the representation of small and large numbers. We performed single neuron recordings in the medial temporal lobe of neurosurgical patients judging numbers. We found a boundary in neuronal coding around number 4 that correlates with the behavioural transition from subitizing to estimation. In the subitizing range, neurons showed superior tuning selectivity accompanied by suppression effects suggestive of surround inhibition as a selectivity-increasing mechanism. In contrast, tuning selectivity decreased with increasing numbers beyond 4, characterizing a ratio-dependent number estimation system. The two systems with the coding boundary separating them were also indicated using decoding and clustering analyses. The identified small-number subitizing system could be linked to attention and working memory that show comparable capacity limitations. Kutter et al. show that neurons in the human brain encode small numbers (up to 4) more precisely than large numbers, indicating a distinction between a small-number subitizing system and a large-number estimation system.
一个多世纪以来,人们一直在争论小数值是否由一个不同于大数值估计系统的特殊子系统来表示。在这里,我们展示了两种独立的神经机制,它们是小数字和大数字表示的基础。我们在神经外科患者的内侧颞叶进行了单神经元记录,以判断数字。我们在神经元编码中发现了一个围绕数字4的边界,它与从分类到估计的行为转变相关。在亚极化范围内,神经元表现出优异的调谐选择性,并伴有抑制效应,提示周围抑制是一种选择性增加机制。相反,调谐选择性随着超过4的数字的增加而降低,这是一个与比率相关的数字估计系统的特征。通过解码和聚类分析,还指出了编码边界将它们分开的两个系统。识别出的小数字分类系统可能与注意力和工作记忆有关,这些记忆显示出相当的容量限制。
{"title":"Distinct neuronal representation of small and large numbers in the human medial temporal lobe","authors":"Esther F. Kutter, Gert Dehnen, Valeri Borger, Rainer Surges, Florian Mormann, Andreas Nieder","doi":"10.1038/s41562-023-01709-3","DOIUrl":"10.1038/s41562-023-01709-3","url":null,"abstract":"Whether small numerical quantities are represented by a special subitizing system that is distinct from a large-number estimation system has been debated for over a century. Here we show that two separate neural mechanisms underlie the representation of small and large numbers. We performed single neuron recordings in the medial temporal lobe of neurosurgical patients judging numbers. We found a boundary in neuronal coding around number 4 that correlates with the behavioural transition from subitizing to estimation. In the subitizing range, neurons showed superior tuning selectivity accompanied by suppression effects suggestive of surround inhibition as a selectivity-increasing mechanism. In contrast, tuning selectivity decreased with increasing numbers beyond 4, characterizing a ratio-dependent number estimation system. The two systems with the coding boundary separating them were also indicated using decoding and clustering analyses. The identified small-number subitizing system could be linked to attention and working memory that show comparable capacity limitations. Kutter et al. show that neurons in the human brain encode small numbers (up to 4) more precisely than large numbers, indicating a distinction between a small-number subitizing system and a large-number estimation system.","PeriodicalId":19074,"journal":{"name":"Nature Human Behaviour","volume":"7 11","pages":"1998-2007"},"PeriodicalIF":29.9,"publicationDate":"2023-10-02","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"41137924","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The positive–negative–competence (PNC) model of psychological responses to representations of robots 机器人表征心理反应的正-负能力(PNC)模型。
IF 29.9 1区 心理学 Q1 MULTIDISCIPLINARY SCIENCES Pub Date : 2023-10-02 DOI: 10.1038/s41562-023-01705-7
Dario Krpan, Jonathan E. Booth, Andreea Damien
Robots are becoming an increasingly prominent part of society. Despite their growing importance, there exists no overarching model that synthesizes people’s psychological reactions to robots and identifies what factors shape them. To address this, we created a taxonomy of affective, cognitive and behavioural processes in response to a comprehensive stimulus sample depicting robots from 28 domains of human activity (for example, education, hospitality and industry) and examined its individual difference predictors. Across seven studies that tested 9,274 UK and US participants recruited via online panels, we used a data-driven approach combining qualitative and quantitative techniques to develop the positive–negative–competence model, which categorizes all psychological processes in response to the stimulus sample into three dimensions: positive, negative and competence-related. We also established the main individual difference predictors of these dimensions and examined the mechanisms for each predictor. Overall, this research provides an in-depth understanding of psychological functioning regarding representations of robots. The authors find that psychological responses towards representations of robots fall into three dimensions: positive, negative and competence. They also examine their individual difference predictors.
机器人正成为社会中日益突出的一部分。尽管机器人的重要性越来越大,但目前还没有一个综合人们对机器人的心理反应并确定影响机器人的因素的总体模型。为了解决这个问题,我们创建了一个情感、认知和行为过程的分类法,以应对一个全面的刺激样本,该样本描绘了28个机器人 人类活动领域(例如,教育、酒店和工业),并检验了其个体差异预测因素。在测试9274项的七项研究中 通过在线小组招募的英国和美国参与者,我们使用数据驱动的方法,结合定性和定量技术,开发了正负能力模型,该模型将对刺激样本的所有心理过程分为三个维度:积极、消极和能力相关。我们还建立了这些维度的主要个体差异预测因子,并检验了每个预测因子的机制。总的来说,这项研究提供了对机器人表征的心理功能的深入理解。
{"title":"The positive–negative–competence (PNC) model of psychological responses to representations of robots","authors":"Dario Krpan, Jonathan E. Booth, Andreea Damien","doi":"10.1038/s41562-023-01705-7","DOIUrl":"10.1038/s41562-023-01705-7","url":null,"abstract":"Robots are becoming an increasingly prominent part of society. Despite their growing importance, there exists no overarching model that synthesizes people’s psychological reactions to robots and identifies what factors shape them. To address this, we created a taxonomy of affective, cognitive and behavioural processes in response to a comprehensive stimulus sample depicting robots from 28 domains of human activity (for example, education, hospitality and industry) and examined its individual difference predictors. Across seven studies that tested 9,274 UK and US participants recruited via online panels, we used a data-driven approach combining qualitative and quantitative techniques to develop the positive–negative–competence model, which categorizes all psychological processes in response to the stimulus sample into three dimensions: positive, negative and competence-related. We also established the main individual difference predictors of these dimensions and examined the mechanisms for each predictor. Overall, this research provides an in-depth understanding of psychological functioning regarding representations of robots. The authors find that psychological responses towards representations of robots fall into three dimensions: positive, negative and competence. They also examine their individual difference predictors.","PeriodicalId":19074,"journal":{"name":"Nature Human Behaviour","volume":"7 11","pages":"1933-1954"},"PeriodicalIF":29.9,"publicationDate":"2023-10-02","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC10663151/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"41131395","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
State-led homophobia threatens African academic freedom 国家主导的恐同症威胁着非洲的学术自由。
IF 29.9 1区 心理学 Q1 MULTIDISCIPLINARY SCIENCES Pub Date : 2023-09-28 DOI: 10.1038/s41562-023-01727-1
Stella Nyanzi
Academic freedom is increasingly threatened by homophobic legislation. Stella Nyanzi describes how this affects queer African scholars, and calls for resistance.
学术自由日益受到恐同立法的威胁。Stella Nyanzi 描述了这对非洲同性恋学者的影响,并呼吁人们进行抵抗。
{"title":"State-led homophobia threatens African academic freedom","authors":"Stella Nyanzi","doi":"10.1038/s41562-023-01727-1","DOIUrl":"10.1038/s41562-023-01727-1","url":null,"abstract":"Academic freedom is increasingly threatened by homophobic legislation. Stella Nyanzi describes how this affects queer African scholars, and calls for resistance.","PeriodicalId":19074,"journal":{"name":"Nature Human Behaviour","volume":"7 12","pages":"2046-2047"},"PeriodicalIF":29.9,"publicationDate":"2023-09-28","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"41136409","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Vaccine mandates and public trust do not have to be antagonistic 疫苗授权和公众信任不一定是对立的。
IF 29.9 1区 心理学 Q1 MULTIDISCIPLINARY SCIENCES Pub Date : 2023-09-26 DOI: 10.1038/s41562-023-01720-8
Maya J. Goldenberg, Bipin Adhikari, Lorenz von Seidlein, Phaik Yeong Cheah, Heidi J. Larson
{"title":"Vaccine mandates and public trust do not have to be antagonistic","authors":"Maya J. Goldenberg, Bipin Adhikari, Lorenz von Seidlein, Phaik Yeong Cheah, Heidi J. Larson","doi":"10.1038/s41562-023-01720-8","DOIUrl":"10.1038/s41562-023-01720-8","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":19074,"journal":{"name":"Nature Human Behaviour","volume":"7 10","pages":"1605-1606"},"PeriodicalIF":29.9,"publicationDate":"2023-09-26","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"41124776","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
From alternative conceptions of honesty to alternative facts in communications by US politicians 从诚实的另类概念到美国政客沟通中的另类事实。
IF 29.9 1区 心理学 Q1 MULTIDISCIPLINARY SCIENCES Pub Date : 2023-09-25 DOI: 10.1038/s41562-023-01691-w
Jana Lasser, Segun T. Aroyehun, Fabio Carrella, Almog Simchon, David Garcia, Stephan Lewandowsky
The spread of online misinformation on social media is increasingly perceived as a problem for societal cohesion and democracy. The role of political leaders in this process has attracted less research attention, even though politicians who ‘speak their mind’ are perceived by segments of the public as authentic and honest even if their statements are unsupported by evidence. By analysing communications by members of the US Congress on Twitter between 2011 and 2022, we show that politicians’ conception of honesty has undergone a distinct shift, with authentic belief speaking that may be decoupled from evidence becoming more prominent and more differentiated from explicitly evidence-based fact speaking. We show that for Republicans—but not Democrats—an increase in belief speaking of 10% is associated with a decrease of 12.8 points of quality (NewsGuard scoring system) in the sources shared in a tweet. In contrast, an increase in fact-speaking language is associated with an increase in quality of sources for both parties. Our study is observational and cannot support causal inferences. However, our results are consistent with the hypothesis that the current dissemination of misinformation in political discourse is linked to an alternative understanding of truth and honesty that emphasizes invocation of subjective belief at the expense of reliance on evidence. By examining patterns in public-facing communications of US politicians, the authors identify two honesty-related concepts: belief speaking and fact speaking. They find that for Republicans, but not Democrats, an increase of belief speaking is associated with a decrease in the quality of the shared content sources.
社交媒体上网络错误信息的传播越来越被视为社会凝聚力和民主的问题。政治领导人在这一过程中的作用引起的研究关注较少,尽管“说出自己想法”的政客被部分公众认为是真实和诚实的,即使他们的言论没有证据支持。通过分析2011年至2022年间美国国会议员在推特上的交流,我们发现政客们对诚实的概念发生了明显的转变,可能与证据脱钩的真实信念演讲变得更加突出,与明确的循证事实演讲更加不同。我们发现,对于共和党人而不是民主党人来说,在推特中分享的消息来源中,10%的信念增加与12.8分的质量下降(NewsGuard评分系统)有关。相比之下,口语的增加与双方来源质量的提高有关。我们的研究是观察性的,不能支持因果推断。然而,我们的研究结果与以下假设一致,即当前政治话语中错误信息的传播与对真理和诚实的另一种理解有关,这种理解强调以牺牲对证据的依赖为代价援引主观信念。
{"title":"From alternative conceptions of honesty to alternative facts in communications by US politicians","authors":"Jana Lasser, Segun T. Aroyehun, Fabio Carrella, Almog Simchon, David Garcia, Stephan Lewandowsky","doi":"10.1038/s41562-023-01691-w","DOIUrl":"10.1038/s41562-023-01691-w","url":null,"abstract":"The spread of online misinformation on social media is increasingly perceived as a problem for societal cohesion and democracy. The role of political leaders in this process has attracted less research attention, even though politicians who ‘speak their mind’ are perceived by segments of the public as authentic and honest even if their statements are unsupported by evidence. By analysing communications by members of the US Congress on Twitter between 2011 and 2022, we show that politicians’ conception of honesty has undergone a distinct shift, with authentic belief speaking that may be decoupled from evidence becoming more prominent and more differentiated from explicitly evidence-based fact speaking. We show that for Republicans—but not Democrats—an increase in belief speaking of 10% is associated with a decrease of 12.8 points of quality (NewsGuard scoring system) in the sources shared in a tweet. In contrast, an increase in fact-speaking language is associated with an increase in quality of sources for both parties. Our study is observational and cannot support causal inferences. However, our results are consistent with the hypothesis that the current dissemination of misinformation in political discourse is linked to an alternative understanding of truth and honesty that emphasizes invocation of subjective belief at the expense of reliance on evidence. By examining patterns in public-facing communications of US politicians, the authors identify two honesty-related concepts: belief speaking and fact speaking. They find that for Republicans, but not Democrats, an increase of belief speaking is associated with a decrease in the quality of the shared content sources.","PeriodicalId":19074,"journal":{"name":"Nature Human Behaviour","volume":"7 12","pages":"2140-2151"},"PeriodicalIF":29.9,"publicationDate":"2023-09-25","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC10730411/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"41142704","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Could a shift in society’s conception of ‘honesty’ explain the spread of misinformation in the USA? 社会对“诚实”概念的转变能解释美国错误信息的传播吗?
IF 29.9 1区 心理学 Q1 MULTIDISCIPLINARY SCIENCES Pub Date : 2023-09-25 DOI: 10.1038/s41562-023-01692-9
We identified two components of honesty — ‘belief speaking’ and ‘fact speaking’ — in public-facing communication by US politicians. For Republicans, belief speaking is strongly associated with the sharing of untrustworthy information. Fact speaking is associated with the sharing of more reliable information, irrespective of party affiliation.
我们在美国政界人士面向公众的交流中发现了诚实的两个组成部分--"信念发言 "和 "事实发言"。对于共和党人来说,"信念发言 "与分享不可信的信息密切相关。无论属于哪个党派,讲事实都与分享更可靠的信息有关。
{"title":"Could a shift in society’s conception of ‘honesty’ explain the spread of misinformation in the USA?","authors":"","doi":"10.1038/s41562-023-01692-9","DOIUrl":"10.1038/s41562-023-01692-9","url":null,"abstract":"We identified two components of honesty — ‘belief speaking’ and ‘fact speaking’ — in public-facing communication by US politicians. For Republicans, belief speaking is strongly associated with the sharing of untrustworthy information. Fact speaking is associated with the sharing of more reliable information, irrespective of party affiliation.","PeriodicalId":19074,"journal":{"name":"Nature Human Behaviour","volume":"7 12","pages":"2062-2063"},"PeriodicalIF":29.9,"publicationDate":"2023-09-25","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"41142601","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Discrimination reduces work effort of those who are disadvantaged and those who are advantaged by it 歧视减少了处于不利地位的人和处于有利地位的人的工作努力。
IF 29.9 1区 心理学 Q1 MULTIDISCIPLINARY SCIENCES Pub Date : 2023-09-21 DOI: 10.1038/s41562-023-01703-9
Nicholas Heiserman, Brent Simpson
Research shows that discrimination is widespread in work organizations, yet we know little about the causal effects of discrimination on employees’ work effort. Here we argue that, by decoupling effort from rewards, discrimination reduces the work effort of those who are disadvantaged by discrimination and those advantaged by it. We test these arguments against the results of five experiments designed to model promotion situations in organizations (total N = 1,184). Together, these studies show that when supervised by a manager with a discriminatory preference, both disadvantaged and advantaged workers reduce their work effort relative to a control condition where the manager is not discriminatory. The negative effect of discrimination is larger for those disadvantaged by it. These effects are mediated by employees’ beliefs about how strongly work will impact their chances of reward. We then demonstrate that the relatively greater effort of advantaged—versus disadvantaged—workers in discriminatory organizations leads to a self-fulfilling prophecy: when faced with this effort differential, managers (N = 119) who did not have a priori discriminatory attitudes judged the advantaged category as more competent and deserving of workplace advancement than the disadvantaged category. Our results show that even though discrimination reduces all workers’ effort, it can ultimately produce outcomes that reify and entrench discriminatory beliefs. Using a set of experiments, the authors show that discrimination reduces work effort of those who are disadvantaged and those who are advantaged by it.
研究表明,歧视在工作组织中普遍存在,但我们对歧视对员工工作努力的因果影响知之甚少。在这里,我们认为,通过将努力与奖励脱钩,歧视减少了那些因歧视而处于不利地位的人和那些因歧视处于有利地位的人的工作努力 = 1184)。总之,这些研究表明,在具有歧视性偏好的管理者的监督下,相对于管理者没有歧视性的控制条件,弱势和优势工人都会减少他们的工作努力。歧视对那些处于不利地位的人的负面影响更大。这些影响是由员工对工作将对他们获得奖励的机会产生多大影响的信念所介导的。然后我们证明,在歧视性组织中,优势员工与弱势员工相对更大的努力会导致一个自我实现的预言:当面临这种努力差异时,管理者(N = 119)没有先验歧视态度的人认为优势类别比劣势类别更有能力,更值得在工作场所获得晋升。我们的研究结果表明,即使歧视减少了所有工人的努力,但它最终也会产生具体化和巩固歧视信念的结果。
{"title":"Discrimination reduces work effort of those who are disadvantaged and those who are advantaged by it","authors":"Nicholas Heiserman, Brent Simpson","doi":"10.1038/s41562-023-01703-9","DOIUrl":"10.1038/s41562-023-01703-9","url":null,"abstract":"Research shows that discrimination is widespread in work organizations, yet we know little about the causal effects of discrimination on employees’ work effort. Here we argue that, by decoupling effort from rewards, discrimination reduces the work effort of those who are disadvantaged by discrimination and those advantaged by it. We test these arguments against the results of five experiments designed to model promotion situations in organizations (total N = 1,184). Together, these studies show that when supervised by a manager with a discriminatory preference, both disadvantaged and advantaged workers reduce their work effort relative to a control condition where the manager is not discriminatory. The negative effect of discrimination is larger for those disadvantaged by it. These effects are mediated by employees’ beliefs about how strongly work will impact their chances of reward. We then demonstrate that the relatively greater effort of advantaged—versus disadvantaged—workers in discriminatory organizations leads to a self-fulfilling prophecy: when faced with this effort differential, managers (N = 119) who did not have a priori discriminatory attitudes judged the advantaged category as more competent and deserving of workplace advancement than the disadvantaged category. Our results show that even though discrimination reduces all workers’ effort, it can ultimately produce outcomes that reify and entrench discriminatory beliefs. Using a set of experiments, the authors show that discrimination reduces work effort of those who are disadvantaged and those who are advantaged by it.","PeriodicalId":19074,"journal":{"name":"Nature Human Behaviour","volume":"7 11","pages":"1890-1898"},"PeriodicalIF":29.9,"publicationDate":"2023-09-21","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"41140169","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
A social-semantic working-memory account for two canonical language areas 社会语义工作记忆涉及两个规范语言领域。
IF 29.9 1区 心理学 Q1 MULTIDISCIPLINARY SCIENCES Pub Date : 2023-09-21 DOI: 10.1038/s41562-023-01704-8
Guangyao Zhang, Yangwen Xu, Xiuyi Wang, Jixing Li, Weiting Shi, Yanchao Bi, Nan Lin
Language and social cognition are traditionally studied as separate cognitive domains, yet accumulative studies reveal overlapping neural correlates at the left ventral temporoparietal junction (vTPJ) and the left lateral anterior temporal lobe (lATL), which have been attributed to sentence processing and social concept activation. We propose a common cognitive component underlying both effects: social-semantic working memory. We confirmed two key predictions of our hypothesis using functional MRI. First, the left vTPJ and lATL showed sensitivity to sentences only when the sentences conveyed social meaning; second, these regions showed persistent social-semantic-selective activity after the linguistic stimuli disappeared. We additionally found that both regions were sensitive to the socialness of non-linguistic stimuli and were more tightly connected with the social-semantic-processing areas than with the sentence-processing areas. The converging evidence indicates the social-semantic working-memory function of the left vTPJ and lATL and challenges the general-semantic and/or syntactic accounts for the neural activity of these regions. In a series of human functional MRI studies, Zhang et al. find that the activation of two brain areas typically involved in language comprehension reflects working memory of social semantics rather than general semantic or syntactic processing.
传统上,语言和社会认知是作为独立的认知领域进行研究的,但累积研究揭示了左腹侧颞顶叶交界处(vTPJ)和左外侧前颞叶(lATL)的重叠神经相关性,这归因于句子处理和社会概念激活。我们提出了一个共同的认知成分,作为这两种效应的基础:社会语义工作记忆。我们使用功能MRI证实了我们假设的两个关键预测。首先,左vTPJ和lATL只有在句子传达社会意义时才对句子表现出敏感性;其次,在语言刺激消失后,这些区域表现出持续的社会语义选择活动。我们还发现,这两个区域对非语言刺激的社会性都很敏感,并且与社会语义处理区域的联系比与句子处理区域的更紧密。趋同证据表明左vTPJ和lATL的社会语义工作记忆功能,并对这些区域神经活动的一般语义和/或句法解释提出了质疑。
{"title":"A social-semantic working-memory account for two canonical language areas","authors":"Guangyao Zhang, Yangwen Xu, Xiuyi Wang, Jixing Li, Weiting Shi, Yanchao Bi, Nan Lin","doi":"10.1038/s41562-023-01704-8","DOIUrl":"10.1038/s41562-023-01704-8","url":null,"abstract":"Language and social cognition are traditionally studied as separate cognitive domains, yet accumulative studies reveal overlapping neural correlates at the left ventral temporoparietal junction (vTPJ) and the left lateral anterior temporal lobe (lATL), which have been attributed to sentence processing and social concept activation. We propose a common cognitive component underlying both effects: social-semantic working memory. We confirmed two key predictions of our hypothesis using functional MRI. First, the left vTPJ and lATL showed sensitivity to sentences only when the sentences conveyed social meaning; second, these regions showed persistent social-semantic-selective activity after the linguistic stimuli disappeared. We additionally found that both regions were sensitive to the socialness of non-linguistic stimuli and were more tightly connected with the social-semantic-processing areas than with the sentence-processing areas. The converging evidence indicates the social-semantic working-memory function of the left vTPJ and lATL and challenges the general-semantic and/or syntactic accounts for the neural activity of these regions. In a series of human functional MRI studies, Zhang et al. find that the activation of two brain areas typically involved in language comprehension reflects working memory of social semantics rather than general semantic or syntactic processing.","PeriodicalId":19074,"journal":{"name":"Nature Human Behaviour","volume":"7 11","pages":"1980-1997"},"PeriodicalIF":29.9,"publicationDate":"2023-09-21","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"41156898","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Warming up cool cooperators 热身酷的合作者。
IF 29.9 1区 心理学 Q1 MULTIDISCIPLINARY SCIENCES Pub Date : 2023-09-14 DOI: 10.1038/s41562-023-01687-6
Eamonn Ferguson, Claire Lawrence, Sarah Bowen, Carley N. Gemelli, Amy Rozsa, Konrad Niekrasz, Anne van Dongen, Lisa A. Williams, Amanda Thijsen, Nicola Guerin, Barbara Masser, Tanya E. Davison
Explaining why someone repeats high-cost cooperation towards non-reciprocating strangers is difficult. Warm glow offers an explanation. We argue that warm glow, as a mechanism to sustain long-term cooperation, cools off over time but can be warmed up with a simple intervention message. We tested our predictions in the context of repeat voluntary blood donation (high-cost helping of a non-reciprocating stranger) across 6 studies: a field-based experiment (n = 5,821) comparing warm-glow and impure-altruism messages; an implementation study comparing a 3-yr pre-implementation period among all first-time donors in Australia (N = 270,353) with a 2-yr post-implementation period (N = 170, 317); and 4 studies (n = 716, 1,124, 932, 1,592) exploring mechanisms. We show that there are relatively warm and cool cooperators, not cooling cooperators. Cooperation among cool cooperators is enhanced by a warm-glow-plus-identity message. Furthermore, the behavioural facilitation of future cooperation, by booking an appointment, is associated with being a warm cooperator. Societal implications are discussed. Ferguson et al. test the effectiveness of messages designed to increase rates of repeat blood donation and find that warm-glow feelings as a motivation for cooperation cool over time but can be reactivated.
很难解释为什么有人会对不互相回报的陌生人重复高成本的合作。暖光提供了一个解释。我们认为,暖光作为一种维持长期合作的机制,随着时间的推移会逐渐冷却,但可以通过简单的干预信息进行预热。我们在6项研究中测试了我们在重复自愿献血(非互惠陌生人的高成本帮助)背景下的预测:一项基于现场的实验(n = 5,821),比较了温暖的光辉和纯粹的利他主义信息;一项实施研究,比较了澳大利亚所有首次捐助者的3年实施前期(N = 270,353)和2年实施后期(N = 170, 317);4项研究(n = 716, 1,124, 932, 1,592)探讨了机制。我们表明有相对温暖的和冷的合作者,而不是冷的合作者。酷酷的合作者之间的合作通过温暖的发光加上身份信息得到加强。此外,通过预约来促进未来合作的行为,与成为热情的合作者有关。讨论了社会影响。
{"title":"Warming up cool cooperators","authors":"Eamonn Ferguson, Claire Lawrence, Sarah Bowen, Carley N. Gemelli, Amy Rozsa, Konrad Niekrasz, Anne van Dongen, Lisa A. Williams, Amanda Thijsen, Nicola Guerin, Barbara Masser, Tanya E. Davison","doi":"10.1038/s41562-023-01687-6","DOIUrl":"10.1038/s41562-023-01687-6","url":null,"abstract":"Explaining why someone repeats high-cost cooperation towards non-reciprocating strangers is difficult. Warm glow offers an explanation. We argue that warm glow, as a mechanism to sustain long-term cooperation, cools off over time but can be warmed up with a simple intervention message. We tested our predictions in the context of repeat voluntary blood donation (high-cost helping of a non-reciprocating stranger) across 6 studies: a field-based experiment (n = 5,821) comparing warm-glow and impure-altruism messages; an implementation study comparing a 3-yr pre-implementation period among all first-time donors in Australia (N = 270,353) with a 2-yr post-implementation period (N = 170, 317); and 4 studies (n = 716, 1,124, 932, 1,592) exploring mechanisms. We show that there are relatively warm and cool cooperators, not cooling cooperators. Cooperation among cool cooperators is enhanced by a warm-glow-plus-identity message. Furthermore, the behavioural facilitation of future cooperation, by booking an appointment, is associated with being a warm cooperator. Societal implications are discussed. Ferguson et al. test the effectiveness of messages designed to increase rates of repeat blood donation and find that warm-glow feelings as a motivation for cooperation cool over time but can be reactivated.","PeriodicalId":19074,"journal":{"name":"Nature Human Behaviour","volume":"7 11","pages":"1917-1932"},"PeriodicalIF":29.9,"publicationDate":"2023-09-14","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC10663147/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10244607","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Nature Human Behaviour
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1