首页 > 最新文献

Polish Psychological Bulletin最新文献

英文 中文
Psychological Aspects of Captivity in the War in the East of Ukraine 乌克兰东部战争中被俘的心理问题
Q4 Psychology Pub Date : 2023-07-19 DOI: 10.24425/ppb.2021.136820
O. Timchenko, I. Prykhodko, Yurii Shyrobokov, N. Onishchenko, V. Lefterov
: The study aimed to determine the psychological aspects of captivity in the War in the East of Ukraine: the purposes and motives of the capture of Ukrainian Forces (UF); the types of captivity and their specifics; the stages and phases of captivity. The measures included a questionnaire and interview method. 694 former prisoners of war (POWs) (servicemen of the Armed Forces of Ukraine and soldiers of volunteer battalions) participated in the study. The research results revealed the purposes of capturing UF: to stop UF advance; obtaining intelligence; demoralization of UF; demonstration of military superiority; capturing prisoners for exchange; unwillingness to kill; receiving a ransom. The UF invaders were military units, professional mercenaries’ units, and gang formation units. The stages of captivity (capture and transportation to a place of permanent detention; first interrogation; being held captive; exchange of POWs and homecoming) were characterized by intimidation, aggression, physical, psychological and sexual violence against POWs, the purposeful creation of an environment of mass psychosis among POWs. Captivity kept the POWs in constant tension and fear. The altered mental status of POWs took place in successive phases: life reactions, shock, psychological demobilization, denouement, recovery, and
:该研究旨在确定乌克兰东部战争中被俘的心理方面:抓捕乌克兰军队的目的和动机;圈养的类型及其具体情况;囚禁的阶段和阶段。这些措施包括问卷调查和访谈方法。694名前战俘(乌克兰武装部队军人和志愿营士兵)参加了这项研究。研究结果揭示了捕捉UF的目的:阻止UF的前进;获取情报;UF士气低落;展示军事优势;俘虏交换;不愿杀人;收到赎金。UF入侵者是军事单位、职业雇佣兵单位和帮派组织单位。囚禁阶段(捕获并运送到永久拘留地;第一次审讯;被囚禁;交换战俘和回家)的特点是对战俘的恐吓、攻击、身体、心理和性暴力,有目的地在战俘中创造一个群体性精神病的环境。被俘使战俘们始终处于紧张和恐惧之中。战俘心理状态的改变分为几个阶段:生活反应、震惊、心理复员、结局、康复和
{"title":"Psychological Aspects of Captivity in the War in the East of Ukraine","authors":"O. Timchenko, I. Prykhodko, Yurii Shyrobokov, N. Onishchenko, V. Lefterov","doi":"10.24425/ppb.2021.136820","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.24425/ppb.2021.136820","url":null,"abstract":": The study aimed to determine the psychological aspects of captivity in the War in the East of Ukraine: the purposes and motives of the capture of Ukrainian Forces (UF); the types of captivity and their specifics; the stages and phases of captivity. The measures included a questionnaire and interview method. 694 former prisoners of war (POWs) (servicemen of the Armed Forces of Ukraine and soldiers of volunteer battalions) participated in the study. The research results revealed the purposes of capturing UF: to stop UF advance; obtaining intelligence; demoralization of UF; demonstration of military superiority; capturing prisoners for exchange; unwillingness to kill; receiving a ransom. The UF invaders were military units, professional mercenaries’ units, and gang formation units. The stages of captivity (capture and transportation to a place of permanent detention; first interrogation; being held captive; exchange of POWs and homecoming) were characterized by intimidation, aggression, physical, psychological and sexual violence against POWs, the purposeful creation of an environment of mass psychosis among POWs. Captivity kept the POWs in constant tension and fear. The altered mental status of POWs took place in successive phases: life reactions, shock, psychological demobilization, denouement, recovery, and","PeriodicalId":38657,"journal":{"name":"Polish Psychological Bulletin","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2023-07-19","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"45986755","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 3
Coping and Mental Health during Emerging Adulthood. The Relationships between Coping Strategies and Risk of Depression and Life Satisfaction among Students in Higher Education 成年初期的应对和心理健康。大学生应对策略与抑郁风险、生活满意度的关系
Q4 Psychology Pub Date : 2023-07-19 DOI: 10.24425/ppb.2022.141139
Karol Konaszewski, Małgorzata Niesiobędzka, Marcin Kolemba
: Background: The phenomenon of accumulating tasks, characteristic of emerging adulthood, intensifies perceived stress and stimulates coping activity. The nature and intensity of the coping strategies used to deal with challenges can affect mental health in emerging adulthood. The purpose of the study was to investigate the relationship between coping strategies and mental health in a group of emerging adults-students in higher education. Methods: The study included 390 emerging adults, students in higher education. Coping strategies were measured with the COPE Questionnaire and information on mental health was called using the Kutcher Adolescent Depression Scale and the Satisfaction with Life Scale. The confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) with maximum likelihood (ML) estimation was used to assess the factor structure of the variables and structural equation modeling was used to test the hypotheses. Results: The data mostly confirmed the hypotheses. Avoidance strategies turned out to be the strongest predictor of mental health, specifically negative mental health outcomes. Problem-focused strategies were a stronger predictor of quality of life than emotion-focused and support-seeking strategies. Emotion-focused strategies did not predict depression. Coping strategies, especially avoidance strategies, play a crucial role in mental health during emerging adulthood. Conclusions: Learning to cope enables students to deal with difficult tasks and challenges of this period more effectively, and minimizes their risk of depression, and increases their life satisfaction.
背景:任务积累现象是初成人的特征,它加剧了感知压力并刺激了应对活动。用于应对挑战的应对策略的性质和强度会影响成年初期的心理健康。摘要本研究的目的是探讨一组初成成人高等教育学生的应对策略与心理健康的关系。方法:对390名初出期成人、高等院校在校生进行调查。采用COPE问卷对应对策略进行测量,采用Kutcher青少年抑郁量表和生活满意度量表对心理健康信息进行评估。采用验证性因子分析(CFA)和最大似然(ML)估计来评估变量的因素结构,并采用结构方程模型来检验假设。结果:数据基本证实了假设。逃避策略被证明是心理健康的最强预测因子,特别是负面的心理健康结果。以问题为中心的策略比以情绪为中心和寻求支持的策略更能预测生活质量。以情绪为中心的策略并不能预测抑郁症。应对策略,特别是回避策略,在成年初期的心理健康中起着至关重要的作用。结论:学会应对能使学生更有效地应对这一时期的困难任务和挑战,降低抑郁风险,提高生活满意度。
{"title":"Coping and Mental Health during Emerging Adulthood. The Relationships between Coping Strategies and Risk of Depression and Life Satisfaction among Students in Higher Education","authors":"Karol Konaszewski, Małgorzata Niesiobędzka, Marcin Kolemba","doi":"10.24425/ppb.2022.141139","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.24425/ppb.2022.141139","url":null,"abstract":": Background: The phenomenon of accumulating tasks, characteristic of emerging adulthood, intensifies perceived stress and stimulates coping activity. The nature and intensity of the coping strategies used to deal with challenges can affect mental health in emerging adulthood. The purpose of the study was to investigate the relationship between coping strategies and mental health in a group of emerging adults-students in higher education. Methods: The study included 390 emerging adults, students in higher education. Coping strategies were measured with the COPE Questionnaire and information on mental health was called using the Kutcher Adolescent Depression Scale and the Satisfaction with Life Scale. The confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) with maximum likelihood (ML) estimation was used to assess the factor structure of the variables and structural equation modeling was used to test the hypotheses. Results: The data mostly confirmed the hypotheses. Avoidance strategies turned out to be the strongest predictor of mental health, specifically negative mental health outcomes. Problem-focused strategies were a stronger predictor of quality of life than emotion-focused and support-seeking strategies. Emotion-focused strategies did not predict depression. Coping strategies, especially avoidance strategies, play a crucial role in mental health during emerging adulthood. Conclusions: Learning to cope enables students to deal with difficult tasks and challenges of this period more effectively, and minimizes their risk of depression, and increases their life satisfaction.","PeriodicalId":38657,"journal":{"name":"Polish Psychological Bulletin","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2023-07-19","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"44704860","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 2
The Child as an Observer: The Influence of Adults’ Nonverbal Messages on Children’s Social Attitudes 儿童作为观察者:成人非言语信息对儿童社会态度的影响
Q4 Psychology Pub Date : 2023-07-19 DOI: 10.24425/PPB.2019.131500
Anna Jurasińska, Marcin Bukowski, M. Białecka-Pikul
Previous research showed that children can exhibit preferences for social categories already at preschool age. One of the crucial factors in the development of children’s attitudes toward others is children’s observation and imitation of adults’ nonverbal messages. The aim of our study is to examine whether children’s tendency to perceive and follow nonverbally expressed attitudes toward other people is related to ingroup bias, i.e. the tendency to favor one’s own group over other groups. We examined 175 preschool children (age in months: 61–87; M = 72.6, SD = 6.53) presenting them with a video of a conversation between a message sender and a message recipient. The study was conducted in a minimal group paradigm. We found that children accurately identified the message sender’s attitude toward the recipient and also generalized this attitude to other members of the new group. We also found explicit ingroup bias among children from the message sender’s group. However, no generalization of the sender’s attitude to other ingroup members was found. The results are discussed in reference to previous findings on the role of imitation of adult’s non-verbal behavior for the development of social attitudes among children.
先前的研究表明,儿童在学龄前就可以表现出对社会类别的偏好。儿童对他人态度发展的关键因素之一是儿童对成人非语言信息的观察和模仿。我们研究的目的是检验儿童感知和遵循非语言表达的对他人态度的倾向是否与群体内偏见有关,即倾向于偏袒自己的群体而非其他群体。我们调查了175名学龄前儿童(月龄:61~87;M=72.6,SD=6.53),向他们展示了一段信息发送者和信息接收者之间的对话视频。这项研究是在最小群体范式下进行的。我们发现,孩子们准确地识别了信息发送者对接收者的态度,并将这种态度推广到了新群体的其他成员身上。我们还发现,来自信息发送者群体的儿童存在明显的群体内偏见。然而,没有发现发件人对其他小组成员的态度的概括。这些结果是参考先前关于模仿成人非语言行为对儿童社会态度发展的作用的研究结果进行讨论的。
{"title":"The Child as an Observer: The Influence of Adults’ Nonverbal Messages on Children’s Social Attitudes","authors":"Anna Jurasińska, Marcin Bukowski, M. Białecka-Pikul","doi":"10.24425/PPB.2019.131500","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.24425/PPB.2019.131500","url":null,"abstract":"Previous research showed that children can exhibit preferences for social categories already at preschool age. One of the crucial factors in the development of children’s attitudes toward others is children’s observation and imitation of adults’ nonverbal messages. The aim of our study is to examine whether children’s tendency to perceive and follow nonverbally expressed attitudes toward other people is related to ingroup bias, i.e. the tendency to favor one’s own group over other groups. We examined 175 preschool children (age in months: 61–87; M = 72.6, SD = 6.53) presenting them with a video of a conversation between a message sender and a message recipient. The study was conducted in a minimal group paradigm. We found that children accurately identified the message sender’s attitude toward the recipient and also generalized this attitude to other members of the new group. We also found explicit ingroup bias among children from the message sender’s group. However, no generalization of the sender’s attitude to other ingroup members was found. The results are discussed in reference to previous findings on the role of imitation of adult’s non-verbal behavior for the development of social attitudes among children.","PeriodicalId":38657,"journal":{"name":"Polish Psychological Bulletin","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2023-07-19","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"45061725","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Being more idiographic in the nomothetic world 在本体世界中更加具体
Q4 Psychology Pub Date : 2023-07-19 DOI: 10.24425/PPB.2019.129453
Tomasz Korulczyk, A. Biela, N. Blampied
Since psychology emerged as an independent field of knowledge, there has been no consensus as to how it should develop, either, in the idiographic or nomothetic way. In the course of time, due to a commitment to what was seen as objectivity in science, the nomothetic approach came to dominate psychology. Thus, researchers used mostly quantitative psychometric methods to establish general rules of human behaviour. In doing so, the essence of nomothetic research is to be extremely careful when interpreting results not to make a reasoning mistake such as the ecological fallacy, as may happen when a researcher draws conclusions about nature of the individual in the group based on average results of the whole group. In the article, we presented two methods for longitudinal research designs which address this problem, and give more idiographic information about participants; via the Reliable Change Index and the Modified Brinley Plot. Finally, we provide a IBM SPSS Statistics syntax automatizing the whole process of computation for these new features.
自从心理学作为一个独立的知识领域出现以来,对于它应该如何发展,无论是以具体的方式还是以规范的方式,都没有达成共识。随着时间的推移,由于对科学客观性的承诺,诺模主义方法开始主导心理学。因此,研究人员主要使用定量心理测量方法来建立人类行为的一般规则。在这样做的过程中,法理研究的本质是在解释结果时要格外小心,不要犯生态谬误等推理错误,因为当研究人员根据整个群体的平均结果得出关于群体中个体性质的结论时,可能会发生这种错误。在这篇文章中,我们提出了两种纵向研究设计方法来解决这个问题,并提供了更多关于参与者的具体信息;通过可靠变化指数和修正的Brinley图。最后,我们提供了一个IBMSPSS统计语法,使这些新功能的整个计算过程自动化。
{"title":"Being more idiographic in the nomothetic world","authors":"Tomasz Korulczyk, A. Biela, N. Blampied","doi":"10.24425/PPB.2019.129453","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.24425/PPB.2019.129453","url":null,"abstract":"Since psychology emerged as an independent field of knowledge, there has been no consensus as to how it should develop, either, in the idiographic or nomothetic way. In the course of time, due to a commitment to what was seen as objectivity in science, the nomothetic approach came to dominate psychology. Thus, researchers used mostly quantitative psychometric methods to establish general rules of human behaviour. In doing so, the essence of nomothetic research is to be extremely careful when interpreting results not to make a reasoning mistake such as the ecological fallacy, as may happen when a researcher draws conclusions about nature of the individual in the group based on average results of the whole group. In the article, we presented two methods for longitudinal research designs which address this problem, and give more idiographic information about participants; via the Reliable Change Index and the Modified Brinley Plot. Finally, we provide a IBM SPSS Statistics syntax automatizing the whole process of computation for these new features.","PeriodicalId":38657,"journal":{"name":"Polish Psychological Bulletin","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2023-07-19","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"45330704","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
Personality traits and subjective well-being with regard to problem of infertility: The mediating role of implicit self-theories and life-engagement 人格特质与不孕问题的主观幸福感:内隐自我理论与生活投入的中介作用
Q4 Psychology Pub Date : 2023-07-19 DOI: 10.24425/119481
E. Brygoła
In integrated approaches to personality (McAdams & Pals, 2006; McCrae & Costa, 1999), it is possible to examine relationships between personality traits, beliefs as characteristic adaptations, and subjective well-being. This research aimed to verify if implicit self-theories (belief about stability of human nature) proposed by Dweck (2000) and life-engagement proposed by Scheier et al. (2006) play a mediating role in relationships between personality traits and satisfaction with life. The relationships were examined with respect to infertility problem. A sample of 120 adults (aged 26–48; M = 36.60; SD = 4.82; 50% women) participated in the research. The mediation hypotheses were examined, and furthermore, four groups of couples were compared in terms of measured variables. The groups were: couples with (1) cured and (2) uncured infertility and couples who were not infertile and (3) have and (4) do not have children. Life-engagement mediated the relationship between Conscientiousness and satisfaction with life in the whole sample. The belief about stability of human nature mediated relationships between subjective well-being and Conscientiousness, Agreeableness, Neuroticism, and Extraversion only among couples with an infertility problem.
在人格的综合方法中(McAdams&Pals,2006;McCrae&Costa,1999),可以研究人格特征、作为特征适应的信念和主观幸福感之间的关系。本研究旨在验证Dweck(2000)提出的内隐自我理论(关于人性稳定性的信念)和Scheier等人提出的生活参与理论(2006)是否在人格特征和生活满意度之间的关系中发挥中介作用。研究了不孕不育的关系。120名成年人(年龄26-48岁;M=36.60;SD=4.82;50%为女性)参与了这项研究。对中介假说进行了检验,此外,四组夫妇在测量变量方面进行了比较。这些组是:(1)已治愈和(2)未治愈不孕的夫妇,以及未不孕和(3)有孩子和(4)没有孩子的夫妇。在整个样本中,生活投入介导了尽责性和对生活的满意度之间的关系。关于人性稳定性的信念只在有不孕问题的夫妇中介导了主观幸福感与尽责性、合意性、神经质和外向性之间的关系。
{"title":"Personality traits and subjective well-being with regard to problem of infertility: The mediating role of implicit self-theories and life-engagement","authors":"E. Brygoła","doi":"10.24425/119481","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.24425/119481","url":null,"abstract":"In integrated approaches to personality (McAdams & Pals, 2006; McCrae & Costa, 1999), it is possible to examine relationships between personality traits, beliefs as characteristic adaptations, and subjective well-being. This research aimed to verify if implicit self-theories (belief about stability of human nature) proposed by Dweck (2000) and life-engagement proposed by Scheier et al. (2006) play a mediating role in relationships between personality traits and satisfaction with life. The relationships were examined with respect to infertility problem. A sample of 120 adults (aged 26–48; M = 36.60; SD = 4.82; 50% women) participated in the research. The mediation hypotheses were examined, and furthermore, four groups of couples were compared in terms of measured variables. The groups were: couples with (1) cured and (2) uncured infertility and couples who were not infertile and (3) have and (4) do not have children. Life-engagement mediated the relationship between Conscientiousness and satisfaction with life in the whole sample. The belief about stability of human nature mediated relationships between subjective well-being and Conscientiousness, Agreeableness, Neuroticism, and Extraversion only among couples with an infertility problem.","PeriodicalId":38657,"journal":{"name":"Polish Psychological Bulletin","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2023-07-19","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"44663905","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 2
Cognitive processes involved in metaphor aptness 隐喻能力的认知过程
Q4 Psychology Pub Date : 2023-07-19 DOI: 10.24425/ppb.2021.137257
Omid Khatin-Zadeh, Z. Eskandari
: This article looks at metaphor aptness from the perspective of the class-inclusion model of metaphor comprehension and those models that assume a componential nature for the meanings of concepts. When the metaphor X is a Y is processed, the concept of X is included in a metaphorical class that is represented by Y , which is usually the most typical member of the metaphorical class. Degree of saliency of the defining feature in the vehicle and the extent to which this feature matches a relevant dimension of topic is the key factor in the degree of aptness of the metaphor. Degree of aptness becomes more complex in those metaphors that describe an abstract concept in terms of another concept. These metaphors include X into a metaphorical class through the mediation of those concepts that are associated to the abstract concept. If the associated concepts have a high degree of typicality in the metaphorical class, they could be better mediators for including the abstract concept into the metaphorical class. The variations of abstract concepts across individuals and their dependency on contexts and cultures could explain why such metaphors may have different degrees of aptness for different people.
本文从隐喻理解的类包含模型和那些假设概念意义具有组件性的模型的角度来考察隐喻的适宜性。当隐喻X是一个Y被加工时,X的概念被包含在一个隐喻类中,这个隐喻类以Y为代表,而Y通常是隐喻类中最典型的成员。喻体中定义特征的显著程度以及该特征与主题相关维度的匹配程度是隐喻恰当程度的关键因素。在那些用另一个概念来描述一个抽象概念的隐喻中,适应程度变得更加复杂。这些隐喻通过与抽象概念相关的那些概念的中介,将X纳入一个隐喻类。如果相关概念在隐喻类中具有高度的典型性,则它们可能是将抽象概念纳入隐喻类的更好中介。抽象概念在个体之间的差异及其对环境和文化的依赖可以解释为什么这些隐喻对不同的人有不同程度的适应性。
{"title":"Cognitive processes involved in metaphor aptness","authors":"Omid Khatin-Zadeh, Z. Eskandari","doi":"10.24425/ppb.2021.137257","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.24425/ppb.2021.137257","url":null,"abstract":": This article looks at metaphor aptness from the perspective of the class-inclusion model of metaphor comprehension and those models that assume a componential nature for the meanings of concepts. When the metaphor X is a Y is processed, the concept of X is included in a metaphorical class that is represented by Y , which is usually the most typical member of the metaphorical class. Degree of saliency of the defining feature in the vehicle and the extent to which this feature matches a relevant dimension of topic is the key factor in the degree of aptness of the metaphor. Degree of aptness becomes more complex in those metaphors that describe an abstract concept in terms of another concept. These metaphors include X into a metaphorical class through the mediation of those concepts that are associated to the abstract concept. If the associated concepts have a high degree of typicality in the metaphorical class, they could be better mediators for including the abstract concept into the metaphorical class. The variations of abstract concepts across individuals and their dependency on contexts and cultures could explain why such metaphors may have different degrees of aptness for different people.","PeriodicalId":38657,"journal":{"name":"Polish Psychological Bulletin","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2023-07-19","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"44972489","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Observation inflation and interrogative suggestibility: Different but related memory errors 观察膨胀和疑问暗示性:不同但相关的记忆错误
Q4 Psychology Pub Date : 2023-07-19 DOI: 10.24425/ppb.2020.134728
Magdalena Kękuś, Regina Dziubańska, Iga Komęza, I. Dudek, Klaudia Chylińska, Malwina Szpitalak, R. Polczyk
: The observation inflation effect consists in the fact that observing an action being performed can create false memories that this action has actually been performed by the observer. The present study examined the relationship between this effect and interrogative suggestibility. A procedure based on the Gudjonsson Suggestibility Scale was used to assess two kinds of suggestibility: the tendency to yield to suggestive questions (Yield) and the tendency to change answers after feedback (Shift). The participants first watched a film depicting a woman performing simple activities and performed various activities themselves during the film. In order to determine whether the observation inflation effect occurred, the participants performed a source-monitoring test. The observation inflation effect was replicated. Observation inflation correlated positively with Yield but not with Shift. This pattern of results can be explained by the fact these two indicators are different aspects of interrogative suggestibility. Shift is more related to social influence, while Yield is more cognitive in its nature.
观察膨胀效应包括这样一个事实,即观察一个正在执行的动作可以产生错误的记忆,认为这个动作实际上是由观察者执行的。本研究考察了这种效应与询问性暗示之间的关系。采用基于Gudjonsson建议性量表的程序来评估两种建议性:屈服于暗示问题的倾向(yield)和反馈后改变答案的倾向(Shift)。参与者首先观看了一部电影,描述了一个女人在做简单的活动,并在电影中自己做了各种各样的活动。为了确定观察膨胀效应是否发生,参与者进行了源监测测试。观测到的膨胀效应被复制了。观测通货膨胀与收益率正相关,与Shift不相关。这种结果的模式可以用这两个指标是疑问性暗示的不同方面来解释。Shift更多地与社会影响有关,而Yield在本质上更具有认知性。
{"title":"Observation inflation and interrogative suggestibility: Different but related memory errors","authors":"Magdalena Kękuś, Regina Dziubańska, Iga Komęza, I. Dudek, Klaudia Chylińska, Malwina Szpitalak, R. Polczyk","doi":"10.24425/ppb.2020.134728","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.24425/ppb.2020.134728","url":null,"abstract":": The observation inflation effect consists in the fact that observing an action being performed can create false memories that this action has actually been performed by the observer. The present study examined the relationship between this effect and interrogative suggestibility. A procedure based on the Gudjonsson Suggestibility Scale was used to assess two kinds of suggestibility: the tendency to yield to suggestive questions (Yield) and the tendency to change answers after feedback (Shift). The participants first watched a film depicting a woman performing simple activities and performed various activities themselves during the film. In order to determine whether the observation inflation effect occurred, the participants performed a source-monitoring test. The observation inflation effect was replicated. Observation inflation correlated positively with Yield but not with Shift. This pattern of results can be explained by the fact these two indicators are different aspects of interrogative suggestibility. Shift is more related to social influence, while Yield is more cognitive in its nature.","PeriodicalId":38657,"journal":{"name":"Polish Psychological Bulletin","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2023-07-19","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"44238942","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
How to measure math anxiety in young children? Psychometric properties of the modified Abbreviated Math Anxiety Scale for Elementary Children (mAMAS-E) 如何测量幼儿的数学焦虑?小学生数学焦虑简易量表(mAMAS-E)的心理测量特征
Q4 Psychology Pub Date : 2023-07-19 DOI: 10.24425/PPB.2019.131003
Monika Szczygieł
Starting in the early years of education, math anxiety is negatively related to mathematic outcomes, therefore there is a need for its adequate measurement in young children. This study presents the psychometric properties of the modified Abbreviated Math Anxiety Scale for Elementary Children (mAMAS-E) for firstto third-grade children based on mAMAS. The validity of mAMAS-E was determined by a series of tests. The analysis confirmed its two-factor structure (Testing and Learning), positive relationships between mAMAS-E and math, general, and test anxiety, and a negative relationship with mathematical achievement. Children with a high level of math self-esteem and math self-confidence (but not Polish language self-esteem and self-confidence) have lower math anxiety in comparison to those with a moderate level. The results also indicate that girls have a higher level of math anxiety than boys. The validity and internal consistency of mAMAS-E are satisfactory; therefore, mAMAS-E may be a recommendable questionnaire for measuring math anxiety in young children.
从教育的早期开始,数学焦虑与数学成绩呈负相关,因此有必要在幼儿中对其进行充分的测量。本研究介绍了基于简易数学焦虑量表(mAMAS-E)的小学一至三年级儿童的心理测量特性。通过一系列测试确定了mAMAS-E的有效性。该分析证实了其双因素结构(测试和学习),mAMAS-E与数学、一般和考试焦虑之间的正相关关系,以及与数学成绩的负相关关系。与中等水平的儿童相比,数学自尊和数学自信水平较高(但波兰语自尊和自信水平不高)的儿童数学焦虑较低。研究结果还表明,女孩的数学焦虑程度高于男孩。mAMAS-E的有效性和内部一致性令人满意;因此,mAMAS-E可能是一种值得推荐的测量幼儿数学焦虑的问卷。
{"title":"How to measure math anxiety in young children? Psychometric properties of the modified Abbreviated Math Anxiety Scale for Elementary Children (mAMAS-E)","authors":"Monika Szczygieł","doi":"10.24425/PPB.2019.131003","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.24425/PPB.2019.131003","url":null,"abstract":"Starting in the early years of education, math anxiety is negatively related to mathematic outcomes, therefore there is a need for its adequate measurement in young children. This study presents the psychometric properties of the modified Abbreviated Math Anxiety Scale for Elementary Children (mAMAS-E) for firstto third-grade children based on mAMAS. The validity of mAMAS-E was determined by a series of tests. The analysis confirmed its two-factor structure (Testing and Learning), positive relationships between mAMAS-E and math, general, and test anxiety, and a negative relationship with mathematical achievement. Children with a high level of math self-esteem and math self-confidence (but not Polish language self-esteem and self-confidence) have lower math anxiety in comparison to those with a moderate level. The results also indicate that girls have a higher level of math anxiety than boys. The validity and internal consistency of mAMAS-E are satisfactory; therefore, mAMAS-E may be a recommendable questionnaire for measuring math anxiety in young children.","PeriodicalId":38657,"journal":{"name":"Polish Psychological Bulletin","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2023-07-19","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"46360940","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 13
Tactile and Visual Virtual Reality Attention Distraction From Pain in Cold Pressor Test 触觉和视觉虚拟现实技术在冷压试验中对疼痛的注意力转移
Q4 Psychology Pub Date : 2023-07-19 DOI: 10.24425/ppb.2020.135464
Anna Bagrij
: The goal of this study was to test the efficacy of a tactile attention distraction from pain and compare its effectiveness with a virtual reality (VR) distraction on an analogous task. VR is considered to be the gold standard for attention distraction, but it cannot be used in certain clinical cases or for particular medical procedures. A repeated-measures experimental study was carried out with 42 participants using tactile and VR variants of an n -back task and a cold pressor test for pain. The independent variable was the distraction type (tactile, VR, or no-distraction) and the dependent variable was pain tolerance (i.e., time participants kept their hand in cold water). The results showed that both tactile and VR games effectively increased pain tolerance compared to the control condition. Effect sizes for both interventions were similar. However, the effect was observed only for female participants.
:本研究的目的是测试触觉注意力分散对疼痛的效果,并将其与虚拟现实(VR)分散对类似任务的效果进行比较。VR被认为是分散注意力的黄金标准,但它不能用于某些临床病例或特定的医疗程序。对42名参与者进行了一项重复测量实验研究,使用触觉和VR变体的n-back任务和冷加压疼痛测试。自变量是分心类型(触觉、VR或无分心),因变量是疼痛耐受性(即参与者把手放在冷水中的时间)。结果表明,与对照条件相比,触觉和VR游戏都有效地提高了疼痛耐受性。两种干预措施的效果大小相似。然而,这种影响只在女性参与者身上观察到。
{"title":"Tactile and Visual Virtual Reality Attention Distraction From Pain in Cold Pressor Test","authors":"Anna Bagrij","doi":"10.24425/ppb.2020.135464","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.24425/ppb.2020.135464","url":null,"abstract":": The goal of this study was to test the efficacy of a tactile attention distraction from pain and compare its effectiveness with a virtual reality (VR) distraction on an analogous task. VR is considered to be the gold standard for attention distraction, but it cannot be used in certain clinical cases or for particular medical procedures. A repeated-measures experimental study was carried out with 42 participants using tactile and VR variants of an n -back task and a cold pressor test for pain. The independent variable was the distraction type (tactile, VR, or no-distraction) and the dependent variable was pain tolerance (i.e., time participants kept their hand in cold water). The results showed that both tactile and VR games effectively increased pain tolerance compared to the control condition. Effect sizes for both interventions were similar. However, the effect was observed only for female participants.","PeriodicalId":38657,"journal":{"name":"Polish Psychological Bulletin","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2023-07-19","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"47523905","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Complaining during closed training: its functions and consequences 封闭训练中的抱怨:功能与后果
Q4 Psychology Pub Date : 2023-07-19 DOI: 10.24425/ppb.2020.133772
Marta Damroka Kaliszewska, D. Godlewska-Werner
: During occupational trainings given to Polish employees, one can quite often observe complaining. The instructor can use it for problem-solving or for purification. Thus, complaining plays an instrumental or cathartic function. This has consequences for the entire training process. The aim of the article is to present the phenomenon of complaining during training courses and to discuss its correlation with different variables such as learning results, participants’ mood and the evaluation of the training course. Questions were therefore posed about which function of complaining would be more conducive to the process of learning the material and result in an improvement of the participants’ mood, as well as how the instructor would be evaluated, at the response level, depending on which function of complaining is activated during the training. In order to answer these questions, the authors designed an experiment in which complaining was induced in members of an organization, performing either an instrumental function or a cathartic function. The results show that the most effective strategy is the use of its object as a point of departure for problem solution.
:在对波兰员工进行职业培训期间,人们经常会抱怨。教练可以使用它来解决问题或净化。因此,抱怨起到了一种工具或宣泄的作用。这会对整个培训过程产生影响。本文旨在介绍培训过程中的抱怨现象,并讨论其与学习结果、参与者情绪和培训课程评价等不同变量的相关性。因此,有人提出了以下问题:哪种抱怨功能更有利于学习材料的过程,并能改善参与者的情绪,以及如何在回应层面上评估讲师,这取决于在培训期间激活了哪种抱怨的功能。为了回答这些问题,作者设计了一个实验,在这个实验中,一个组织的成员被诱导抱怨,发挥工具功能或宣泄功能。结果表明,最有效的策略是将其对象作为问题解决的出发点。
{"title":"Complaining during closed training: its functions and consequences","authors":"Marta Damroka Kaliszewska, D. Godlewska-Werner","doi":"10.24425/ppb.2020.133772","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.24425/ppb.2020.133772","url":null,"abstract":": During occupational trainings given to Polish employees, one can quite often observe complaining. The instructor can use it for problem-solving or for purification. Thus, complaining plays an instrumental or cathartic function. This has consequences for the entire training process. The aim of the article is to present the phenomenon of complaining during training courses and to discuss its correlation with different variables such as learning results, participants’ mood and the evaluation of the training course. Questions were therefore posed about which function of complaining would be more conducive to the process of learning the material and result in an improvement of the participants’ mood, as well as how the instructor would be evaluated, at the response level, depending on which function of complaining is activated during the training. In order to answer these questions, the authors designed an experiment in which complaining was induced in members of an organization, performing either an instrumental function or a cathartic function. The results show that the most effective strategy is the use of its object as a point of departure for problem solution.","PeriodicalId":38657,"journal":{"name":"Polish Psychological Bulletin","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2023-07-19","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"47559631","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Polish Psychological Bulletin
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1