首页 > 最新文献

Journal of Creative Behavior最新文献

英文 中文
Association Between the Creative Experience of Haiku Poetry and a Tendency Toward Self-Transcendent Emotions 俳句诗创作经验与自我超越情感倾向之间的联系
IF 2.8 2区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EDUCATIONAL Pub Date : 2024-05-22 DOI: 10.1002/jocb.657
Juri Kato, Jimpei Hitsuwari

Haiku is the world's shortest form of poetry, describing nature and ordinary everyday life. Previous studies and quotes from professional haiku poets suggest that haiku can foster self-transcendent emotions, such as gratitude and awe. This study compares how those who did and did not create at least one haiku in the past month experience self-transcendent emotions. A total of 192 haiku writers and 177 non-writers responded to scales related to self-transcendent emotions, such as gratitude for serenity, gratitude trait, and trait awe. The results of the Bayesian implementation of Generalized Linear Mixed models revealed that haiku writing increased the frequency of gratitude for serenity and awe, rather than general gratitude. These effects persisted even after controlling for interest in art and educational level, indicating that haiku writing has unique characteristics, including encouragement of attention to nature and a different perspective on daily life. Even in the absence of special events, a change in perspective toward everyday life occurs through creating haiku, and people appreciate and feel awe toward ordinary, everyday things. These novel findings contribute to the study of creativity and emotion.

俳句是世界上最短的诗歌形式,描述自然和普通的日常生活。以往的研究和专业俳句诗人的名言表明,俳句可以培养自我超越的情感,如感激和敬畏。本研究比较了那些在过去一个月中至少创作过一首俳句和没有创作过一首俳句的人是如何体验自我超越情绪的。共有 192 位俳句作者和 177 位非俳句作者回答了与自我超越情绪相关的量表,如对宁静的感激、感激特质和特质敬畏。贝叶斯广义线性混合模型的结果显示,俳句写作增加了对宁静和敬畏的感激之情,而不是一般的感激之情。即使在控制了对艺术的兴趣和教育水平之后,这些影响依然存在,这表明俳句写作具有独特的特点,包括鼓励关注自然和从不同的角度看待日常生活。即使在没有特殊事件的情况下,人们也会通过创作俳句来改变对日常生活的看法,并对平凡的日常事物产生欣赏和敬畏之情。这些新发现有助于对创造力和情感的研究。
{"title":"Association Between the Creative Experience of Haiku Poetry and a Tendency Toward Self-Transcendent Emotions","authors":"Juri Kato,&nbsp;Jimpei Hitsuwari","doi":"10.1002/jocb.657","DOIUrl":"10.1002/jocb.657","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Haiku is the world's shortest form of poetry, describing nature and ordinary everyday life. Previous studies and quotes from professional haiku poets suggest that haiku can foster self-transcendent emotions, such as gratitude and awe. This study compares how those who did and did not create at least one haiku in the past month experience self-transcendent emotions. A total of 192 haiku writers and 177 non-writers responded to scales related to self-transcendent emotions, such as gratitude for serenity, gratitude trait, and trait awe. The results of the Bayesian implementation of Generalized Linear Mixed models revealed that haiku writing increased the frequency of gratitude for serenity and awe, rather than general gratitude. These effects persisted even after controlling for interest in art and educational level, indicating that haiku writing has unique characteristics, including encouragement of attention to nature and a different perspective on daily life. Even in the absence of special events, a change in perspective toward everyday life occurs through creating haiku, and people appreciate and feel awe toward ordinary, everyday things. These novel findings contribute to the study of creativity and emotion.</p>","PeriodicalId":39915,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Creative Behavior","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.8,"publicationDate":"2024-05-22","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1002/jocb.657","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141110405","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Unveiling the Associative Mechanisms Underlying the Additive Bias: Using an Implicit Association Test to Gain Insight into People's Preference for Additive Actions 揭示加法偏差的联想机制:利用内隐联想测试深入了解人们对加法行动的偏好
IF 2.8 2区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EDUCATIONAL Pub Date : 2024-05-20 DOI: 10.1002/jocb.660
Maria Adriana Neroni, Nathan Crilly, Maria Antonella Brandimonte

When faced with the need to transform an object, idea, or situation, people have a tendency to favor adding new components rather than removing existing ones. This is called the additive bias. Previous research, along with historical and anecdotal examples, shows that this bias may significantly reduce problem-solving abilities and have a detrimental impact on the innovation process. In this study, our objective was to develop a novel tool, the additive bias implicit association test (ad-IAT), to investigate the reasons underlying people's preference for additive actions. By using this tool, we empirically demonstrated that people displayed an inherent tendency to assign a positive valence to additive concepts and to perceive additive actions as safer and more functional than subtractive concepts. Importantly, we also found that implicit preference for addition resulted in participants favoring additive actions while neglecting subtractive alternatives when engaged in a problem-solving task. Collectively, our series of experiments substantiated the effectiveness of our ad-IAT in uncovering and quantifying the additive bias. This, in turn, provided a deeper comprehension of the underlying factors contributing to the bias and its impact on people's behavior.

当需要改造一个物体、想法或情况时,人们往往倾向于添加新的成分,而不是去除现有的成分。这就是所谓的添加偏差。以往的研究以及历史和轶事实例表明,这种偏见可能会大大降低解决问题的能力,并对创新过程产生不利影响。在本研究中,我们的目标是开发一种新工具--加法偏差内隐联想测试(ad-IAT),以调查人们偏好加法行为的原因。通过使用这一工具,我们通过经验证明,人们天生倾向于对加法概念赋予积极的价值,并认为加法行为比减法概念更安全、更实用。重要的是,我们还发现,在参与问题解决任务时,对加法的内隐偏好会导致参与者偏爱加法行动,而忽视减法行动。总之,我们的一系列实验证实了我们的 ad-IAT 在发现和量化加法偏差方面的有效性。这反过来又让我们更深入地了解了导致偏见的潜在因素及其对人们行为的影响。
{"title":"Unveiling the Associative Mechanisms Underlying the Additive Bias: Using an Implicit Association Test to Gain Insight into People's Preference for Additive Actions","authors":"Maria Adriana Neroni,&nbsp;Nathan Crilly,&nbsp;Maria Antonella Brandimonte","doi":"10.1002/jocb.660","DOIUrl":"10.1002/jocb.660","url":null,"abstract":"<div>\u0000 \u0000 <p>When faced with the need to transform an object, idea, or situation, people have a tendency to favor adding new components rather than removing existing ones. This is called the <i>additive bias</i>. Previous research, along with historical and anecdotal examples, shows that this bias may significantly reduce problem-solving abilities and have a detrimental impact on the innovation process. In this study, our objective was to develop a novel tool, the additive bias implicit association test (ad-IAT), to investigate the reasons underlying people's preference for additive actions. By using this tool, we empirically demonstrated that people displayed an inherent tendency to assign a positive valence to additive concepts and to perceive additive actions as safer and more functional than subtractive concepts. Importantly, we also found that implicit preference for addition resulted in participants favoring additive actions while neglecting subtractive alternatives when engaged in a problem-solving task. Collectively, our series of experiments substantiated the effectiveness of our ad-IAT in uncovering and quantifying the additive bias. This, in turn, provided a deeper comprehension of the underlying factors contributing to the bias and its impact on people's behavior.</p>\u0000 </div>","PeriodicalId":39915,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Creative Behavior","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.8,"publicationDate":"2024-05-20","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141121673","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
How Much Retrieval Ability Is in Originality? 原创性的检索能力有多强?
IF 2.8 2区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EDUCATIONAL Pub Date : 2024-05-17 DOI: 10.1002/jocb.659
Selina Weiss, Benjamin Goecke, Oliver Wilhelm

Creative fluency and originality are pivotal indicators of creative potential. Both have been embedded in hierarchical intelligence models as part of the ability to retrieve information from long-term memory; an ability that is often measured with indicators of retrieval fluency. Creative fluency and retrieval fluency, both expressed by the count of correct responses, are procedurally highly similar. This raises the question how creative fluency and originality are related with retrieval fluency and how both are predicted by other cognitive abilities. In a multivariate study (N = 320), we found that retrieval fluency is very strongly related with creative fluency (r = .87) and substantially related with originality (r = .59). A combined fluency factor still fitted the data well. Cognitive abilities accounted for 63% variance in fluency and 47% variance in originality. After controlling for established cognitive abilities, latent variables for fluency and originality were unrelated with one another. This suggests that the procedural proximity of the ability to fluently generate either information from long-term memory or ad-hoc solutions to unusual tasks and the ability to come up with original ideas needs reconsideration. Locating originality below an overarching retrieval factor is contradicted by the present data.

创造性流畅性和独创性是衡量创造潜力的关键指标。这两种能力都被作为从长期记忆中检索信息的能力的一部分纳入了等级智力模型;这种能力通常用检索流畅性指标来衡量。创造性流畅性和检索流畅性都以正确回答的次数来表示,两者在程序上高度相似。这就提出了一个问题:创造性流畅性和独创性与检索流畅性之间的关系如何,以及其他认知能力如何预测这两种能力。在一项多变量研究(N = 320)中,我们发现检索流畅性与创造流畅性的关系非常密切(r = .87),与独创性的关系也非常密切(r = .59)。综合流畅性因子仍能很好地匹配数据。认知能力占流畅性方差的 63%,占独创性方差的 47%。在控制了既有的认知能力后,流畅性和独创性的潜变量互不相关。这表明,从长期记忆中流畅地生成信息或临时解决异常任务的能力与提出独创性想法的能力在程序上的近似性需要重新考虑。将独创性置于总体检索因素之下的观点与目前的数据相矛盾。
{"title":"How Much Retrieval Ability Is in Originality?","authors":"Selina Weiss,&nbsp;Benjamin Goecke,&nbsp;Oliver Wilhelm","doi":"10.1002/jocb.659","DOIUrl":"10.1002/jocb.659","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Creative fluency and originality are pivotal indicators of creative potential. Both have been embedded in hierarchical intelligence models as part of the ability to retrieve information from long-term memory; an ability that is often measured with indicators of retrieval fluency. Creative fluency and retrieval fluency, both expressed by the count of correct responses, are procedurally highly similar. This raises the question how creative fluency and originality are related with retrieval fluency and how both are predicted by other cognitive abilities. In a multivariate study (<i>N</i> = 320), we found that retrieval fluency is very strongly related with creative fluency (<i>r</i> = .87) and substantially related with originality (<i>r</i> = .59). A combined fluency factor still fitted the data well. Cognitive abilities accounted for 63% variance in fluency and 47% variance in originality. After controlling for established cognitive abilities, latent variables for fluency and originality were unrelated with one another. This suggests that the procedural proximity of the ability to fluently generate either information from long-term memory or ad-hoc solutions to unusual tasks and the ability to come up with original ideas needs reconsideration. Locating originality below an overarching retrieval factor is contradicted by the present data.</p>","PeriodicalId":39915,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Creative Behavior","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.8,"publicationDate":"2024-05-17","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1002/jocb.659","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140964408","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Automated Scoring of Scientific Creativity in German 德语科学创造力自动评分系统
IF 2.8 2区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EDUCATIONAL Pub Date : 2024-05-15 DOI: 10.1002/jocb.658
Benjamin Goecke, Paul V. DiStefano, Wolfgang Aschauer, Kurt Haim, Roger Beaty, Boris Forthmann

Automated scoring is a current hot topic in creativity research. However, most research has focused on the English language and popular verbal creative thinking tasks, such as the alternate uses task. Therefore, in this study, we present a large language model approach for automated scoring of a scientific creative thinking task that assesses divergent ideation in experimental tasks in the German language. Participants are required to generate alternative explanations for an empirical observation. This work analyzed a total of 13,423 unique responses. To predict human ratings of originality, we used XLM-RoBERTa (Cross-lingual Language Model-RoBERTa), a large, multilingual model. The prediction model was trained on 9,400 responses. Results showed a strong correlation between model predictions and human ratings in a held-out test set (n = 2,682; r = 0.80; CI-95% [0.79, 0.81]). These promising findings underscore the potential of large language models for automated scoring of scientific creative thinking in the German language. We encourage researchers to further investigate automated scoring of other domain-specific creative thinking tasks.

自动评分是当前创造力研究的热门话题。然而,大多数研究都集中在英语语言和流行的口头创造性思维任务上,如交替使用任务。因此,在本研究中,我们提出了一种大语言模型方法,用于在德语实验任务中评估发散性构思的科学创造性思维任务的自动评分。该任务要求参与者对经验观察结果做出替代性解释。这项工作共分析了 13,423 个独特的回答。为了预测人类对原创性的评分,我们使用了一个大型多语言模型 XLM-RoBERTa(跨语言语言模型-RoBERTa)。预测模型在 9,400 条回复上进行了训练。结果表明,在保留测试集中,模型预测与人类评分之间存在很强的相关性(n = 2,682; r = 0.80; CI-95% [0.79, 0.81])。这些令人鼓舞的发现强调了大型语言模型在德语科学创造性思维自动评分方面的潜力。我们鼓励研究人员进一步研究其他特定领域创造性思维任务的自动评分。
{"title":"Automated Scoring of Scientific Creativity in German","authors":"Benjamin Goecke,&nbsp;Paul V. DiStefano,&nbsp;Wolfgang Aschauer,&nbsp;Kurt Haim,&nbsp;Roger Beaty,&nbsp;Boris Forthmann","doi":"10.1002/jocb.658","DOIUrl":"10.1002/jocb.658","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Automated scoring is a current hot topic in creativity research. However, most research has focused on the English language and popular verbal creative thinking tasks, such as the alternate uses task. Therefore, in this study, we present a large language model approach for automated scoring of a scientific creative thinking task that assesses divergent ideation in experimental tasks in the German language. Participants are required to generate alternative explanations for an empirical observation. This work analyzed a total of 13,423 unique responses. To predict human ratings of originality, we used XLM-RoBERTa (Cross-lingual Language Model-RoBERTa), a large, multilingual model. The prediction model was trained on 9,400 responses. Results showed a strong correlation between model predictions and human ratings in a held-out test set (<i>n</i> = 2,682; <i>r</i> = 0.80; CI-95% [0.79, 0.81]). These promising findings underscore the potential of large language models for automated scoring of scientific creative thinking in the German language. We encourage researchers to further investigate automated scoring of other domain-specific creative thinking tasks.</p>","PeriodicalId":39915,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Creative Behavior","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.8,"publicationDate":"2024-05-15","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1002/jocb.658","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140976304","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Is Creativity Expressed at Home Related to Creativity Expressed at School? A Re-Examination of the Creativity Gap with Gifted and Nongifted Students 在家中表现出的创造力与在学校表现出的创造力有关吗?重新审视资优和非资优学生的创造力差距
IF 2.8 2区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EDUCATIONAL Pub Date : 2024-05-10 DOI: 10.1002/jocb.656
Ahmed M. Abdulla Alabbasi, Mark A. Runco, Maha S. Almutairi, Alaa Eldin A. Ayoub

Previous research suggests that environment can play an important role in encouraging or discouraging creative expression and productivity. Additional research has uncovered a discrepancy between the creativity students express at school and the creativity they express outside of school. The fact that, in previous research, students expressed more creativity outside of school than when in school implies that school discourages creativity. So far, the creativity gap has only been studied with nongifted students. One objective of the present investigation was to check for a similar discrepancy among gifted learners. Four hundred and eighty-seven middle school and high school students from the State of Kuwait (240 gifted and 247 nongifted) were recruited. The Creativity Activities and Accomplishment Checklist (CAAC) was administered to compare students' creative activities at home and school. The primary finding of this investigation was that the strength of the relationship between creativity at home and creativity at school differed in gifted and nongifted students. What was called a creativity gap existed in both groups, but it was smaller in the gifted group. A second finding was that gifted students expressed more creativity at school compared with the nongifted group, in particular CAAC domains. There were no differences between the gifted and the nongifted groups in the creativity expressed at home, except for one subscale, namely everyday creativity. Although gifted students expressed more creativity at school, compared with their nongifted peers, they were nonetheless more creative at home compared with school. Finally, hierarchical regression analyses indicated that measure of personality significantly moderated the relationship between creativity at home and creativity at school. Limitations and future directions are discussed.

以往的研究表明,环境在鼓励或抑制创造性表达和生产力方面起着重要作用。其他研究发现,学生在校内表达的创造力与在校外表达的创造力之间存在差异。在以往的研究中,学生在校外比在校内表现出更多的创造力,这意味着学校不鼓励创造力。迄今为止,只对非资优学生的创造力差距进行过研究。本次调查的目的之一是检查资优学生中是否存在类似的差异。我们招募了来自科威特国的 487 名初中和高中学生(240 名资优生和 247 名非资优生)。调查采用了创造性活动和成就检查表(CAAC),以比较学生在家里和学校的创造性活动。这项调查的主要发现是,资优学生和非资优学生在家庭创造力和学校创造力之间的关系强度不同。两个群体都存在所谓的创造力差距,但资优群体的差距较小。第二个发现是,与非资优组相比,资优学生在学校表现出更多的创造力,尤其是在CAAC领域。资优组和非资优组的学生在家中表现出的创造力没有差异,只有一个分量表除外,即日常创造力。虽然资优学生与非资优学生相比,在学校表现出更多的创造力,但与学校相比,他们在家里表现出更多的创造力。最后,分层回归分析表明,人格测量在很大程度上调节了家庭创造力和学校创造力之间的关系。本文讨论了研究的局限性和未来发展方向。
{"title":"Is Creativity Expressed at Home Related to Creativity Expressed at School? A Re-Examination of the Creativity Gap with Gifted and Nongifted Students","authors":"Ahmed M. Abdulla Alabbasi,&nbsp;Mark A. Runco,&nbsp;Maha S. Almutairi,&nbsp;Alaa Eldin A. Ayoub","doi":"10.1002/jocb.656","DOIUrl":"10.1002/jocb.656","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Previous research suggests that environment can play an important role in encouraging or discouraging creative expression and productivity. Additional research has uncovered a discrepancy between the creativity students express at school and the creativity they express outside of school. The fact that, in previous research, students expressed more creativity outside of school than when in school implies that school discourages creativity. So far, the creativity gap has only been studied with nongifted students. One objective of the present investigation was to check for a similar discrepancy among gifted learners. Four hundred and eighty-seven middle school and high school students from the State of Kuwait (240 gifted and 247 nongifted) were recruited. The <i>Creativity Activities and Accomplishment Checklist</i> (CAAC) was administered to compare students' creative activities at home and school. The primary finding of this investigation was that the strength of the relationship between creativity at home and creativity at school differed in gifted and nongifted students. What was called a creativity <i>gap</i> existed in both groups, but it was smaller in the gifted group. A second finding was that gifted students expressed more creativity at school compared with the nongifted group, in particular CAAC domains. There were no differences between the gifted and the nongifted groups in the creativity expressed at home, except for one subscale, namely everyday creativity. Although gifted students expressed more creativity at school, compared with their nongifted peers, they were nonetheless more creative at home compared with school. Finally, hierarchical regression analyses indicated that measure of personality significantly moderated the relationship between creativity at home and creativity at school. Limitations and future directions are discussed.</p>","PeriodicalId":39915,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Creative Behavior","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.8,"publicationDate":"2024-05-10","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1002/jocb.656","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140929030","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Performance and Perception of Creativity and Academic Achievement in Elementary School Students: A Normal Mixture Modeling Study 小学生创造力和学业成绩的表现与感知:正态混合模型研究
IF 3.9 2区 心理学 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2024-05-06 DOI: 10.1002/jocb.646
Alisa Scherbakova, Denis Dumas, Selcuk Acar, Kelly Berthiaume, Peter Organisciak

Creativity can be assessed using various methods, including divergent thinking performance, self-ratings, and teacher ratings. However, these measures may not always align, as they may not consistently identify creative potential in the same manner. The present study aimed to identify latent subgroups of students based on their observed originality, creative self-efficacy, teacher-rated originality, academic achievement in reading and mathematics, and demographic background characteristics. Data were collected from 243 elementary school students. We applied the normal mixture technique to classify participants into latent subgroups. Five latent subgroups of students were identified: Overconfident Low Performers, Creative High Achievers, Under-Confident Below-Average Achievers, Mathematically Oriented Students, and Calibrated Above-Average Achievers. Female students tended to fall disproportionately into the subgroup of Creative High Achievers. Students receiving free/reduced lunch had a lower probability of being Creative High Achievers. Special education students had a higher probability of falling into the subgroup Overconfident Low Performers. Teacher ratings of students' originality were more in line with student academic performance rather than with their performance-based originality scores. Students' self-ratings of creativity bifurcated across subgroups, with Creative High Achievers and Overconfident Low Performers reporting the highest self-ratings of originality, despite displaying very different levels of performance on the divergent thinking assessment.

创造力可以通过各种方法进行评估,包括发散思维表现、自我评分和教师评分。然而,这些测量方法可能并不总是一致的,因为它们可能无法以相同的方式持续识别创造力潜能。本研究旨在根据观察到的独创性、创造性自我效能感、教师评定的独创性、阅读和数学学业成绩以及人口背景特征,确定潜在的学生亚群。我们收集了 243 名小学生的数据。我们采用正态混合技术将参与者划分为潜在亚组。我们确定了五个潜在的学生亚群:过度自信的低成绩学生、有创造力的高成绩学生、自信不足的低于平均水平的成绩学生、数学导向型学生和校准型的高于平均水平的成绩学生。女生在 "创新型高分学生 "这一子组中的比例偏高。享受免费/减免午餐的学生成为创新高成就者的概率较低。接受特殊教育的学生成为 "过于自信的低成就者 "亚群的概率较高。教师对学生独创性的评价与学生的学业成绩更为一致,而不是与他们基于成绩的独创性分数更为一致。学生对创造力的自我评价在不同的亚组之间出现了分叉,创造力强的学生和过于自信的低成绩学生对原创性的自我评价最高,尽管他们在发散思维评估中的表现水平非常不同。
{"title":"Performance and Perception of Creativity and Academic Achievement in Elementary School Students: A Normal Mixture Modeling Study","authors":"Alisa Scherbakova,&nbsp;Denis Dumas,&nbsp;Selcuk Acar,&nbsp;Kelly Berthiaume,&nbsp;Peter Organisciak","doi":"10.1002/jocb.646","DOIUrl":"10.1002/jocb.646","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Creativity can be assessed using various methods, including divergent thinking performance, self-ratings, and teacher ratings. However, these measures may not always align, as they may not consistently identify creative potential in the same manner. The present study aimed to identify latent subgroups of students based on their observed originality, creative self-efficacy, teacher-rated originality, academic achievement in reading and mathematics, and demographic background characteristics. Data were collected from 243 elementary school students. We applied the normal mixture technique to classify participants into latent subgroups. Five latent subgroups of students were identified: Overconfident Low Performers, Creative High Achievers, Under-Confident Below-Average Achievers, Mathematically Oriented Students, and Calibrated Above-Average Achievers. Female students tended to fall disproportionately into the subgroup of Creative High Achievers. Students receiving free/reduced lunch had a lower probability of being Creative High Achievers. Special education students had a higher probability of falling into the subgroup Overconfident Low Performers. Teacher ratings of students' originality were more in line with student academic performance rather than with their performance-based originality scores. Students' self-ratings of creativity bifurcated across subgroups, with Creative High Achievers and Overconfident Low Performers reporting the highest self-ratings of originality, despite displaying very different levels of performance on the divergent thinking assessment.</p>","PeriodicalId":39915,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Creative Behavior","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.9,"publicationDate":"2024-05-06","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1002/jocb.646","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140929032","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Contributions of Metacognition to Creative Performance and Behavior 元认知对创造性表现和行为的贡献
IF 3.9 2区 心理学 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2024-05-01 DOI: 10.1002/jocb.652
Izabela Lebuda, Mathias Benedek
How are ideas born? Contrary to commonly held beliefs, creative performance, like any goal‐oriented action, requires understanding and managing one's own cognitive processes – thus, efficient metacognition. Recently, a systematic framework of creative metacognition (CMC) has been proposed, assuming the relevance of metacognitive knowledge, monitoring, and control in creative performance. Here, we provide the first comprehensive empirical examination of this conception. Specifically, an online sample (N = 425) performed divergent thinking (DT) tasks and gave insight in relevant aspects of metacognitive processes during task performance. The study revealed that all three proposed components of CMC played independent roles in enhancing creative cognitive performance, including divergent thinking creativity and fluency. Among these components, metacognitive control showed the strongest positive association with creative cognitive performance. As expected, CMC was especially relevant to imminent creative task performance but showed some association with real‐life creativity. These findings provide the first empirical evidence that all three postulated components of CMC support creative performance and, to a lesser degree, to creative behavior. In the discussion, we delve deeper into the specific roles of these metacognitive subcomponents in enhancing creative cognitive performance and touch upon the differences between the roles of self‐regulation and metacognition in creativity.
创意是如何诞生的?与通常的看法相反,创意表现与任何以目标为导向的行动一样,都需要了解和管理自己的认知过程,因此需要高效的元认知。最近,有人提出了创意元认知(CMC)的系统框架,认为元认知知识、监测和控制与创意表现息息相关。在此,我们首次对这一概念进行了全面的实证研究。具体来说,在线样本(N = 425)执行了发散思维(DT)任务,并对任务执行过程中元认知过程的相关方面进行了深入了解。研究结果表明,CMC 的三个拟议组成部分在提高创造性认知表现(包括发散思维的创造性和流畅性)方面都发挥了独立作用。其中,元认知控制与创造性认知表现的正相关性最强。正如预期的那样,元认知控制与即将到来的创造性任务表现尤其相关,但与现实生活中的创造性也有一定关联。这些研究结果首次提供了实证证据,证明 CMC 的所有三个假设成分都支持创造性表现,并在较小程度上支持创造性行为。在讨论中,我们深入探讨了这些元认知子成分在提高创造性认知表现方面的具体作用,并探讨了自我调节和元认知在创造性方面的作用差异。
{"title":"Contributions of Metacognition to Creative Performance and Behavior","authors":"Izabela Lebuda, Mathias Benedek","doi":"10.1002/jocb.652","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1002/jocb.652","url":null,"abstract":"How are ideas born? Contrary to commonly held beliefs, creative performance, like any goal‐oriented action, requires understanding and managing one's own cognitive processes – thus, efficient metacognition. Recently, a systematic framework of creative metacognition (CMC) has been proposed, assuming the relevance of metacognitive knowledge, monitoring, and control in creative performance. Here, we provide the first comprehensive empirical examination of this conception. Specifically, an online sample (<jats:italic>N</jats:italic> = 425) performed divergent thinking (DT) tasks and gave insight in relevant aspects of metacognitive processes during task performance. The study revealed that all three proposed components of CMC played independent roles in enhancing creative cognitive performance, including divergent thinking creativity and fluency. Among these components, metacognitive control showed the strongest positive association with creative cognitive performance. As expected, CMC was especially relevant to imminent creative task performance but showed some association with real‐life creativity. These findings provide the first empirical evidence that all three postulated components of CMC support creative performance and, to a lesser degree, to creative behavior. In the discussion, we delve deeper into the specific roles of these metacognitive subcomponents in enhancing creative cognitive performance and touch upon the differences between the roles of self‐regulation and metacognition in creativity.","PeriodicalId":39915,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Creative Behavior","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.9,"publicationDate":"2024-05-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140830426","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Reeling in Stories: An Investigation of Creative Behaviors and Creativity‐Support on Instagram 缫丝故事:Instagram上的创意行为和创意支持调查
IF 3.9 2区 心理学 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2024-05-01 DOI: 10.1002/jocb.653
Simon M. Ceh, Mathias Benedek
Creative behaviors are increasingly impacted by digital technologies, but little is known about the way digital technologies support everyday creativity and what factors predict their creative use. We investigated to what extent individual differences in person‐specific (creativity, personality) and platform‐specific (e.g., perceived creativity support) characteristics relate to the creative use of Instagram. The results from a sample of 191 Instagram users revealed that more frequent creative use of Instagram was related to greater engagement in everyday creative behaviors, creative self‐beliefs, the openness facet of creative imagination, extraversion, a more positive attitude toward Instagram, greater platform‐related self‐efficacy, and greater perceived creativity‐support. Regression analysis further revealed a unique contribution of everyday creativity, creative personal identity, extraversion, attitude toward Instagram, Instagram self‐efficacy, and perceived creativity‐support for creative use of Instagram. Although creativity was not the most central motive for using Instagram, the results from the present study indicated considerable levels of creative use, suggesting that common online spheres are important for everyday creative behavior. Together, this study identified relevant person‐ and platform‐specific factors predicting creative behavior at Instagram, while also highlighting the relevance of creativity in the digital world even outside of devoted digital creativity tools.
创意行为越来越多地受到数字技术的影响,但人们对数字技术如何支持日常创意以及哪些因素可以预测创意使用却知之甚少。我们研究了个人特异性(创造力、个性)和平台特异性(如感知到的创造力支持)特征的个体差异在多大程度上与 Instagram 的创造性使用有关。对191名Instagram用户的抽样调查结果显示,Instagram的创造性使用频率与日常创造性行为的参与度、创造性自我信念、创造性想象力的开放性、外向性、对Instagram更积极的态度、更高的平台相关自我效能感以及更高的感知创造性支持有关。回归分析进一步揭示了日常创造力、创造性个人身份、外向性、对 Instagram 的态度、Instagram 自我效能感和感知到的创造力支持对创造性使用 Instagram 的独特贡献。虽然创造力并不是使用 Instagram 的最主要动机,但本研究的结果表明,创造力的使用水平相当高,这表明常见的网络领域对日常创造性行为非常重要。总之,本研究确定了预测 Instagram 创意行为的相关个人和平台特定因素,同时也强调了即使在专用数字创意工具之外,数字世界中创意的相关性。
{"title":"Reeling in Stories: An Investigation of Creative Behaviors and Creativity‐Support on Instagram","authors":"Simon M. Ceh, Mathias Benedek","doi":"10.1002/jocb.653","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1002/jocb.653","url":null,"abstract":"Creative behaviors are increasingly impacted by digital technologies, but little is known about the way digital technologies support everyday creativity and what factors predict their creative use. We investigated to what extent individual differences in person‐specific (creativity, personality) and platform‐specific (e.g., perceived creativity support) characteristics relate to the creative use of Instagram. The results from a sample of 191 Instagram users revealed that more frequent creative use of Instagram was related to greater engagement in everyday creative behaviors, creative self‐beliefs, the openness facet of creative imagination, extraversion, a more positive attitude toward Instagram, greater platform‐related self‐efficacy, and greater perceived creativity‐support. Regression analysis further revealed a unique contribution of everyday creativity, creative personal identity, extraversion, attitude toward Instagram, Instagram self‐efficacy, and perceived creativity‐support for creative use of Instagram. Although creativity was not the most central motive for using Instagram, the results from the present study indicated considerable levels of creative use, suggesting that common online spheres are important for everyday creative behavior. Together, this study identified relevant person‐ and platform‐specific factors predicting creative behavior at Instagram, while also highlighting the relevance of creativity in the digital world even outside of devoted digital creativity tools.","PeriodicalId":39915,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Creative Behavior","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.9,"publicationDate":"2024-05-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140830181","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Embodied and Enactive Creativity: Moving Beyond the Mind–Body Dichotomy in School Education 体现式创造力和主动式创造力:超越学校教育中的身心二分法
IF 3.9 2区 心理学 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2024-04-27 DOI: 10.1002/jocb.651
Anne Bertin‐Renoux
This study explores the ways in which embodied creativity is conceived and implemented in french schools through the study of a corpus of professional articles published since the 1960s in a journal dedicated to physical education. The analysis focuses on pedagogical experiments to foster bodily creativity carried out in primary schools, as part of a wide‐ranging educational reform during the 1970s. Those practices mark a radical break with the grammar of schooling whose worldwide spread is linked to the colonial expansion of western Europe. They revealed many similarities with indigenous pedagogies through the willingness to go beyond the mind–body dichotomy, to value a sensitive and intuitive body, to anchor knowledge in lived experience and to move from a top‐down relationship between teacher and pupil to a more horizontal one. The 1980s marked a return to more traditional methods, but these pedagogical experiments nurtured a conception of embodied and enactive creativity that sought to go beyond a western vision of the body, of action and of the relationship with the world in school education. The convergences with non‐western pedagogies underline the interest of these approaches to explore and foster embodied and enactive creativity.
本研究通过对 20 世纪 60 年代以来发表在一份体育教育期刊上的专业文章的研究,探讨了法国学校如何构想和实施身体创造力。分析的重点是作为 20 世纪 70 年代广泛教育改革的一部分,在小学开展的培养身体创造力的教学实验。这些做法标志着与学校教育语法的彻底决裂,而学校教育语法在全世界的传播与西欧的殖民扩张有关。这些做法与本土教学法有许多相似之处,都愿意超越身心二分法,重视敏感和直觉的身体,将知识植根于生活经验之中,并将师生之间自上而下的关系转变为更加横向的关系。20 世纪 80 年代,教学方法开始回归传统,但这些教学实验孕育了一种关于身体和主动创造力的概念,试图在学校教育中超越西方对身体、行动以及与世界关系的看法。与非西方教学法的交汇凸显了这些方法对探索和培养具身和主动创造力的兴趣。
{"title":"Embodied and Enactive Creativity: Moving Beyond the Mind–Body Dichotomy in School Education","authors":"Anne Bertin‐Renoux","doi":"10.1002/jocb.651","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1002/jocb.651","url":null,"abstract":"This study explores the ways in which embodied creativity is conceived and implemented in french schools through the study of a corpus of professional articles published since the 1960s in a journal dedicated to physical education. The analysis focuses on pedagogical experiments to foster bodily creativity carried out in primary schools, as part of a wide‐ranging educational reform during the 1970s. Those practices mark a radical break with the grammar of schooling whose worldwide spread is linked to the colonial expansion of western Europe. They revealed many similarities with indigenous pedagogies through the willingness to go beyond the mind–body dichotomy, to value a sensitive and intuitive body, to anchor knowledge in lived experience and to move from a top‐down relationship between teacher and pupil to a more horizontal one. The 1980s marked a return to more traditional methods, but these pedagogical experiments nurtured a conception of embodied and enactive creativity that sought to go beyond a western vision of the body, of action and of the relationship with the world in school education. The convergences with non‐western pedagogies underline the interest of these approaches to explore and foster embodied and enactive creativity.","PeriodicalId":39915,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Creative Behavior","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.9,"publicationDate":"2024-04-27","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140809939","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Are Children with Autistic Traits More or Less Creative? Links between Autistic Traits and Creative Attributes in Children 具有自闭症特征的儿童创造力更强还是更弱?自闭症特质与儿童创造力之间的联系
IF 2.8 2区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EDUCATIONAL Pub Date : 2024-04-23 DOI: 10.1002/jocb.650
Rebecca Smees, Julia Simner, Louisa J. Rinaldi

Autistic traits are found throughout the general population, but their link to creative attributes has received little attention in childhood populations. In adults, autistic traits are linked to both creative benefits and disadvantages, moderated by the autistic trait and the creative domain under investigation. The current study investigates the link between autistic traits and creative attributes (creative personality traits, creative talent, creative artistic choices) in children aged 10–14 years. Autistic traits were measured using the Adolescent-AQ, both globally (AQ-Total) and for individual subscales (AQ-Attention to detail, AQ-Imagination, and “AQ-Core”, i.e., combining AQ-Social skills, AQ-Attention switching, AQ-Communication). Using child and parent reports, data from 149 children revealed an association between autistic traits and creative personality traits (both positive and negative) while also showing a (weaker) relationship with creative artistic choices. Global and core autistic symptoms negatively predicted creative personality traits. At the same time, AQ-Imagination predicted lower creative attributes across nearly all creative domains. Finally, and in contrast, AQ-Attention to detail positively predicted a number of creative attributes (i.e., creative personality traits, creative talent). Our results show how autistic traits map to a range of creative attributes, across children in the general population.

自闭症特征在普通人群中随处可见,但在儿童群体中,自闭症特征与创造力属性之间的联系却很少受到关注。在成人中,自闭症特质与创造力的益处和弊端都有联系,但受自闭症特质和所调查的创造力领域的调节。本研究调查了 10-14 岁儿童的自闭症特质与创造性属性(创造性人格特质、创造性才能、创造性艺术选择)之间的联系。自闭症特质采用青少年自闭症量表(Adolescent-AQ)进行测量,包括总体量表(AQ-Total)和单个分量表(AQ-Attention to detail、AQ-Imagination 和 "AQ-Core",即 AQ-Social skills、AQ-Attention switching 和 AQ-Communication)。通过儿童和家长的报告,149 名儿童的数据显示自闭症特征与创造性人格特征(包括积极和消极)之间存在关联,同时也显示出与创造性艺术选择之间的关系(较弱)。全面自闭症症状和核心自闭症症状对创造性人格特质有负面预测作用。同时,AQ-Imagination 预测了几乎所有创意领域中较低的创意属性。最后,与之形成鲜明对比的是,自闭症智力水平--对细节的关注对一些创造性特质(即创造性人格特质、创造性天赋)具有积极的预测作用。我们的研究结果表明,自闭症特质是如何在普通儿童中映射出一系列创造性特质的。
{"title":"Are Children with Autistic Traits More or Less Creative? Links between Autistic Traits and Creative Attributes in Children","authors":"Rebecca Smees,&nbsp;Julia Simner,&nbsp;Louisa J. Rinaldi","doi":"10.1002/jocb.650","DOIUrl":"10.1002/jocb.650","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Autistic traits are found throughout the general population, but their link to creative attributes has received little attention in childhood populations. In adults, autistic traits are linked to both creative benefits and disadvantages, moderated by the autistic trait and the creative domain under investigation. The current study investigates the link between autistic traits and creative attributes (creative personality traits, creative talent, creative artistic choices) in children aged 10–14 years. Autistic traits were measured using the Adolescent-AQ, both globally (AQ-Total) and for individual subscales (AQ-Attention to detail, AQ-Imagination, and “AQ-Core”, i.e., combining AQ-Social skills, AQ-Attention switching, AQ-Communication). Using child and parent reports, data from 149 children revealed an association between autistic traits and creative personality traits (both positive and negative) while also showing a (weaker) relationship with creative artistic choices. Global and core autistic symptoms negatively predicted creative personality traits. At the same time, AQ-Imagination predicted lower creative attributes across nearly all creative domains. Finally, and in contrast, AQ-Attention to detail positively predicted a number of creative attributes (i.e., creative personality traits, creative talent). Our results show how autistic traits map to a range of creative attributes, across children in the general population.</p>","PeriodicalId":39915,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Creative Behavior","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.8,"publicationDate":"2024-04-23","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1002/jocb.650","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140670251","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Journal of Creative Behavior
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1