首页 > 最新文献

Journal of Creative Behavior最新文献

英文 中文
Using Daily Creativity Planning to Promote Creative Behavior 使用每日创意计划促进创意行为
IF 3.9 2区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EDUCATIONAL Pub Date : 2022-08-22 DOI: 10.1002/jocb.563
Ting Liu, Pin Li, Skylor Zhitian Zhang, Wanlun Liu

Research has demonstrated that on the path from a creative idea to a creative outcome, high creativity motivation and self-efficacy do not necessarily lead to creative behavior. The present study proposed and examined the notion that daily creativity planning could promote creative behavior and contribute to the cultivation of creativity. A total of 77 middle school students (39 students in the experimental group and 38 in the control group) participated in this study, for which a quasi-experimental design was administered. The experimental group conducted a two-week daily planning for creative activities, while the control group did not conduct any intervention. The results showed that students' creativity motivation and creative self-efficacy were at relatively high levels overall and were positively and moderately correlated with creative behavior. Daily planning could effectively facilitate students' creative behavior. These findings point to a promising and simple creativity enhancement strategy for cultivating students to develop the habit of making creative plans in their daily lives.

研究表明,在从创意到创意成果的过程中,高创意动机和自我效能感并不一定会导致创意行为。本研究提出并检验了日常创意计划可以促进创意行为,有助于创造力的培养。本研究共选取77名中学生(试验组39名,对照组38名),采用准实验设计。实验组每天进行为期两周的创意活动计划,而对照组不进行任何干预。结果表明,学生的创造动机和创造自我效能感总体上处于较高水平,并与创造行为呈正、中相关。日常计划可以有效地促进学生的创造性行为。这些发现为培养学生养成在日常生活中制定创造性计划的习惯提供了一种有前途的、简单的创造力增强策略。
{"title":"Using Daily Creativity Planning to Promote Creative Behavior","authors":"Ting Liu,&nbsp;Pin Li,&nbsp;Skylor Zhitian Zhang,&nbsp;Wanlun Liu","doi":"10.1002/jocb.563","DOIUrl":"10.1002/jocb.563","url":null,"abstract":"<div>\u0000 \u0000 <p>Research has demonstrated that on the path from a creative idea to a creative outcome, high creativity motivation and self-efficacy do not necessarily lead to creative behavior. The present study proposed and examined the notion that daily creativity planning could promote creative behavior and contribute to the cultivation of creativity. A total of 77 middle school students (39 students in the experimental group and 38 in the control group) participated in this study, for which a quasi-experimental design was administered. The experimental group conducted a two-week daily planning for creative activities, while the control group did not conduct any intervention. The results showed that students' creativity motivation and creative self-efficacy were at relatively high levels overall and were positively and moderately correlated with creative behavior. Daily planning could effectively facilitate students' creative behavior. These findings point to a promising and simple creativity enhancement strategy for cultivating students to develop the habit of making creative plans in their daily lives.</p>\u0000 </div>","PeriodicalId":39915,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Creative Behavior","volume":"57 1","pages":"82-95"},"PeriodicalIF":3.9,"publicationDate":"2022-08-22","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"41516615","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
The Machines Take Over: A Comparison of Various Supervised Learning Approaches for Automated Scoring of Divergent Thinking Tasks 机器接管:用于发散思维任务自动评分的各种监督学习方法的比较
IF 3.9 2区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EDUCATIONAL Pub Date : 2022-08-08 DOI: 10.1002/jocb.559
Philip Buczak, He Huang, Boris Forthmann, Philipp Doebler

Traditionally, researchers employ human raters for scoring responses to creative thinking tasks. Apart from the associated costs this approach entails two potential risks. First, human raters can be subjective in their scoring behavior (inter-rater-variance). Second, individual raters are prone to inconsistent scoring patterns (intra-rater-variance). In light of these issues, we present an approach for automated scoring of Divergent Thinking (DT) Tasks. We implemented a pipeline aiming to generate accurate rating predictions for DT responses using text mining and machine learning methods. Based on two existing data sets from two different laboratories, we constructed several prediction models incorporating features representing meta information of the response or features engineered from the response’s word embeddings that were obtained using pre-trained GloVe and Word2Vec word vector spaces. Out of these features, word embeddings and features derived from them proved to be particularly effective. Overall, longer responses tended to achieve higher ratings as well as responses that were semantically distant from the stimulus object. In our comparison of three state-of-the-art machine learning algorithms, Random Forest and XGBoost tended to slightly outperform the Support Vector Regression.

传统上,研究人员雇用人类评分员对创造性思维任务的反应进行评分。除了相关成本之外,这种方法还会带来两个潜在风险。首先,人类评分者的评分行为可能是主观的(评分间方差)。其次,个别评分者倾向于不一致的评分模式(评分内方差)。鉴于这些问题,我们提出了一种自动评分发散思维(DT)任务的方法。我们实现了一个管道,旨在使用文本挖掘和机器学习方法为DT响应生成准确的评级预测。基于来自两个不同实验室的两个现有数据集,我们构建了几个预测模型,这些模型结合了代表响应元信息的特征或从响应的词嵌入中设计的特征,这些词嵌入是使用预训练的GloVe和Word2Vec词向量空间获得的。在这些特征中,词嵌入及其衍生的特征被证明是特别有效的。总的来说,较长的反应倾向于获得更高的评分,以及语义上远离刺激对象的反应。在我们对三种最先进的机器学习算法的比较中,随机森林和XGBoost倾向于略微优于支持向量回归。
{"title":"The Machines Take Over: A Comparison of Various Supervised Learning Approaches for Automated Scoring of Divergent Thinking Tasks","authors":"Philip Buczak,&nbsp;He Huang,&nbsp;Boris Forthmann,&nbsp;Philipp Doebler","doi":"10.1002/jocb.559","DOIUrl":"10.1002/jocb.559","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Traditionally, researchers employ human raters for scoring responses to creative thinking tasks. Apart from the associated costs this approach entails two potential risks. First, human raters can be subjective in their scoring behavior (inter-rater-variance). Second, individual raters are prone to inconsistent scoring patterns (intra-rater-variance). In light of these issues, we present an approach for automated scoring of Divergent Thinking (DT) Tasks. We implemented a pipeline aiming to generate accurate rating predictions for DT responses using text mining and machine learning methods. Based on two existing data sets from two different laboratories, we constructed several prediction models incorporating features representing meta information of the response or features engineered from the response’s word embeddings that were obtained using pre-trained GloVe and Word2Vec word vector spaces. Out of these features, word embeddings and features derived from them proved to be particularly effective. Overall, longer responses tended to achieve higher ratings as well as responses that were semantically distant from the stimulus object. In our comparison of three state-of-the-art machine learning algorithms, Random Forest and XGBoost tended to slightly outperform the Support Vector Regression.</p>","PeriodicalId":39915,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Creative Behavior","volume":"57 1","pages":"17-36"},"PeriodicalIF":3.9,"publicationDate":"2022-08-08","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1002/jocb.559","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"42243196","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 6
Are the Verbal TTCT Forms Actually Interchangeable? 口头TTCT形式真的可以互换吗?
IF 3.9 2区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EDUCATIONAL Pub Date : 2022-08-04 DOI: 10.1002/jocb.556
Katalin Grajzel, Denis Dumas, Selcuk Acar

One of the best-known and most frequently used measures of creative idea generation is the Torrance Test of Creative Thinking (TTCT). The TTCT Verbal, assessing verbal ideation, contains two forms created to be used interchangeably by researchers and practitioners. However, the parallel forms reliability of the two versions of the TTCT Verbal has not been examined for over 50 years. This study provides a long-needed evaluation of the parallel forms reliability of the TTCT Verbal by correlating publisher generated and text-mining-based scores across the forms. The relatively weak relationship between the two forms, ranging between .21 and .40 for the overall TTCT Verbal and ranging between .03 and .33 for the individual TTCT Verbal tasks, suggests that caution should be exercised when researchers and practitioners use the two forms as equivalent measures of verbal creative idea generation.

托兰斯创造性思维测试(TTCT)是最有名和最常用的创造性思维产生方法之一。TTCT口头测试,评估口头思维,包含两种形式,可供研究人员和实践者互换使用。然而,两个版本的TTCT口头测试的平行形式可靠性已经超过50年没有被研究过了。本研究通过关联出版商生成的表格和基于文本挖掘的表格得分,对TTCT口头测试的平行表格可靠性进行了长期需要的评估。两种形式之间的关系相对较弱,整体TTCT口头测试的关系范围在0.21到0.40之间,单个TTCT口头测试的关系范围在0.03到0.33之间,这表明当研究人员和从业者使用这两种形式作为口头创意产生的等效度量时,应该谨慎行事。
{"title":"Are the Verbal TTCT Forms Actually Interchangeable?","authors":"Katalin Grajzel,&nbsp;Denis Dumas,&nbsp;Selcuk Acar","doi":"10.1002/jocb.556","DOIUrl":"10.1002/jocb.556","url":null,"abstract":"<div>\u0000 \u0000 <p>One of the best-known and most frequently used measures of creative idea generation is the Torrance Test of Creative Thinking (TTCT). The TTCT Verbal, assessing verbal ideation, contains two forms created to be used interchangeably by researchers and practitioners. However, the parallel forms reliability of the two versions of the TTCT Verbal has not been examined for over 50 years. This study provides a long-needed evaluation of the parallel forms reliability of the TTCT Verbal by correlating publisher generated and text-mining-based scores across the forms. The relatively weak relationship between the two forms, ranging between .21 and .40 for the overall TTCT Verbal and ranging between .03 and .33 for the individual TTCT Verbal tasks, suggests that caution should be exercised when researchers and practitioners use the two forms as equivalent measures of verbal creative idea generation.</p>\u0000 </div>","PeriodicalId":39915,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Creative Behavior","volume":"56 4","pages":"629-646"},"PeriodicalIF":3.9,"publicationDate":"2022-08-04","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"41964669","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 3
Orientation Toward Intrinsic Motivation Mediates the Relationship Between Metacognition and Creativity 内在动机取向在元认知与创造力关系中的中介作用
IF 3.9 2区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EDUCATIONAL Pub Date : 2022-07-29 DOI: 10.1002/jocb.558
Marek Urban, Kamila Urban

Metacognition and motivation are considered key facets of self-regulation in various contexts. Recent studies identified a link between metacognition and creative performance, with metacognitively aware students performing more creatively and exhibiting higher levels of intrinsic and identified extrinsic motivation. The present study aims to examine the relationship between metacognition, orientation toward intrinsic or extrinsic motivation, and creative performance. One hundred nineteen university students completed the Metacognitive Awareness Inventory (MAI) and Scale of Intrinsic versus Extrinsic Orientation in the Classroom and performed four verbal creativity tasks (product improvement task, consequences task, and two unusual uses tasks). The partial correlation network showed that all the creativity tasks were uniquely related to at least one facet of metacognition, and that the most complex task (product improvement task) was linked to both metacognitive knowledge and regulation. Furthermore, the structural equation model indicated that orientation toward intrinsic motivation mediated the relationship between metacognition and creative performance, explaining 16% of the variance in creative performance.

元认知和动机被认为是各种情境下自我调节的关键方面。最近的研究发现了元认知和创造性表现之间的联系,具有元认知意识的学生表现得更有创造性,表现出更高水平的内在动机和确定的外在动机。本研究旨在探讨元认知、内在动机取向和外在动机取向与创造性表现之间的关系。191名大学生在课堂上完成了元认知意识量表(MAI)和内在取向与外在取向量表,并执行了四项言语创造力任务(产品改进任务、后果任务和两项不寻常用途任务)。部分相关网络表明,所有创造力任务至少与元认知的一个方面有独特的关系,最复杂的任务(产品改进任务)与元认知知识和调节都有联系。此外,结构方程模型表明,内在动机取向在元认知与创意绩效之间起中介作用,解释了16%的创意绩效差异。
{"title":"Orientation Toward Intrinsic Motivation Mediates the Relationship Between Metacognition and Creativity","authors":"Marek Urban,&nbsp;Kamila Urban","doi":"10.1002/jocb.558","DOIUrl":"10.1002/jocb.558","url":null,"abstract":"<div>\u0000 \u0000 <p>Metacognition and motivation are considered key facets of self-regulation in various contexts. Recent studies identified a link between metacognition and creative performance, with metacognitively aware students performing more creatively and exhibiting higher levels of intrinsic and identified extrinsic motivation. The present study aims to examine the relationship between metacognition, orientation toward intrinsic or extrinsic motivation, and creative performance. One hundred nineteen university students completed the Metacognitive Awareness Inventory (MAI) and Scale of Intrinsic versus Extrinsic Orientation in the Classroom and performed four verbal creativity tasks (product improvement task, consequences task, and two unusual uses tasks). The partial correlation network showed that all the creativity tasks were uniquely related to at least one facet of metacognition, and that the most complex task (product improvement task) was linked to both metacognitive knowledge and regulation. Furthermore, the structural equation model indicated that orientation toward intrinsic motivation mediated the relationship between metacognition and creative performance, explaining 16% of the variance in creative performance.</p>\u0000 </div>","PeriodicalId":39915,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Creative Behavior","volume":"57 1","pages":"6-16"},"PeriodicalIF":3.9,"publicationDate":"2022-07-29","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"43706957","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 5
Creative Self-Efficacy and Metacognitive Feelings as Sources of Information when Generating, Evaluating, and Selecting Creative Ideas: A Metacognitive Perspective 创造性自我效能感和元认知感受在产生、评估和选择创造性想法时的信息来源:一个元认知视角
IF 3.9 2区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EDUCATIONAL Pub Date : 2022-07-29 DOI: 10.1002/jocb.557
Rogelio Puente-Díaz, Judith Cavazos-Arroyo

In one study, we examined the relationships between pre-task creative self-efficacy, post-task creative confidence, self-rated and judges-rated creativity evaluations, metacognitive feelings, and idea selection under a metacognitive framework. Four hundred and fourteen college students were asked to complete a divergent thinking problem in the domain of marketing and completed several questionnaires measuring pre-task creative self-efficacy, post-task creative confidence, idea evaluation and selection, and metacognitive feelings. Independent judges evaluated the creativity of each idea generated and ranked ideas from the most to the least creative. Results showed positive relationships between pre-task creative self-efficacy and post-task creative confidence, self- and judges-rated creativity evaluations and metacognitive feelings. In addition, metacognitive feelings and self-rated creative evaluations predicted post-task confidence. We also observed evidence for positive connections between metacognitive feelings and self-rated evaluations and greater probability of choosing the first idea brought to mind as the best idea than what was expected by chance. The implications of the results were discussed.

在一项研究中,我们在元认知框架下考察了任务前创造性自我效能、任务后创造性自信、自我评价和评判评价、元认知感受和想法选择之间的关系。本研究要求414名大学生完成一个市场营销领域的发散性思维问题,并完成任务前创造性自我效能、任务后创造性自信、想法评价和选择以及元认知感受的问卷调查。独立评委评估每个创意的创意,并将创意从高到低进行排序。结果显示,任务前创造性自我效能感与任务后创造性自信、自我评价和评判评价以及元认知感受之间存在正相关关系。此外,元认知感受和自评创造性评价预测任务后自信。我们还观察到元认知感受和自我评价之间存在积极联系的证据,并且选择脑海中第一个想法作为最佳想法的可能性比随机预期的可能性更大。讨论了研究结果的意义。
{"title":"Creative Self-Efficacy and Metacognitive Feelings as Sources of Information when Generating, Evaluating, and Selecting Creative Ideas: A Metacognitive Perspective","authors":"Rogelio Puente-Díaz,&nbsp;Judith Cavazos-Arroyo","doi":"10.1002/jocb.557","DOIUrl":"10.1002/jocb.557","url":null,"abstract":"<div>\u0000 \u0000 <p>In one study, we examined the relationships between pre-task creative self-efficacy, post-task creative confidence, self-rated and judges-rated creativity evaluations, metacognitive feelings, and idea selection under a metacognitive framework. Four hundred and fourteen college students were asked to complete a divergent thinking problem in the domain of marketing and completed several questionnaires measuring pre-task creative self-efficacy, post-task creative confidence, idea evaluation and selection, and metacognitive feelings. Independent judges evaluated the creativity of each idea generated and ranked ideas from the most to the least creative. Results showed positive relationships between pre-task creative self-efficacy and post-task creative confidence, self- and judges-rated creativity evaluations and metacognitive feelings. In addition, metacognitive feelings and self-rated creative evaluations predicted post-task confidence. We also observed evidence for positive connections between metacognitive feelings and self-rated evaluations and greater probability of choosing the first idea brought to mind as the best idea than what was expected by chance. The implications of the results were discussed.</p>\u0000 </div>","PeriodicalId":39915,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Creative Behavior","volume":"56 4","pages":"647-658"},"PeriodicalIF":3.9,"publicationDate":"2022-07-29","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"44220270","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 4
What Factors Influence Children's Creative Artistic Orientation? The Novel Children's Creative Orientation Test: Artistic 影响儿童创造性艺术取向的因素是什么?小说儿童的创造性取向测试:艺术性
IF 3.9 2区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EDUCATIONAL Pub Date : 2022-07-01 DOI: 10.1002/jocb.555
Julia Simner, Rebecca Smees, Louisa J. Rinaldi, Duncan A. Carmichael, Toby J. McDonald

Creative orientation is the extent to which different individuals are drawn toward creative activities (e.g., art, music). We know relatively little about child-level creative orientation given certain testing limitations. Adult tools often measure time spent engaged in creative pursuits, but this method is unsuitable for children because their free time is often dictated by parents. To overcome this, we devised an entirely novel measure of creative orientation, the Children's Creative Orientation Test: Artistic (C-COT: Artistic). This short task, suitable for children as young as 6, elicits children's creative urges toward artistic pursuits independently of parental influence and provides quantitative scoring. We applied our measure to over 3000 children aged 6–10 years, where it showed robust reliability, suggesting that creative orientation is a stable trait over time. We show that creative orientation is also influenced by classroom cohort, age, and gender but is unaffected by socio-economic status or seasonal changes (autumn vs. spring testing). We showed too that creative orientation converges with creative thinking (divergent thinking), creative personality (openness to experiences, especially the aesthetics subtrait), and creative engagements in the home. We present our test here in full, as a simple, fast, and robust measure of creative artistic orientation in children.

创造性取向是指不同个体被创造性活动(如艺术、音乐)所吸引的程度。由于某些测试的限制,我们对儿童水平的创造性取向知之甚少。成人的工具经常测量花在创造性追求上的时间,但这种方法不适合孩子,因为他们的空闲时间通常是由父母决定的。为了克服这一点,我们设计了一种全新的创造性取向测量方法,儿童创造性取向测试:艺术(C-COT:艺术)。这个简短的任务适合6岁的孩子,它能激发孩子在不受父母影响的情况下对艺术追求的创造性冲动,并提供定量评分。我们将我们的测量方法应用于3000多名6-10岁的儿童,结果显示出了很强的可靠性,这表明创造性取向是一种长期稳定的特征。我们发现,创造性取向也受到班级队列、年龄和性别的影响,但不受社会经济地位或季节变化(秋季与春季测试)的影响。我们还表明,创造性取向与创造性思维(发散性思维)、创造性人格(对经验的开放性,尤其是美学的子特征)和家庭创造性参与是一致的。我们在这里完整地展示了我们的测试,作为一种简单、快速、可靠的衡量儿童创造性艺术取向的方法。
{"title":"What Factors Influence Children's Creative Artistic Orientation? The Novel Children's Creative Orientation Test: Artistic","authors":"Julia Simner,&nbsp;Rebecca Smees,&nbsp;Louisa J. Rinaldi,&nbsp;Duncan A. Carmichael,&nbsp;Toby J. McDonald","doi":"10.1002/jocb.555","DOIUrl":"10.1002/jocb.555","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Creative orientation is the extent to which different individuals are drawn toward creative activities (e.g., art, music). We know relatively little about child-level creative orientation given certain testing limitations. Adult tools often measure time spent engaged in creative pursuits, but this method is unsuitable for children because their free time is often dictated by parents. To overcome this, we devised an entirely novel measure of creative orientation, the <i>Children's Creative Orientation Test: Artistic (C-COT: Artistic</i>). This short task, suitable for children as young as 6, elicits children's creative urges toward artistic pursuits independently of parental influence and provides quantitative scoring. We applied our measure to over 3000 children aged 6–10 years, where it showed robust reliability, suggesting that creative orientation is a stable trait over time. We show that creative orientation is also influenced by classroom cohort, age, and gender but is unaffected by socio-economic status or seasonal changes (autumn vs. spring testing). We showed too that creative orientation converges with creative thinking (divergent thinking), creative personality (openness to experiences, especially the aesthetics subtrait), and creative engagements in the home. We present our test here in full, as a simple, fast, and robust measure of creative artistic orientation in children.</p>","PeriodicalId":39915,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Creative Behavior","volume":"56 4","pages":"609-628"},"PeriodicalIF":3.9,"publicationDate":"2022-07-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1002/jocb.555","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"47836766","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Synesthetes are More Involved in Art — Evidence From the Artistic Creativity Domains Compendium (ACDC) 通感者更多地参与艺术——来自艺术创造领域纲要(ACDC)的证据
IF 3.9 2区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EDUCATIONAL Pub Date : 2022-07-01 DOI: 10.1002/jocb.554
Katrin Lunke, Beat Meier

Creativity is a multidimensional, multistage, and time-dependent process, which can be expressed in various artistic domains and sub-domains (e.g., visual arts, literature, music, and performing arts). The present study investigated the involvement of synesthetes in art, and whether the type of synesthesia determines the preferred artistic domain. We tested 709 participants with either grapheme-color, sound-color, or sequence-space synesthesia (monotypical synesthesia) or a combination thereof (multiple synesthesia) and non-synesthete controls with the Artistic Creativity Domains Compendium (ACDC). The ACDC measures the involvement in art on the three levels “interest,” “ability,” and “performance” for the four domains “visual arts,” “literature,” “music,” and “performing arts.” Overall, the results showed that synesthetes have an affinity for all four artistic domains compared with non-synesthete controls. Moreover, the presence of multiple types of synesthesia affected the specific preference. Besides, compared with monotoypical synesthetes, the group of grapheme-color-sound-color-and-sequence-space synesthetes showed higher involvement in all artistic domains, most pronounced in visual arts. Overall, the study demonstrates that synesthesia is associated with higher interest, ability, and performance in art.

创造力是一个多维度、多阶段和时间依赖性的过程,它可以在不同的艺术领域和子领域(如视觉艺术、文学、音乐和表演艺术)中表达出来。本研究调查了通感者在艺术中的参与,以及是否通感的类型决定了首选的艺术领域。我们测试了709名参与者,他们有文字-颜色、声音-颜色或序列-空间联觉(单一性联觉)或它们的组合(多重联觉),并使用艺术创造力领域纲要(ACDC)进行非联觉控制。ACDC在“视觉艺术”、“文学”、“音乐”和“表演艺术”四个领域的“兴趣”、“能力”和“表现”三个层面上衡量对艺术的参与。总的来说,结果表明,与非联觉者相比,联觉者对所有四个艺术领域都有亲和力。此外,多种类型联觉的存在影响了特定偏好。此外,与单模联觉者相比,字母-颜色-声音-颜色-序列-空间联觉者在所有艺术领域都表现出更高的参与程度,在视觉艺术领域表现得最为明显。总的来说,研究表明,联觉与更高的兴趣、能力和艺术表现有关。
{"title":"Synesthetes are More Involved in Art — Evidence From the Artistic Creativity Domains Compendium (ACDC)","authors":"Katrin Lunke,&nbsp;Beat Meier","doi":"10.1002/jocb.554","DOIUrl":"10.1002/jocb.554","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Creativity is a multidimensional, multistage, and time-dependent process, which can be expressed in various artistic domains and sub-domains (e.g., visual arts, literature, music, and performing arts). The present study investigated the involvement of synesthetes in art, and whether the type of synesthesia determines the preferred artistic domain. We tested 709 participants with either grapheme-color, sound-color, or sequence-space synesthesia (monotypical synesthesia) or a combination thereof (multiple synesthesia) and non-synesthete controls with the Artistic Creativity Domains Compendium (ACDC). The ACDC measures the involvement in art on the three levels “interest,” “ability,” and “performance” for the four domains “visual arts,” “literature,” “music,” and “performing arts.” Overall, the results showed that synesthetes have an affinity for all four artistic domains compared with non-synesthete controls. Moreover, the presence of multiple types of synesthesia affected the specific preference. Besides, compared with monotoypical synesthetes, the group of grapheme-color-sound-color-and-sequence-space synesthetes showed higher involvement in all artistic domains, most pronounced in visual arts. Overall, the study demonstrates that synesthesia is associated with higher interest, ability, and performance in art.</p>","PeriodicalId":39915,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Creative Behavior","volume":"56 4","pages":"601-608"},"PeriodicalIF":3.9,"publicationDate":"2022-07-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1002/jocb.554","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"47186998","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Spontaneous or Deliberate: The Dual Influence of Mind Wandering on Creative Incubation 自发或刻意:思维漫游对创意孵化的双重影响
IF 3.9 2区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EDUCATIONAL Pub Date : 2022-06-19 DOI: 10.1002/jocb.553
Tianchen Yang, Guohong Wu

Two experiments examined the dual influence of mind wandering (MW) on the incubation of both deliberate and spontaneous modes of creativity. Specifically, using a modified version of Sustained Attention Response Task as the incubation task, this study assessed whether taking a break from a creative task and engaging in either an MW-allowed task or an MW-prevented task can exert differential effects on different aspects of creativity. Results showed that after engaging in an incubation task that allowed MW rather than prevented MW, participants generated ideas more flexibly but less persistently in the subsequent divergent thinking tasks, and were more likely to solve creative insight problems through intuitive insight but not systematic analysis. The results suggest that MW during incubation may simultaneously facilitate the spontaneous mode of creativity while suppressing the deliberate mode of creativity. These findings also indicate that creativity must be parsed into different subtypes in order to identify more specific ways to enhance creativity.

两个实验检验了走神(MW)对有意和自发创造力模式的孵化的双重影响。具体地说,本研究使用一个改进版本的持续注意反应任务作为孵化任务,评估了从创造性任务中休息一下,参与一个允许或不允许的任务,是否会对创造力的不同方面产生不同的影响。结果表明,在参与了一项允许而非阻止MW的孵化任务后,参与者在随后的发散思维任务中更灵活地产生想法,但持久性较差,并且更有可能通过直觉洞察力而不是系统分析来解决创造性洞察力问题。研究结果表明,在培养过程中,大脑活动可能在促进自发模式的同时抑制蓄意模式的创造力。这些发现还表明,创造力必须被分解成不同的亚型,以确定更具体的方式来提高创造力。
{"title":"Spontaneous or Deliberate: The Dual Influence of Mind Wandering on Creative Incubation","authors":"Tianchen Yang,&nbsp;Guohong Wu","doi":"10.1002/jocb.553","DOIUrl":"10.1002/jocb.553","url":null,"abstract":"<div>\u0000 \u0000 <p>Two experiments examined the dual influence of mind wandering (MW) on the incubation of both deliberate and spontaneous modes of creativity. Specifically, using a modified version of Sustained Attention Response Task as the incubation task, this study assessed whether taking a break from a creative task and engaging in either an MW-allowed task or an MW-prevented task can exert differential effects on different aspects of creativity. Results showed that after engaging in an incubation task that allowed MW rather than prevented MW, participants generated ideas more flexibly but less persistently in the subsequent divergent thinking tasks, and were more likely to solve creative insight problems through intuitive insight but not systematic analysis. The results suggest that MW during incubation may simultaneously facilitate the spontaneous mode of creativity while suppressing the deliberate mode of creativity. These findings also indicate that creativity must be parsed into different subtypes in order to identify more specific ways to enhance creativity.</p>\u0000 </div>","PeriodicalId":39915,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Creative Behavior","volume":"56 4","pages":"584-600"},"PeriodicalIF":3.9,"publicationDate":"2022-06-19","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"44666207","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 5
IF 3.9 2区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EDUCATIONAL Pub Date : 2022-06-16 DOI: 10.1002/jocb.550
Beth Miller
{"title":"","authors":"Beth Miller","doi":"10.1002/jocb.550","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1002/jocb.550","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":39915,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Creative Behavior","volume":"56 2","pages":"163"},"PeriodicalIF":3.9,"publicationDate":"2022-06-16","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"137687547","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The Sociomateriality of the Creative Process: Script Roles in Film Production 创作过程的社会物质性:电影制作中的剧本角色
IF 3.9 2区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EDUCATIONAL Pub Date : 2022-06-16 DOI: 10.1002/jocb.552
José Edemir da Silva Anjo, César Tureta

The article aims to identify and analyze the roles played by the script in the creative process of independent short film production. The theoretical framework is based on distributed creativity and sociomateriality. These approaches are complementary and helped us discuss how the script can shape the creativity of the subjects involved in the film production. We have followed the daily teamwork during the film production through participant observation, interviews, documents, photographs, and video. We identified three roles played by the script: (1) protagonist: centralizes creative ideas around its framing; (2) feature player: responsible for mediating creative actions, and (3) antagonist: hinders creative actions. Each of these roles directly influences the different stages of the creative process, which takes place through creativity distributed between people and artifacts. We conclude that artifacts have temporary roles in the creative process, and more than enhancing or limiting this process, they are an integral part of creativity. Therefore, humans and artifacts are entangled in the creative process and should not be analyzed separately.

本文旨在识别和分析剧本在独立短片创作过程中所扮演的角色。理论框架是基于分布式创造力和社会物质性。这些方法是互补的,并帮助我们讨论剧本如何塑造电影制作中涉及的主题的创造力。我们通过参与观察、访谈、文献、照片、视频等方式跟踪了电影制作过程中团队的日常工作。我们确定了剧本扮演的三个角色:(1)主角:围绕其框架集中创意;(2)特色玩家:负责协调创造行为;(3)对抗者:阻碍创造行为。这些角色中的每一个都直接影响着创造过程的不同阶段,这是通过人与人工制品之间的创造力分布而发生的。我们得出的结论是,人工制品在创造过程中具有暂时的作用,而不仅仅是增强或限制这一过程,它们是创造力的一个组成部分。因此,人与器物在创作过程中是纠缠在一起的,不应该分开分析。
{"title":"The Sociomateriality of the Creative Process: Script Roles in Film Production","authors":"José Edemir da Silva Anjo,&nbsp;César Tureta","doi":"10.1002/jocb.552","DOIUrl":"10.1002/jocb.552","url":null,"abstract":"<div>\u0000 \u0000 <p>The article aims to identify and analyze the roles played by the script in the creative process of independent short film production. The theoretical framework is based on distributed creativity and sociomateriality. These approaches are complementary and helped us discuss how the script can shape the creativity of the subjects involved in the film production. We have followed the daily teamwork during the film production through participant observation, interviews, documents, photographs, and video. We identified three roles played by the script: (1) protagonist: centralizes creative ideas around its framing; (2) feature player: responsible for mediating creative actions, and (3) antagonist: hinders creative actions. Each of these roles directly influences the different stages of the creative process, which takes place through creativity distributed between people and artifacts. We conclude that artifacts have temporary roles in the creative process, and more than enhancing or limiting this process, they are an integral part of creativity. Therefore, humans and artifacts are entangled in the creative process and should not be analyzed separately.</p>\u0000 </div>","PeriodicalId":39915,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Creative Behavior","volume":"56 4","pages":"566-583"},"PeriodicalIF":3.9,"publicationDate":"2022-06-16","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"41542503","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
期刊
Journal of Creative Behavior
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1