首页 > 最新文献

Collabra-Psychology最新文献

英文 中文
Perpetrators’ and Victims’ Folk Explanations of Aggressive Behaviors and Desires for Apologies 加害者与受害者对攻击行为的民间解释与道歉欲望
IF 2.5 3区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2023-01-01 DOI: 10.1525/collabra.84918
Randy J. McCarthy, Jared P Wilson
After an aggressive interaction, perpetrators most want to offer apologies when they have unintentionally harmed another person and victims most want to receive an apology when another person intentionally harmed them. Perpetrators and victims also explain aggressive behaviors differently—perpetrators often explain their own aggressive behaviors by referring to beliefs they considered that led to their behaviors (i.e., “belief” explanations), whereas victims explain perpetrators’ behaviors by referring to background factors that do not mention the perpetrators’ mental deliberations (i.e., “causal history explanations”). Putting these ideas together, the current Registered Report had participants recall either a time they intentionally harmed another person or a time when they were intentionally harmed by another person. Participants then rated several characteristics of the recalled behavior, explained why the behavior occurred, and reported their desire for an apology. As predicted, we found that perpetrators who gave “belief” explanations wanted to give an apology much less than participants who gave “causal history explanations.” However, and inconsistent with our predictions, victims’ desire to receive an apology was similar regardless of how they explained the perpetrators’ behaviors. These findings underscore how perpetrators’ explanations can emphasize (or de-emphasize) the deliberateness of their harmful behaviors and how these explanations are related to their desire to make amends.
在一场激烈的互动之后,行凶者最希望在无意中伤害他人时道歉,而受害者最希望在他人故意伤害他们时得到道歉。施暴者和受害者对攻击行为的解释也不同——施暴者通常用他们认为导致其行为的信念来解释自己的攻击行为(即“信念”解释),而受害者则用不提及施暴者心理考虑的背景因素来解释施暴者的行为(即“因果史解释”)。把这些想法放在一起,当前的注册报告让参与者回忆他们故意伤害他人的时间或他们被他人故意伤害的时间。然后,参与者对回忆起的行为的几个特征进行评分,解释为什么会发生这种行为,并报告他们想要道歉的愿望。正如预测的那样,我们发现,给出“信念”解释的肇事者比给出“因果历史”解释的参与者更不愿意道歉。然而,与我们的预测不一致的是,无论受害者如何解释加害者的行为,他们对道歉的渴望都是相似的。这些发现强调了肇事者的解释如何强调(或不强调)他们有害行为的故意性,以及这些解释如何与他们弥补的愿望相关。
{"title":"Perpetrators’ and Victims’ Folk Explanations of Aggressive Behaviors and Desires for Apologies","authors":"Randy J. McCarthy, Jared P Wilson","doi":"10.1525/collabra.84918","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1525/collabra.84918","url":null,"abstract":"After an aggressive interaction, perpetrators most want to offer apologies when they have unintentionally harmed another person and victims most want to receive an apology when another person intentionally harmed them. Perpetrators and victims also explain aggressive behaviors differently—perpetrators often explain their own aggressive behaviors by referring to beliefs they considered that led to their behaviors (i.e., “belief” explanations), whereas victims explain perpetrators’ behaviors by referring to background factors that do not mention the perpetrators’ mental deliberations (i.e., “causal history explanations”). Putting these ideas together, the current Registered Report had participants recall either a time they intentionally harmed another person or a time when they were intentionally harmed by another person. Participants then rated several characteristics of the recalled behavior, explained why the behavior occurred, and reported their desire for an apology. As predicted, we found that perpetrators who gave “belief” explanations wanted to give an apology much less than participants who gave “causal history explanations.” However, and inconsistent with our predictions, victims’ desire to receive an apology was similar regardless of how they explained the perpetrators’ behaviors. These findings underscore how perpetrators’ explanations can emphasize (or de-emphasize) the deliberateness of their harmful behaviors and how these explanations are related to their desire to make amends.","PeriodicalId":45791,"journal":{"name":"Collabra-Psychology","volume":"1 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.5,"publicationDate":"2023-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"66882525","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Disentangling the Contributions of Repeating Targets, Distractors, and Stimulus Positions to Practice Benefits in D2-Like Tests of Attention 重复目标、干扰物和刺激位置对2d样注意测试练习益处的贡献
IF 2.5 3区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2023-01-01 DOI: 10.1525/collabra.71297
Peter Wühr, B. Wühr
When a test of attention, such as the d2 test, is repeated, performance improves. These practice benefits threaten the validity of a test because it is impossible to separate the contributions of ability and practice, respectively, to a particular result. A possible solution to this dilemma would be to determine the sources of practice effects, and to use this knowledge for constructing tests that are less prone to practice. The present study investigates the contribution of three components of a d2-like test of attention to practice benefits: targets, distractors, and stimulus configurations. In Experiment 1, we compared practice effects in a target-change condition, where targets changed between sessions, to a target-repetition condition. Similarly, in Experiment 2, we compared practice effects in a distractor-change condition to a distractor-repetition condition. Finally, in Experiment 3, we compared practice effects in a position-repetition condition, where stimulus configurations were repeated within and between tests, to a position-change condition. Results showed that repeating targets and repeating distractors contribute to practice effects, whereas repeating stimulus configurations does not. Hence, in order to reduce practice effects, one might construct tests in which target learning is prevented, for example, by using multiple targets.
当重复进行注意力测试(如d2测试)时,表现会有所改善。这些实践的好处威胁到测试的有效性,因为不可能将能力和实践分别对特定结果的贡献分开。对于这种困境的一个可能的解决方案是确定实践效果的来源,并使用这些知识来构建不太容易实践的测试。本研究探讨了2d样注意测试对练习收益的三个组成部分的贡献:目标、干扰物和刺激配置。在实验1中,我们比较了目标变化条件下的练习效果,其中目标在会话之间变化,目标重复条件下。同样,在实验2中,我们比较了在分心物变化条件下和在分心物重复条件下的练习效果。最后,在实验3中,我们比较了位置重复条件下的练习效果,其中刺激配置在测试内和测试之间重复,以及位置变化条件。结果表明,重复的目标和重复的干扰对练习效果有影响,而重复的刺激配置对练习效果没有影响。因此,为了减少练习效果,人们可能会设计一些测试,例如,通过使用多个目标来阻止目标学习。
{"title":"Disentangling the Contributions of Repeating Targets, Distractors, and Stimulus Positions to Practice Benefits in D2-Like Tests of Attention","authors":"Peter Wühr, B. Wühr","doi":"10.1525/collabra.71297","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1525/collabra.71297","url":null,"abstract":"When a test of attention, such as the d2 test, is repeated, performance improves. These practice benefits threaten the validity of a test because it is impossible to separate the contributions of ability and practice, respectively, to a particular result. A possible solution to this dilemma would be to determine the sources of practice effects, and to use this knowledge for constructing tests that are less prone to practice. The present study investigates the contribution of three components of a d2-like test of attention to practice benefits: targets, distractors, and stimulus configurations. In Experiment 1, we compared practice effects in a target-change condition, where targets changed between sessions, to a target-repetition condition. Similarly, in Experiment 2, we compared practice effects in a distractor-change condition to a distractor-repetition condition. Finally, in Experiment 3, we compared practice effects in a position-repetition condition, where stimulus configurations were repeated within and between tests, to a position-change condition. Results showed that repeating targets and repeating distractors contribute to practice effects, whereas repeating stimulus configurations does not. Hence, in order to reduce practice effects, one might construct tests in which target learning is prevented, for example, by using multiple targets.","PeriodicalId":45791,"journal":{"name":"Collabra-Psychology","volume":"1 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.5,"publicationDate":"2023-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"66879940","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The Social Media Sexist Content (SMSC) Database: A Database of Content and Comments for Research Use 社会媒体性别歧视内容(SMSC)数据库:一个用于研究的内容和评论数据库
IF 2.5 3区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2023-01-01 DOI: 10.1525/collabra.71341
Hannah S. Buie, A. Croft
We introduce the Social Media Sexist Content (SMSC) database, an open-access online stimulus set consisting of 382 social media content items and 221 comments related to the content. The content items include 90 sexist posts and 292 neutral posts. The comment items include 75 sexist comments along with 238 neutral comments. The database consists of a broad range of topics including lifestyle, memes, and school posts. All posts were anonymized after being retrieved from publicly available sources. All content and comments were rated across two domains: degree of sexism and emotional reaction to the post. In terms of sexism, the posts were rated along three dimensions of gender bias: Hostile Sexism, Benevolent Sexism, and Objectification. Participants also provided their emotional reactions to the posts in terms of feeling Ashamed, Insecure, and/or Angry. Data were collected online in two separate studies: one rating the content and the other rating the comments. The sexism and emotion ratings were highly reliable and showed the posts displayed either sexism or neutral content. The SMSC database is beneficial to researchers because it offers updated social media content for research use online and in the lab. The database affords researchers the ability to explore stimuli either by content or by ratings, and the database is free to use for research purposes. The SMSC is available for download from hannahbuie.com.
我们引入了社交媒体性别歧视内容(SMSC)数据库,这是一个开放获取的在线刺激集,由382个社交媒体内容项目和221个与内容相关的评论组成。内容项包括90个性别歧视帖子和292个中性帖子。评论条目包括75条性别歧视评论和238条中性评论。该数据库包含广泛的主题,包括生活方式、表情包和学校帖子。所有帖子都是在从公开来源检索后匿名的。所有的内容和评论都分为两个方面:性别歧视程度和对帖子的情绪反应。在性别歧视方面,这些帖子根据性别偏见的三个维度进行评分:敌意性别歧视、善意性别歧视和物化。参与者还提供了他们对帖子的情绪反应,如感到羞耻、不安全和/或愤怒。数据是在两个独立的研究中在线收集的:一个对内容进行评级,另一个对评论进行评级。性别歧视和情绪评级是高度可靠的,表明帖子要么显示性别歧视,要么显示中性内容。SMSC数据库对研究人员是有益的,因为它为在线和实验室的研究提供了最新的社会媒体内容。该数据库为研究人员提供了通过内容或评级来探索刺激的能力,并且该数据库可免费用于研究目的。SMSC可从hannahbuie.com下载。
{"title":"The Social Media Sexist Content (SMSC) Database: A Database of Content and Comments for Research Use","authors":"Hannah S. Buie, A. Croft","doi":"10.1525/collabra.71341","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1525/collabra.71341","url":null,"abstract":"We introduce the Social Media Sexist Content (SMSC) database, an open-access online stimulus set consisting of 382 social media content items and 221 comments related to the content. The content items include 90 sexist posts and 292 neutral posts. The comment items include 75 sexist comments along with 238 neutral comments. The database consists of a broad range of topics including lifestyle, memes, and school posts. All posts were anonymized after being retrieved from publicly available sources. All content and comments were rated across two domains: degree of sexism and emotional reaction to the post. In terms of sexism, the posts were rated along three dimensions of gender bias: Hostile Sexism, Benevolent Sexism, and Objectification. Participants also provided their emotional reactions to the posts in terms of feeling Ashamed, Insecure, and/or Angry. Data were collected online in two separate studies: one rating the content and the other rating the comments. The sexism and emotion ratings were highly reliable and showed the posts displayed either sexism or neutral content. The SMSC database is beneficial to researchers because it offers updated social media content for research use online and in the lab. The database affords researchers the ability to explore stimuli either by content or by ratings, and the database is free to use for research purposes. The SMSC is available for download from hannahbuie.com.","PeriodicalId":45791,"journal":{"name":"Collabra-Psychology","volume":"1 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.5,"publicationDate":"2023-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"66880000","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
What Diverse Samples Can Teach Us About Cognitive Vulnerability to Depression 不同的样本可以告诉我们抑郁症的认知脆弱性
IF 2.5 3区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2023-01-01 DOI: 10.1525/collabra.71346
G. Haeffel, Hugh H Burke, Marissa Vander Missen, Lily M. Brouder
Tests of generalizability can diversify psychological science and improve theories and measurement. To this end, we conducted five studies testing the cognitive vulnerability to depression hypothesis featured in the hopelessness theory of depression: Study 1 was conducted with Honduran young adults (n = 50); Study 2 was conducted with Nepali adults (n = 34); Study 3 was conducted with Western hemisphere adults (n = 104); Study 4 was conducted with Black U.S. adults (n = 119); and Study 5 was conducted with U.S. undergraduates (n = 110). Results showed that cognitive vulnerability could be measured reliably in diverse populations and the distribution of vulnerability scores was similar for all samples. However, the tendency to generate negative inferences about stress had different implications for depression depending on sample; the association between cognitive vulnerability and depressive symptoms did not generalize to Honduran and Nepali participants. It is now necessary to understand why a negative cognitive style confers risk for depression in some contexts but not others (e.g., is it issues related to measurement, theory, or both). The results also suggest that understanding and reducing the global burden of depression will require more than simply “translating” existing cognitive measures and theories to other countries.
概括性测试可以使心理科学多样化,改进理论和测量方法。为此,我们进行了五项研究,以测试抑郁症绝望理论中的抑郁认知脆弱性假说:研究1是对洪都拉斯年轻人进行的(n = 50);研究2在尼泊尔成年人中进行(n = 34);研究3是在西半球成年人中进行的(n = 104);研究4是在美国黑人成年人中进行的(n = 119);研究5在美国大学生中进行(n = 110)。结果表明,认知脆弱性可以在不同的人群中可靠地测量,并且所有样本的脆弱性得分分布相似。然而,根据样本的不同,对压力产生负面推断的倾向对抑郁有不同的影响;认知脆弱性和抑郁症状之间的关联并没有推广到洪都拉斯和尼泊尔的参与者。现在有必要了解为什么消极的认知风格在某些情况下会带来抑郁的风险,而在其他情况下却不会(例如,它是与测量、理论相关的问题,还是两者兼而有之)。研究结果还表明,理解和减轻全球抑郁症负担需要的不仅仅是将现有的认知措施和理论“翻译”到其他国家。
{"title":"What Diverse Samples Can Teach Us About Cognitive Vulnerability to Depression","authors":"G. Haeffel, Hugh H Burke, Marissa Vander Missen, Lily M. Brouder","doi":"10.1525/collabra.71346","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1525/collabra.71346","url":null,"abstract":"Tests of generalizability can diversify psychological science and improve theories and measurement. To this end, we conducted five studies testing the cognitive vulnerability to depression hypothesis featured in the hopelessness theory of depression: Study 1 was conducted with Honduran young adults (n = 50); Study 2 was conducted with Nepali adults (n = 34); Study 3 was conducted with Western hemisphere adults (n = 104); Study 4 was conducted with Black U.S. adults (n = 119); and Study 5 was conducted with U.S. undergraduates (n = 110). Results showed that cognitive vulnerability could be measured reliably in diverse populations and the distribution of vulnerability scores was similar for all samples. However, the tendency to generate negative inferences about stress had different implications for depression depending on sample; the association between cognitive vulnerability and depressive symptoms did not generalize to Honduran and Nepali participants. It is now necessary to understand why a negative cognitive style confers risk for depression in some contexts but not others (e.g., is it issues related to measurement, theory, or both). The results also suggest that understanding and reducing the global burden of depression will require more than simply “translating” existing cognitive measures and theories to other countries.","PeriodicalId":45791,"journal":{"name":"Collabra-Psychology","volume":"1 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.5,"publicationDate":"2023-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"66880010","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Does Going Green Feel Good in Russia: Implicit Measurements With Visual Stimuli 在俄罗斯走绿色道路感觉好吗:视觉刺激的隐性测量
IF 2.5 3区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2023-01-01 DOI: 10.1525/collabra.73637
D. Valko
Research has shown that in developed environmental cultures, people typically have positive attitudes towards sustainability and pro-environmental behaviour. This has been measured both explicitly, through surveys and interviews, and implicitly, through indirect measures. However, this phenomenon has not yet been extensively studied in emerging environmental cultures, such as Russia. In this study, we adapted two indirect measures, the Affect misattribution procedure and the Affective priming procedure, to examine whether people in Russia have a positive pro-environmental attitude and whether there is a relationship between this implicitly measured attitude and an explicit environmental concern. To ensure reproducibility, we preregistered and conducted two similar studies. The total sample size of the two studies is 394. Our results showed that both measures converge and successfully detect the existence of a positive implicit attitude towards sustainability and pro-environmental behaviour, but there does not appear to be a relationship with environmental concern.
研究表明,在发达的环境文化中,人们通常对可持续性和亲环境行为持积极态度。这可以通过调查和访谈来明确衡量,也可以通过间接衡量来含蓄衡量。然而,这一现象尚未在新兴的环境文化中得到广泛研究,如俄罗斯。在本研究中,我们采用了情感错误归因程序和情感启动程序两种间接测量方法来检验俄罗斯人是否具有积极的亲环境态度,以及这种内隐测量态度与外显环境关注之间是否存在关系。为了确保可重复性,我们预先注册并进行了两个类似的研究。两项研究的总样本量为394。我们的研究结果表明,这两种措施收敛并成功地发现了对可持续发展和亲环境行为的积极隐性态度的存在,但似乎没有与环境关注的关系。
{"title":"Does Going Green Feel Good in Russia: Implicit Measurements With Visual Stimuli","authors":"D. Valko","doi":"10.1525/collabra.73637","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1525/collabra.73637","url":null,"abstract":"Research has shown that in developed environmental cultures, people typically have positive attitudes towards sustainability and pro-environmental behaviour. This has been measured both explicitly, through surveys and interviews, and implicitly, through indirect measures. However, this phenomenon has not yet been extensively studied in emerging environmental cultures, such as Russia. In this study, we adapted two indirect measures, the Affect misattribution procedure and the Affective priming procedure, to examine whether people in Russia have a positive pro-environmental attitude and whether there is a relationship between this implicitly measured attitude and an explicit environmental concern. To ensure reproducibility, we preregistered and conducted two similar studies. The total sample size of the two studies is 394. Our results showed that both measures converge and successfully detect the existence of a positive implicit attitude towards sustainability and pro-environmental behaviour, but there does not appear to be a relationship with environmental concern.","PeriodicalId":45791,"journal":{"name":"Collabra-Psychology","volume":"1 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.5,"publicationDate":"2023-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"66880238","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Cognitive Load Can Reduce Reason-Giving in a Moral Dumbfounding Task 认知负荷可以减少道德哑巴任务中的推理
IF 2.5 3区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2023-01-01 DOI: 10.1525/collabra.73818
Cillian McHugh, M. McGann, E. Igou, E. Kinsella
Moral dumbfounding occurs when people defend a moral judgment, without reasons in support of this judgment. The phenomenon has been influential in moral psychology, however, despite its influence, it remains poorly understood. Based on the notion that cognitive load enhances biases and shortcomings in human judgment when elaboration is beneficial, we hypothesized that under cognitive load, people would be less likely to provide reasons for a judgment and more likely to be dumbfounded (or to change their judgment). In a pre-registered study (N = 1686) we tested this prediction. Our findings suggest that cognitive load reduces reason-giving, and increases dumbfounding (but does not lead to changes in judgments). Our results provide new insights into the phenomenon of moral dumbfounding while also advancing theory in moral psychology.
当人们捍卫一个道德判断,却没有理由支持这个判断时,就会出现道德哑巴。这一现象对道德心理学产生了影响,然而,尽管它的影响很大,但人们对它的了解仍然很少。基于认知负荷增强了人类判断的偏差和缺陷,当详细阐述是有益的,我们假设在认知负荷下,人们不太可能为判断提供理由,更有可能目瞪口呆(或改变他们的判断)。在一项预登记研究(N = 1686)中,我们检验了这一预测。我们的研究结果表明,认知负荷减少了推理,增加了哑巴(但不会导致判断的改变)。我们的研究结果为道德哑巴现象提供了新的见解,同时也推动了道德心理学的理论发展。
{"title":"Cognitive Load Can Reduce Reason-Giving in a Moral Dumbfounding Task","authors":"Cillian McHugh, M. McGann, E. Igou, E. Kinsella","doi":"10.1525/collabra.73818","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1525/collabra.73818","url":null,"abstract":"Moral dumbfounding occurs when people defend a moral judgment, without reasons in support of this judgment. The phenomenon has been influential in moral psychology, however, despite its influence, it remains poorly understood. Based on the notion that cognitive load enhances biases and shortcomings in human judgment when elaboration is beneficial, we hypothesized that under cognitive load, people would be less likely to provide reasons for a judgment and more likely to be dumbfounded (or to change their judgment). In a pre-registered study (N = 1686) we tested this prediction. Our findings suggest that cognitive load reduces reason-giving, and increases dumbfounding (but does not lead to changes in judgments). Our results provide new insights into the phenomenon of moral dumbfounding while also advancing theory in moral psychology.","PeriodicalId":45791,"journal":{"name":"Collabra-Psychology","volume":"1 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.5,"publicationDate":"2023-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"66880659","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
An Improved Measure for the Strength of Social Gender Norms (SSGN) Developed for Adolescents in Uttar Pradesh, India 为印度北方邦青少年制定的社会性别规范(SSGN)强度改进措施
IF 2.5 3区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2023-01-01 DOI: 10.1525/collabra.75220
Krittika Gorur, B. Cislaghi, Patrick S. Forscher
Social norms can frame how typical and appropriate the choices available to individuals are, making some more difficult while others easier to make. Despite the important role of both descriptive and injunctive norms for intervention, few measures are available that distinguish these types of perceptions. Fewer still are tailored for settings where development challenges are present and behaviorally-informed interventions are implemented. To address gaps in measuring social norms that impact women’s employment in India, this study was conducted with 399 adolescents aged 14-17 years to develop the Strength of Social Gender Norms (SSGN) scale. Exploratory factor analysis demonstrated a good two-factor structure. Psychometric analyses satisfied tests for internal consistency, differentiated it from attitudes, and found moderate test-retest reliability. Using this scale, we found that girls perceived more positive social norms overall but held more negative perceptions of what others in their communities think about women working (i.e. injunctive norms), relative to boys. Our results confirm the ability of the SSGN scale to distinguish different aspects of social norms among low-income Indian adolescents, a population that is neglected in psychology research at large. Future research should aim to replicate results in additional hard-to-reach samples and investigate the association between actual longer-term employment outcomes of women.
社会规范可以界定个人所能做出的选择的典型和适当程度,使一些选择更困难,而另一些选择更容易。尽管描述性规范和禁令性规范对干预都起着重要作用,但很少有措施可以区分这些类型的看法。针对存在发展挑战和实施行为知情干预措施的环境量身定制的方案就更少了。为了解决在衡量影响印度妇女就业的社会规范方面的差距,本研究对399名14-17岁的青少年进行了研究,以制定社会性别规范强度(SSGN)量表。探索性因子分析显示了良好的双因子结构。心理测量分析满足内部一致性测试,将其与态度区分开来,并发现适度的重测信度。使用这个量表,我们发现,相对于男孩,女孩总体上感知到更多积极的社会规范,但对社区中其他人对女性工作的看法(即禁令规范)持有更多消极的看法。我们的研究结果证实了SSGN量表在低收入印度青少年中区分社会规范不同方面的能力,这是一个在心理学研究中被忽视的群体。未来的研究应着眼于在其他难以触及的样本中复制结果,并调查妇女实际长期就业结果之间的关系。
{"title":"An Improved Measure for the Strength of Social Gender Norms (SSGN) Developed for Adolescents in Uttar Pradesh, India","authors":"Krittika Gorur, B. Cislaghi, Patrick S. Forscher","doi":"10.1525/collabra.75220","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1525/collabra.75220","url":null,"abstract":"Social norms can frame how typical and appropriate the choices available to individuals are, making some more difficult while others easier to make. Despite the important role of both descriptive and injunctive norms for intervention, few measures are available that distinguish these types of perceptions. Fewer still are tailored for settings where development challenges are present and behaviorally-informed interventions are implemented. To address gaps in measuring social norms that impact women’s employment in India, this study was conducted with 399 adolescents aged 14-17 years to develop the Strength of Social Gender Norms (SSGN) scale. Exploratory factor analysis demonstrated a good two-factor structure. Psychometric analyses satisfied tests for internal consistency, differentiated it from attitudes, and found moderate test-retest reliability. Using this scale, we found that girls perceived more positive social norms overall but held more negative perceptions of what others in their communities think about women working (i.e. injunctive norms), relative to boys. Our results confirm the ability of the SSGN scale to distinguish different aspects of social norms among low-income Indian adolescents, a population that is neglected in psychology research at large. Future research should aim to replicate results in additional hard-to-reach samples and investigate the association between actual longer-term employment outcomes of women.","PeriodicalId":45791,"journal":{"name":"Collabra-Psychology","volume":"1 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.5,"publicationDate":"2023-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"66881116","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Ageism in Hiring: A Systematic Review and Meta-analysis of Age Discrimination 雇佣中的年龄歧视:年龄歧视的系统回顾与元分析
IF 2.5 3区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2023-01-01 DOI: 10.1525/collabra.82194
Lucija Batinovic, Marlon Howe, Samantha Sinclair, Rickard Carlsson
We aimed to identify effect sizes of age discrimination in recruitment based on evidence from correspondence studies and scenario experiments conducted between 2010 and 2019. To differentiate our results, we separated outcomes (i.e., call-back rates and hiring/invitation to interview likelihood) by age groups (40-49, 50-59, 60-65, 66+) and assessed age discrimination by comparing older applicants to a control group (29-35 year-olds). We conducted searches in PsycInfo, Web of Science, ERIC, BASE, and Google Scholar, along with backward reference searching. Study bias was assessed with a tool developed for this review, and publication bias by calculating R-index, p-curve, and funnel plots. We calculated odds ratios for callback rates, pooled the results using a random-effects meta-analysis and calculated 95% confidence intervals. We included 13 studies from 11 articles in our review, and conducted meta-analyses on the eight studies that we were able to extract data from. The majority of studies were correspondence studies (k=10) and came largely from European countries (k=9), with the rest being from the U.S. (k=3) and Australia (k=1). Seven studies had a between-participants design, and the remaining six studies had a within-participants design. We conducted six random-effects meta-analyses, one for each age category and type of study design and found an average effect of age discrimination against all age groups in both study designs, with varying effect sizes (ranging from OR = 0.38, CI [0.25, 0.59] to OR = 0.89, CI [0.81, 0.97]). There was moderate to high risk of bias on certain factors, e.g., age randomization, problems with application heterogeneity. Generally, there’s an effect of age discrimination and it tends to increase with age. This has important implications regarding the future of the world’s workforce, given the increase in the older workforce and later retirement.
基于2010年至2019年间进行的对应研究和情景实验的证据,我们旨在确定年龄歧视在招聘中的效应大小。为了区分我们的结果,我们按年龄组(40-49岁,50-59岁,60-65岁,66岁以上)将结果(即回调率和招聘/邀请面试的可能性)分开,并通过将年龄较大的申请人与对照组(29-35岁)进行比较来评估年龄歧视。我们在PsycInfo, Web of Science, ERIC, BASE和谷歌Scholar中进行了搜索,并进行了反向参考搜索。使用为本综述开发的工具评估研究偏倚,通过计算r指数、p曲线和漏斗图评估发表偏倚。我们计算回调率的优势比,使用随机效应荟萃分析汇总结果,并计算95%置信区间。我们在综述中纳入了11篇文章中的13项研究,并对我们能够提取数据的8项研究进行了荟萃分析。大多数研究为函授研究(k=10),主要来自欧洲国家(k=9),其余研究来自美国(k=3)和澳大利亚(k=1)。7项研究采用参与者间设计,其余6项研究采用参与者内设计。我们进行了六次随机效应荟萃分析,每个年龄类别和研究设计类型各一次,发现两种研究设计中年龄歧视对所有年龄组的平均影响,影响大小不同(从OR = 0.38, CI[0.25, 0.59]到OR = 0.89, CI[0.81, 0.97])。在某些因素上存在中等到高度的偏倚风险,例如年龄随机化、应用异质性问题。一般来说,年龄歧视会产生影响,而且会随着年龄的增长而加剧。鉴于老年劳动力的增加和退休年龄的推迟,这对世界劳动力的未来具有重要意义。
{"title":"Ageism in Hiring: A Systematic Review and Meta-analysis of Age Discrimination","authors":"Lucija Batinovic, Marlon Howe, Samantha Sinclair, Rickard Carlsson","doi":"10.1525/collabra.82194","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1525/collabra.82194","url":null,"abstract":"We aimed to identify effect sizes of age discrimination in recruitment based on evidence from correspondence studies and scenario experiments conducted between 2010 and 2019. To differentiate our results, we separated outcomes (i.e., call-back rates and hiring/invitation to interview likelihood) by age groups (40-49, 50-59, 60-65, 66+) and assessed age discrimination by comparing older applicants to a control group (29-35 year-olds). We conducted searches in PsycInfo, Web of Science, ERIC, BASE, and Google Scholar, along with backward reference searching. Study bias was assessed with a tool developed for this review, and publication bias by calculating R-index, p-curve, and funnel plots. We calculated odds ratios for callback rates, pooled the results using a random-effects meta-analysis and calculated 95% confidence intervals. We included 13 studies from 11 articles in our review, and conducted meta-analyses on the eight studies that we were able to extract data from. The majority of studies were correspondence studies (k=10) and came largely from European countries (k=9), with the rest being from the U.S. (k=3) and Australia (k=1). Seven studies had a between-participants design, and the remaining six studies had a within-participants design. We conducted six random-effects meta-analyses, one for each age category and type of study design and found an average effect of age discrimination against all age groups in both study designs, with varying effect sizes (ranging from OR = 0.38, CI [0.25, 0.59] to OR = 0.89, CI [0.81, 0.97]). There was moderate to high risk of bias on certain factors, e.g., age randomization, problems with application heterogeneity. Generally, there’s an effect of age discrimination and it tends to increase with age. This has important implications regarding the future of the world’s workforce, given the increase in the older workforce and later retirement.","PeriodicalId":45791,"journal":{"name":"Collabra-Psychology","volume":"1 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.5,"publicationDate":"2023-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"66882169","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 2
From Halo to Conditioning and Back Again: Exploring the Links Between Impression Formation and Learning 从光环到条件反射再回来:探索印象形成和学习之间的联系
IF 2.5 3区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2023-01-01 DOI: 10.1525/collabra.84560
M. Rougier, J. de Houwer, J. Richetin, Sean Hughes, M. Perugini
Impression formation effects – such as the halo effect – and learning effects – such as evaluative or attribute conditioning effects – are often seen as separate classes of phenomena. In a recent conceptual paper, De Houwer et al. (2019) suggested that both may actually qualify as instances of feature transformation, where a source feature (e.g., attractiveness of a face; valence of an unconditioned stimulus; US) influences judgements about a target feature (e.g., social competence of a person; valence of a conditioned stimulus; CS). In halo effects, the source and target features typically differ (e.g., a person with an attractive face is judged as more socially competent) but belong to the same object. In evaluative conditioning, source and target features are the same (e.g., a neutral CS is judged as more positive after being paired with a positive US) but belong to different objects. In this paper, we highlight a phenomenon at the crossroads of the two previous effects: feature transformation where source and target features are different (as in halo studies) and belong to different objects that are paired together (as in evaluative conditioning studies). Across six pre-registered experiments (n = 1050), we obtained evidence for this phenomenon in the context of person perception (i.e., attractiveness halo) and food perception (i.e., health halo). We also show that this type of feature transformation is influenced by several known moderators of halo and conditioning effects (beliefs about traits relationship, memory of pairings, and salience of the source feature).
印象形成效应(如光环效应)和学习效应(如评价或属性条件反射效应)通常被视为不同类别的现象。在最近的一篇概念性论文中,De Houwer等人(2019)认为,两者实际上都可以作为特征转换的实例,其中源特征(例如,面孔的吸引力;无条件刺激的效价;美国)影响对目标特征的判断(例如,一个人的社会能力;条件刺激的效价;CS)。在光环效应中,来源和目标特征通常是不同的(例如,一个拥有迷人面孔的人被认为更有社会能力),但属于同一个对象。在评价性条件反射中,源特征和目标特征是相同的(例如,中性的CS与积极的US配对后被判断为更积极),但属于不同的对象。在本文中,我们强调了前面两种效应的十字路口的一种现象:特征转换,其中源和目标特征不同(如在光晕研究中),并且属于配对在一起的不同对象(如在评价条件反射研究中)。通过六个预先注册的实验(n = 1050),我们在人感知(即吸引力光环)和食物感知(即健康光环)的背景下获得了这种现象的证据。我们还表明,这种类型的特征转换受到光环效应和条件反射效应的几个已知调节因子(关于特征关系的信念、配对记忆和源特征的显著性)的影响。
{"title":"From Halo to Conditioning and Back Again: Exploring the Links Between Impression Formation and Learning","authors":"M. Rougier, J. de Houwer, J. Richetin, Sean Hughes, M. Perugini","doi":"10.1525/collabra.84560","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1525/collabra.84560","url":null,"abstract":"Impression formation effects – such as the halo effect – and learning effects – such as evaluative or attribute conditioning effects – are often seen as separate classes of phenomena. In a recent conceptual paper, De Houwer et al. (2019) suggested that both may actually qualify as instances of feature transformation, where a source feature (e.g., attractiveness of a face; valence of an unconditioned stimulus; US) influences judgements about a target feature (e.g., social competence of a person; valence of a conditioned stimulus; CS). In halo effects, the source and target features typically differ (e.g., a person with an attractive face is judged as more socially competent) but belong to the same object. In evaluative conditioning, source and target features are the same (e.g., a neutral CS is judged as more positive after being paired with a positive US) but belong to different objects. In this paper, we highlight a phenomenon at the crossroads of the two previous effects: feature transformation where source and target features are different (as in halo studies) and belong to different objects that are paired together (as in evaluative conditioning studies). Across six pre-registered experiments (n = 1050), we obtained evidence for this phenomenon in the context of person perception (i.e., attractiveness halo) and food perception (i.e., health halo). We also show that this type of feature transformation is influenced by several known moderators of halo and conditioning effects (beliefs about traits relationship, memory of pairings, and salience of the source feature).","PeriodicalId":45791,"journal":{"name":"Collabra-Psychology","volume":"1 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.5,"publicationDate":"2023-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"66882763","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 2
Machine learning mega-analysis applied to the Response Time Concealed Information Test: No evidence for advantage of model-based predictors over baseline 应用于响应时间隐藏信息测试的机器学习大型分析:没有证据表明基于模型的预测器优于基线
IF 2.5 3区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2022-02-16 DOI: 10.31234/osf.io/mfjx8
Gáspár Lukács, D. Steyrl
The response time Concealed Information Test (RT-CIT) can help to reveal whether a person is concealing the knowledge of a certain information detail. During the RT-CIT, the examinee is repeatedly presented with a probe, the detail in question (e.g., murder weapon), and several irrelevants, other details that are similar to the probe (e.g., other weapons). These items all require the same keypress response, while one further item, the target, requires a different keypress response. Examinees tend to respond to the probe slower than to irrelevants, when they recognize the former as the relevant detail. To classify examinees as having or not having recognized the probe, RT-CIT studies have almost always used the averaged difference between probe and irrelevant RTs as the single predictor variable. In the present study, we tested whether we can improve classification accuracy (recognized the probe: yes or no) by incorporating the average RTs, the accuracy rates, and the SDs of each item type (probe, irrelevant, and target). Using the data from 1,871 individual tests and incorporating various combinations of the additional variables, we built logistic regression, linear discriminant analysis, and extra trees machine learning models (altogether 26), and we compared the classification accuracy of each of the model-based predictors to that of the sole probe-irrelevant RT difference predictor as baseline. None of the models provided significant improvement over the baseline. Nominal gains in classification accuracy ranged between –1.5% and 3.1%. In each of the models, machine learning captured the probe-irrelevant RT difference as the most important contributor to successful predictions, or, when included separately, the probe RT and the irrelevant RT as the first and second most important contributors, respectively.
响应时间隐藏信息测试(RT-CIT)可以帮助揭示一个人是否在隐瞒对某个信息细节的了解。在RT-CIT过程中,受试者被反复出示一个探针、有问题的细节(例如,凶器)和几个无关的、与探针相似的其他细节(例如其他武器)。这些项目都需要相同的按键响应,而另一个项目,目标,需要不同的按键响应。当考生将前者视为相关细节时,他们对调查的反应往往比对无关内容的反应慢。为了将考生归类为已经或没有识别出探针,RT-CIT研究几乎总是使用探针和不相关RT之间的平均差异作为单一预测变量。在本研究中,我们测试了是否可以通过合并每个项目类型(探针、无关和目标)的平均RT、准确率和SD来提高分类准确性(识别探针:是或否)。使用1871个个体测试的数据,并结合其他变量的各种组合,我们建立了逻辑回归、线性判别分析和额外树机器学习模型(共26个),并将每个基于模型的预测因子的分类精度与唯一探针无关的RT差分预测因子作为基线进行了比较。没有一个模型比基线有显著改善。分类准确率的名义增益在-1.5%到3.1%之间。在每一个模型中,机器学习都将与探针无关的RT差异视为成功预测的最重要因素,或者,如果单独包括,则将探针RT和不相关的RT分别视为第一和第二重要因素。
{"title":"Machine learning mega-analysis applied to the Response Time Concealed Information Test: No evidence for advantage of model-based predictors over baseline","authors":"Gáspár Lukács, D. Steyrl","doi":"10.31234/osf.io/mfjx8","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/mfjx8","url":null,"abstract":"The response time Concealed Information Test (RT-CIT) can help to reveal whether a person is concealing the knowledge of a certain information detail. During the RT-CIT, the examinee is repeatedly presented with a probe, the detail in question (e.g., murder weapon), and several irrelevants, other details that are similar to the probe (e.g., other weapons). These items all require the same keypress response, while one further item, the target, requires a different keypress response. Examinees tend to respond to the probe slower than to irrelevants, when they recognize the former as the relevant detail. To classify examinees as having or not having recognized the probe, RT-CIT studies have almost always used the averaged difference between probe and irrelevant RTs as the single predictor variable. In the present study, we tested whether we can improve classification accuracy (recognized the probe: yes or no) by incorporating the average RTs, the accuracy rates, and the SDs of each item type (probe, irrelevant, and target). Using the data from 1,871 individual tests and incorporating various combinations of the additional variables, we built logistic regression, linear discriminant analysis, and extra trees machine learning models (altogether 26), and we compared the classification accuracy of each of the model-based predictors to that of the sole probe-irrelevant RT difference predictor as baseline. None of the models provided significant improvement over the baseline. Nominal gains in classification accuracy ranged between –1.5% and 3.1%. In each of the models, machine learning captured the probe-irrelevant RT difference as the most important contributor to successful predictions, or, when included separately, the probe RT and the irrelevant RT as the first and second most important contributors, respectively.","PeriodicalId":45791,"journal":{"name":"Collabra-Psychology","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.5,"publicationDate":"2022-02-16","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"44616560","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
期刊
Collabra-Psychology
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1