首页 > 最新文献

Collabra-Psychology最新文献

英文 中文
Time spent playing two online shooters has no measurable effect on aggressive affect 玩两个在线射击游戏的时间对攻击性影响没有可测量的影响
IF 2.5 3区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2022-01-24 DOI: 10.31234/osf.io/gt8ze
Niklas Johannes, Matti Vuorre, Kristoffer Magnusson, Andrew K. Przybylski
There is a lively debate whether playing games that feature armed combat and competition (often referred to as violent video games) has measurable effects on aggression. Unfortunately, that debate has produced little actionable insights. Here, we present a secondary analysis of the most authoritative longitudinal data set available on the issue, from our previous study (Vuorre et al., 2021), to deliver such insights. We analyzed objective in-game behavior, provided by video game companies, in 2,580 players over six weeks. Specifically, we asked how time spent playing two popular online shooters, Apex Legends (PEGI 16) and Outriders (PEGI 18), affected self-reported feelings of anger (i.e., aggressive affect). We found that playing these games did not increase aggressive affect; the cross-lagged association between game time and aggressive affect was virtually zero. Our results showcase the value of obtaining accurate industry data as well as an open science of video games and mental health that allows cumulative knowledge building.
关于玩以武装战斗和竞争为特色的游戏(通常被称为暴力视频游戏)是否会对攻击性产生可衡量的影响,存在着激烈的争论。不幸的是,这场辩论几乎没有产生什么可操作的见解。在这里,我们对我们之前的研究(Vuorre et al.,2021)中关于该问题的最权威的纵向数据集进行了二次分析,以提供这些见解。我们分析了视频游戏公司提供的2580名玩家在六周内的游戏内客观行为。具体来说,我们询问了玩两款流行的在线射击游戏Apex Legends(PEGI 16)和Outriders(PEGI 18)的时间如何影响自我报告的愤怒情绪(即攻击性情绪)。我们发现玩这些游戏并没有增加攻击性影响;比赛时间和攻击性情感之间的交叉滞后关联几乎为零。我们的研究结果展示了获得准确的行业数据的价值,以及允许积累知识的电子游戏和心理健康的开放科学。
{"title":"Time spent playing two online shooters has no measurable effect on aggressive affect","authors":"Niklas Johannes, Matti Vuorre, Kristoffer Magnusson, Andrew K. Przybylski","doi":"10.31234/osf.io/gt8ze","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/gt8ze","url":null,"abstract":"There is a lively debate whether playing games that feature armed combat and competition (often referred to as violent video games) has measurable effects on aggression. Unfortunately, that debate has produced little actionable insights. Here, we present a secondary analysis of the most authoritative longitudinal data set available on the issue, from our previous study (Vuorre et al., 2021), to deliver such insights. We analyzed objective in-game behavior, provided by video game companies, in 2,580 players over six weeks. Specifically, we asked how time spent playing two popular online shooters, Apex Legends (PEGI 16) and Outriders (PEGI 18), affected self-reported feelings of anger (i.e., aggressive affect). We found that playing these games did not increase aggressive affect; the cross-lagged association between game time and aggressive affect was virtually zero. Our results showcase the value of obtaining accurate industry data as well as an open science of video games and mental health that allows cumulative knowledge building.","PeriodicalId":45791,"journal":{"name":"Collabra-Psychology","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.5,"publicationDate":"2022-01-24","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"42912208","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 4
The Role of Trait Inferences in Evaluative Conditioning 特质推论在评价条件反射中的作用
IF 2.5 3区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2022-01-04 DOI: 10.31219/osf.io/juqs6
T. Moran, Sean Hughes, Pieter Van Dessel, J. de Houwer
Evaluative Conditioning (EC) effect is a change in evaluative responding to a neutral stimulus (CS) due to its pairing with a valenced stimulus (US). Traditionally, EC effects are viewed as fundamentally different from per-suasion effects. Inspired by a propositional perspective to EC, four studies (N = 1,284) tested if, like persuasion effects, EC effects can also be driven by trait inferences. Experiments 1-2 found that promoting trait inferences (by pairing people with trait words rather than nouns) increased EC effects. Experiments 3-4 found that under-mining trait inferences (by questioning the validity of those inferences) decreased EC effects. In all experiments, however, EC effects were still significant when trait inferences were invalid. Taken together, our findings (a) suggest that trait inferences can play an important role in EC effects, (b) constrain theoretical models of EC, and (c) have important implications for applied EC interventions.
评价条件反射效应是由于中性刺激(CS)与有价刺激(US)配对而引起的评价性反应的变化。传统上,EC效应被认为与劝说效应有着根本的区别。受命题视角的启发,四项研究(N = 1,284)测试了是否像说服效应一样,EC效应也可以由特质推断驱动。实验1-2发现,促进特质推断(通过将人们与特质词而不是名词配对)会增加EC效应。实验3-4发现,挖掘特质推论(通过质疑这些推论的有效性)会降低EC效应。然而,在所有实验中,当性状推断无效时,EC效应仍然显著。综上所述,我们的研究结果(a)表明特质推断可以在交际效应中发挥重要作用,(b)约束交际的理论模型,(c)对交际干预的应用具有重要意义。
{"title":"The Role of Trait Inferences in Evaluative Conditioning","authors":"T. Moran, Sean Hughes, Pieter Van Dessel, J. de Houwer","doi":"10.31219/osf.io/juqs6","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.31219/osf.io/juqs6","url":null,"abstract":"Evaluative Conditioning (EC) effect is a change in evaluative responding to a neutral stimulus (CS) due to its pairing with a valenced stimulus (US). Traditionally, EC effects are viewed as fundamentally different from per-suasion effects. Inspired by a propositional perspective to EC, four studies (N = 1,284) tested if, like persuasion effects, EC effects can also be driven by trait inferences. Experiments 1-2 found that promoting trait inferences (by pairing people with trait words rather than nouns) increased EC effects. Experiments 3-4 found that under-mining trait inferences (by questioning the validity of those inferences) decreased EC effects. In all experiments, however, EC effects were still significant when trait inferences were invalid. Taken together, our findings (a) suggest that trait inferences can play an important role in EC effects, (b) constrain theoretical models of EC, and (c) have important implications for applied EC interventions.","PeriodicalId":45791,"journal":{"name":"Collabra-Psychology","volume":"49 15","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.5,"publicationDate":"2022-01-04","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"41307208","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Expectation violations, expectation change, and expectation persistence: The scientific landscape as revealed by bibliometric network analyses 期望违背、期望变化和期望持续:文献计量网络分析揭示的科学景观
IF 2.5 3区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2022-01-04 DOI: 10.31234/osf.io/73f52
José C. García Alanis, A. Strelow, Martina Dort, H. Christiansen, M. Pinquart, Christian Panitz
Expectation violations occur when there is a discrepancy between expected and perceived events or experiences. However, expectations often persist despite disconfirming evidence. Therefore, research on expectation violations, expectation change, and expectation persistence has been conducted in several fields of psychology with wide-ranging theoretical assumptions and empirical considerations. In the present review, we analyzed how these research fields relate to each other via bibliometric network analyses. For this purpose, we conducted a systematic literature search to identify scientific publications on expectation violations, expectation change, and expectation persistence. The literature corpus was then quantitatively analyzed using similarity measures that allow a data-driven classification of publications into groups, revealing their conceptual, theoretical, and empirical commonalities. Our results indicate that many influential publications have focused on finding reactivity measures (e.g., brain activation) to the discrepancy experienced between expectations and outcomes. Furthermore, these measures have been used to assess when and to which degree learning and behavioral adaptation (i.e., expectation change) takes place. We discuss the potential application of these measures for understanding expectation violations in more complex settings (e.g., social interaction) as well as phenomena such as expectation persistence. The goal of this review was to foster interdisciplinarity in psychology, enabling scientists and practitioners to identify new topics, promising empirical approaches and previously neglected variables.
当期望和感知的事件或经历之间存在差异时,就会发生期望违反。然而,尽管有不确定的证据,人们的期望往往仍然存在。因此,期望违背、期望改变和期望持续的研究已经在心理学的多个领域展开,具有广泛的理论假设和实证考虑。在本文中,我们通过文献计量网络分析来分析这些研究领域之间的关系。为此,我们进行了系统的文献检索,以识别关于期望违反、期望变化和期望持久性的科学出版物。然后使用相似性度量对文献语料库进行定量分析,这些相似性度量允许数据驱动的出版物分类,揭示它们在概念、理论和经验上的共性。我们的结果表明,许多有影响力的出版物都专注于寻找预期和结果之间差异的反应性测量(例如,大脑激活)。此外,这些措施已被用于评估学习和行为适应(即期望变化)发生的时间和程度。我们讨论了这些措施在更复杂的环境中(例如,社会互动)以及期望持久性等现象中理解期望违反的潜在应用。这篇综述的目的是促进心理学的跨学科性,使科学家和实践者能够确定新的主题,有前途的经验方法和以前被忽视的变量。
{"title":"Expectation violations, expectation change, and expectation persistence: The scientific landscape as revealed by bibliometric network analyses","authors":"José C. García Alanis, A. Strelow, Martina Dort, H. Christiansen, M. Pinquart, Christian Panitz","doi":"10.31234/osf.io/73f52","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/73f52","url":null,"abstract":"Expectation violations occur when there is a discrepancy between expected and perceived events or experiences. However, expectations often persist despite disconfirming evidence. Therefore, research on expectation violations, expectation change, and expectation persistence has been conducted in several fields of psychology with wide-ranging theoretical assumptions and empirical considerations. In the present review, we analyzed how these research fields relate to each other via bibliometric network analyses. For this purpose, we conducted a systematic literature search to identify scientific publications on expectation violations, expectation change, and expectation persistence. The literature corpus was then quantitatively analyzed using similarity measures that allow a data-driven classification of publications into groups, revealing their conceptual, theoretical, and empirical commonalities. Our results indicate that many influential publications have focused on finding reactivity measures (e.g., brain activation) to the discrepancy experienced between expectations and outcomes. Furthermore, these measures have been used to assess when and to which degree learning and behavioral adaptation (i.e., expectation change) takes place. We discuss the potential application of these measures for understanding expectation violations in more complex settings (e.g., social interaction) as well as phenomena such as expectation persistence. The goal of this review was to foster interdisciplinarity in psychology, enabling scientists and practitioners to identify new topics, promising empirical approaches and previously neglected variables.","PeriodicalId":45791,"journal":{"name":"Collabra-Psychology","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.5,"publicationDate":"2022-01-04","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"45832053","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 2
Caring About (COVID-19 Related) Social Issues Signals Trustworthiness: Direct and Conceptual Replication of Zlatev (2019) 关注(COVID-19相关)社会问题标志着诚信:对兹拉捷夫的直接和概念性复制(2019)
IF 2.5 3区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2022-01-01 DOI: 10.1525/collabra.31036
A. Dorrough, Nathalie Bick, Lukas Bring, Caroline Brockers, C. Butz, I. Schneider
With three convenient samples (n = 1,087) and one sample representative for the German population in terms of age and gender (n = 210), we replicate research by Zlatev (2019) showing that perceived benevolence-based and perceived integrity-based trustworthiness increase with a target’s level of caring about a social issue. We show that these results generalize to various issues ranging from environmental issues (i.e., installation of wind turbines in the North Sea) to issues related to the COVID-19 pandemic (i.e., online teaching to prevent the spread of the virus). Furthermore, we provide initial behavioral evidence for this effect by showing that transfers in a trust game increase with a target’s caring about a social issue. All results are robust for age, gender, and social issue. To provide best estimates for the effect of a target’s level of caring on perceived trustworthiness, we report results of three mini meta-analyses including our findings as well as the findings of the original research. Policy implications are discussed.
通过三个方便的样本(n = 1087)和一个代表德国人口年龄和性别的样本(n = 210),我们复制了Zlatev(2019)的研究,该研究表明,基于仁慈的感知和基于诚信的感知可信度随着目标对社会问题的关心程度而增加。我们表明,这些结果可以推广到各种问题,从环境问题(即在北海安装风力涡轮机)到与COVID-19大流行相关的问题(即防止病毒传播的在线教学)。此外,我们通过表明信任游戏中的转移随着目标对社会问题的关心而增加,为这种效应提供了初步的行为证据。所有结果在年龄、性别和社会问题上都是可靠的。为了提供对目标关心水平对感知可信度影响的最佳估计,我们报告了三个小型荟萃分析的结果,包括我们的研究结果以及原始研究的结果。讨论了政策影响。
{"title":"Caring About (COVID-19 Related) Social Issues Signals Trustworthiness: Direct and Conceptual Replication of Zlatev (2019)","authors":"A. Dorrough, Nathalie Bick, Lukas Bring, Caroline Brockers, C. Butz, I. Schneider","doi":"10.1525/collabra.31036","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1525/collabra.31036","url":null,"abstract":"With three convenient samples (n = 1,087) and one sample representative for the German population in terms of age and gender (n = 210), we replicate research by Zlatev (2019) showing that perceived benevolence-based and perceived integrity-based trustworthiness increase with a target’s level of caring about a social issue. We show that these results generalize to various issues ranging from environmental issues (i.e., installation of wind turbines in the North Sea) to issues related to the COVID-19 pandemic (i.e., online teaching to prevent the spread of the virus). Furthermore, we provide initial behavioral evidence for this effect by showing that transfers in a trust game increase with a target’s caring about a social issue. All results are robust for age, gender, and social issue. To provide best estimates for the effect of a target’s level of caring on perceived trustworthiness, we report results of three mini meta-analyses including our findings as well as the findings of the original research. Policy implications are discussed.","PeriodicalId":45791,"journal":{"name":"Collabra-Psychology","volume":"1 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.5,"publicationDate":"2022-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"66875939","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Is Passive Priming Really Impervious to Verb Semantics? A High-Powered Replication of Messenger Et al. (2012) 被动启动真的不受动词语义的影响吗?Messenger等的高功率复制(2012)
IF 2.5 3区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2022-01-01 DOI: 10.1525/collabra.31055
I. M. S. Darmasetiyawan, Katherine Messenger, Ben Ambridge
The aim of the present study was to conduct a particularly stringent pre-registered in-vestigation of the claim that there exists a level of linguistic representation that “includes syntactic category information but not semantic information” (Branigan & Pickering, 2017: 8). As a test case, we focussed on the English passive; a construction for which previous findings have been somewhat contradictory. On the one hand, several studies using different methodologies have found an advantage for theme-experiencer passives (e.g., The girl was shocked by the tiger; and also agent-patient passives; e.g., The girl was hit by the tiger) over experiencer-theme passives (e.g., The girl was ignored by the tiger). On the other hand, Messenger et al. (2012) found no evidence that theme-experiencer and experiencer-theme passives vary in their propensity to prime production of agent-patient passives. We therefore conducted an online replication of Messen-ger et al (2012) with a pre-registered appropriately powered sample (N=240). Although a large and significant priming effect (i.e., an effect of prime sentence type) was ob-served, a Bayesian analysis yielded only weak/anecdotal evidence (BF=2.11) for the crucial interaction of verb type by prime type; a finding that was robust to different coding and exclusion decisions, operationalizations of verb semantics (dichoto-mous/continuous), analysis frameworks (Bayesian/frequentist) and – as per a mixed-effects-multiverse analyses – random effects structures. Nevertheless, these findings do no not provide evidence for the absence of semantic effects (as has been argued for the findings of Messenger et al, 2012). We conclude that these and related findings are best explained by a model that includes both lexical, exemplar-level representations and rep-resentations at multiple higher levels of abstraction.
本研究的目的是对存在“包括句法类别信息但不包括语义信息”的语言表征水平的说法进行特别严格的预注册调查(Branigan & Pickering, 2017: 8)。作为一个测试案例,我们关注英语被动语态;先前的研究结果多少有些矛盾。一方面,一些使用不同方法的研究发现了主题-体验者被动语态的优势(例如,女孩被老虎震惊了;还有施者-受者被动语态;例如,女孩被老虎打了)而不是体验主题被动语态(例如,女孩被老虎忽视了)。另一方面,Messenger等人(2012)发现,没有证据表明主题-体验者和体验者-主题被动语在主体-患者被动语的启动生产倾向上存在差异。因此,我们使用预先注册的适当通电样本(N=240)对messenger等人(2012)的研究进行了在线复制。虽然观察到一个大而显著的启动效应(即启动句子类型的效应),但贝叶斯分析只产生了微弱的/轶事证据(BF=2.11),证明动词类型与启动类型之间的关键相互作用;这一发现对不同的编码和排除决策、动词语义的操作化(二分法/连续法)、分析框架(贝叶斯/频率论)以及——根据混合效应-多重宇宙分析——随机效应结构都具有鲁棒性。然而,这些发现并没有为语义效应的缺失提供证据(正如Messenger等人2012年的研究结果所争论的那样)。我们的结论是,这些和相关的发现最好由一个模型来解释,该模型包括词汇、范例级表征和多个更高抽象级别的表征。
{"title":"Is Passive Priming Really Impervious to Verb Semantics? A High-Powered Replication of Messenger Et al. (2012)","authors":"I. M. S. Darmasetiyawan, Katherine Messenger, Ben Ambridge","doi":"10.1525/collabra.31055","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1525/collabra.31055","url":null,"abstract":"The aim of the present study was to conduct a particularly stringent pre-registered in-vestigation of the claim that there exists a level of linguistic representation that “includes syntactic category information but not semantic information” (Branigan & Pickering, 2017: 8). As a test case, we focussed on the English passive; a construction for which previous findings have been somewhat contradictory. On the one hand, several studies using different methodologies have found an advantage for theme-experiencer passives (e.g., The girl was shocked by the tiger; and also agent-patient passives; e.g., The girl was hit by the tiger) over experiencer-theme passives (e.g., The girl was ignored by the tiger). On the other hand, Messenger et al. (2012) found no evidence that theme-experiencer and experiencer-theme passives vary in their propensity to prime production of agent-patient passives. We therefore conducted an online replication of Messen-ger et al (2012) with a pre-registered appropriately powered sample (N=240). Although a large and significant priming effect (i.e., an effect of prime sentence type) was ob-served, a Bayesian analysis yielded only weak/anecdotal evidence (BF=2.11) for the crucial interaction of verb type by prime type; a finding that was robust to different coding and exclusion decisions, operationalizations of verb semantics (dichoto-mous/continuous), analysis frameworks (Bayesian/frequentist) and – as per a mixed-effects-multiverse analyses – random effects structures. Nevertheless, these findings do no not provide evidence for the absence of semantic effects (as has been argued for the findings of Messenger et al, 2012). We conclude that these and related findings are best explained by a model that includes both lexical, exemplar-level representations and rep-resentations at multiple higher levels of abstraction.","PeriodicalId":45791,"journal":{"name":"Collabra-Psychology","volume":"1 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.5,"publicationDate":"2022-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"66875952","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
Hedging Bets in Linguistic Prediction: Younger and Older Adults Vary in the Breadth of Predictive Processing 语言预测中的对冲赌注:年轻人和老年人在预测处理的广度上有所不同
IF 2.5 3区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2022-01-01 DOI: 10.1525/collabra.36945
Katja I. Haeuser, J. Kray, Alexander Borovsky
Language processing is predictive in nature, but it is unknown whether language users generate multiple predictions about upcoming content simultaneously or whether spreading activation from one pre-activated word facilitates other words downstream. Simultaneously, developmental accounts of predictive processing simultaneously highlight potential tension among spreading activation vs. multiple activation accounts. We used self-paced reading to investigate if younger and older readers of German generate (multiple) graded predictions about the grammatical gender of nouns. Gradedness in predictions was operationalized as the difference in cloze probability between the most likely and second-most likely continuation that could complete a sentence. Sentences with a greater probabilistic difference were considered as imbalanced and more biased towards one gender. Sentences with lower probabilistic differences were considered to be more balanced towards multiple genders. Both young and older adults engaged in predictive processing. However, only younger adults activated multiple predictions, with slower reading times (RTs) when gender representations were balanced, but facilitation when one gender was more likely than others. In contrast, older adults’ RTs did not pattern with imbalance but merely with predictability, showing that, while able to generate predictions based on context, older adults did not predict multiple gender continuations. Hence, our findings suggest that (younger) language users generate graded predictions about upcoming content, by weighing possible sentence continuations according to their difference in cloze probability. Compared to younger adults, older adults’ predictions are reduced in scope. The results provide novel theoretical insights into the developmental mechanisms involved in predictive processing.
语言处理本质上是预测性的,但语言用户是否同时对即将到来的内容产生多个预测,或者从一个预激活的单词传播激活是否会促进下游的其他单词,这一点尚不清楚。同时,预测处理的发展理论同时强调了扩张性激活理论与多重激活理论之间的潜在矛盾。我们使用自定节奏阅读来调查年轻和年长的德语读者是否对名词的语法性别产生(多重)分级预测。预测的等级被操作化为最可能和第二可能完成句子的续句之间完形概率的差异。概率差异较大的句子被认为是不平衡的,更偏向于一种性别。概率差异较小的句子被认为对多性别更平衡。年轻人和老年人都参与了预测性处理。然而,只有年轻人激活了多个预测,当性别表征平衡时,阅读时间(RTs)较慢,但当一种性别比其他性别更容易时,阅读时间(RTs)较慢。相比之下,老年人的RTs不存在不平衡模式,而仅仅具有可预测性,这表明,尽管老年人能够基于上下文进行预测,但他们无法预测多性别延续。因此,我们的研究结果表明,(年轻的)语言使用者根据完形概率的差异来权衡可能的句子延续,从而对即将到来的内容产生分级预测。与年轻人相比,老年人的预测范围更小。研究结果为预测加工的发展机制提供了新的理论见解。
{"title":"Hedging Bets in Linguistic Prediction: Younger and Older Adults Vary in the Breadth of Predictive Processing","authors":"Katja I. Haeuser, J. Kray, Alexander Borovsky","doi":"10.1525/collabra.36945","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1525/collabra.36945","url":null,"abstract":"Language processing is predictive in nature, but it is unknown whether language users generate multiple predictions about upcoming content simultaneously or whether spreading activation from one pre-activated word facilitates other words downstream. Simultaneously, developmental accounts of predictive processing simultaneously highlight potential tension among spreading activation vs. multiple activation accounts. We used self-paced reading to investigate if younger and older readers of German generate (multiple) graded predictions about the grammatical gender of nouns. Gradedness in predictions was operationalized as the difference in cloze probability between the most likely and second-most likely continuation that could complete a sentence. Sentences with a greater probabilistic difference were considered as imbalanced and more biased towards one gender. Sentences with lower probabilistic differences were considered to be more balanced towards multiple genders. Both young and older adults engaged in predictive processing. However, only younger adults activated multiple predictions, with slower reading times (RTs) when gender representations were balanced, but facilitation when one gender was more likely than others. In contrast, older adults’ RTs did not pattern with imbalance but merely with predictability, showing that, while able to generate predictions based on context, older adults did not predict multiple gender continuations. Hence, our findings suggest that (younger) language users generate graded predictions about upcoming content, by weighing possible sentence continuations according to their difference in cloze probability. Compared to younger adults, older adults’ predictions are reduced in scope. The results provide novel theoretical insights into the developmental mechanisms involved in predictive processing.","PeriodicalId":45791,"journal":{"name":"Collabra-Psychology","volume":"1 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.5,"publicationDate":"2022-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"66878718","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
How to Detect Concealed Crime Knowledge in Situations With Little Information Using the Forced Choice Test 如何利用强迫选择测验在信息少的情况下发现隐藏的犯罪知识
IF 2.5 3区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2022-01-01 DOI: 10.1525/collabra.37483
Robin Orthey, Ewout H. Meijer, E. Kooistra, N. Broers
The Forced Choice Test (FCT) can be used to detect concealed crime knowledge, but it requires more evidence than typically available from a crime to be constructed. We propose a method to repeat individual pieces of evidence to achieve the necessary test length, hence widening the practical applicability. According to our method, FCT trials are created so that on each trial examinees are presented with a novel and unique decision between two answer alternatives even if a specific piece of information is presented again. We argue that if the decision in each trial is unique, the properties and diagnosticity of a traditional FCT can be maintained. In experiment 1, we provide a proof of concept by comparing our novel method with a traditional FCT and demonstrate that an FCT with repeated presentation of the same evidence has diagnostic value (AUC = .69) albeit less so than a traditional FCT (AUC = .86). In experiment 2, we put our novel FCT to the test in a situation with insufficient information for a traditional FCT alongside the Concealed Information Test (CIT), which also detects concealed information but relies on psychophysiological indices. Both, the FCT (AUC = .81) and CIT (AUC = .83) were diagnostic and combining them increased the detection accuracy even further (AUC = .91). If replicated, our novel FCT increase practical applicability of the FCT in general and in conjunction with the CIT.
强制选择测试(FCT)可以用来检测隐藏的犯罪知识,但它需要比通常从犯罪中获得的证据更多的证据来构建。我们提出了一种方法来重复个别证据,以达到必要的测试长度,从而扩大了实际的适用性。根据我们的方法,创建了FCT试验,以便在每次试验中,即使再次呈现特定信息,考生也要在两个备选答案之间做出新颖而独特的决定。我们认为,如果每个试验中的决策是唯一的,传统FCT的性质和诊断能力可以保持不变。在实验1中,我们通过将我们的新方法与传统的FCT进行比较来提供概念证明,并证明重复呈现相同证据的FCT具有诊断价值(AUC = 0.69),尽管不如传统的FCT (AUC = 0.86)。在实验2中,我们将我们的新FCT与隐藏信息测试(CIT)一起在传统FCT信息不足的情况下进行测试,隐藏信息测试也检测隐藏信息,但依赖于心理生理指标。FCT (AUC = .81)和CIT (AUC = .83)均可诊断,两者结合可进一步提高检测准确性(AUC = .91)。如果能被复制,我们的新FCT将提高FCT的整体实用性,并与CIT相结合。
{"title":"How to Detect Concealed Crime Knowledge in Situations With Little Information Using the Forced Choice Test","authors":"Robin Orthey, Ewout H. Meijer, E. Kooistra, N. Broers","doi":"10.1525/collabra.37483","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1525/collabra.37483","url":null,"abstract":"The Forced Choice Test (FCT) can be used to detect concealed crime knowledge, but it requires more evidence than typically available from a crime to be constructed. We propose a method to repeat individual pieces of evidence to achieve the necessary test length, hence widening the practical applicability. According to our method, FCT trials are created so that on each trial examinees are presented with a novel and unique decision between two answer alternatives even if a specific piece of information is presented again. We argue that if the decision in each trial is unique, the properties and diagnosticity of a traditional FCT can be maintained. In experiment 1, we provide a proof of concept by comparing our novel method with a traditional FCT and demonstrate that an FCT with repeated presentation of the same evidence has diagnostic value (AUC = .69) albeit less so than a traditional FCT (AUC = .86). In experiment 2, we put our novel FCT to the test in a situation with insufficient information for a traditional FCT alongside the Concealed Information Test (CIT), which also detects concealed information but relies on psychophysiological indices. Both, the FCT (AUC = .81) and CIT (AUC = .83) were diagnostic and combining them increased the detection accuracy even further (AUC = .91). If replicated, our novel FCT increase practical applicability of the FCT in general and in conjunction with the CIT.","PeriodicalId":45791,"journal":{"name":"Collabra-Psychology","volume":"1 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.5,"publicationDate":"2022-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"66878763","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
A Metascientific Empirical Review of Cognitive Load Lie Detection 认知负荷测谎的元科学实证研究
IF 2.5 3区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2022-01-01 DOI: 10.1525/collabra.57508
D. A. Neequaye
This article examines the cognitive load lie detection hypothesis. The idea that lying is more challenging than telling the truth—thus, imposing cognitive load can exacerbate the challenge liars face and expose lies. I reviewed 24 publications to flag derivation chains authors employ to justify the hypothesis. The findings indicate that authors recycle the same set of justifications but not systematically. That state of the literature shields cognitive load lie detection from severe testing in two ways. There is no clear justification to focus on when wanting to nominate or design severe tests. And the justifications contain ambiguities that make it challenging to determine what would count as a severe test of the hypothesis. I illustrate those limitations and discuss the need to make cognitive load lie detection amenable to severe testing.
本文考察了认知负荷测谎假说。说谎比讲真话更具挑战性——因此,施加认知负荷会加剧说谎者面临的挑战,从而暴露谎言。我回顾了24篇出版物,以标记作者用来证明假设的衍生链。研究结果表明,作者重复使用了同样的一套理由,但不是系统地。这种文献状态从两个方面屏蔽了认知负荷测谎。当想要提名或设计严格的测试时,没有明确的理由来关注。而且,这些论证中包含模棱两可的内容,这使得确定什么样的论证才算是对假设的严格检验变得具有挑战性。我举例说明了这些局限性,并讨论了使认知负荷测谎适应严格测试的必要性。
{"title":"A Metascientific Empirical Review of Cognitive Load Lie Detection","authors":"D. A. Neequaye","doi":"10.1525/collabra.57508","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1525/collabra.57508","url":null,"abstract":"This article examines the cognitive load lie detection hypothesis. The idea that lying is more challenging than telling the truth—thus, imposing cognitive load can exacerbate the challenge liars face and expose lies. I reviewed 24 publications to flag derivation chains authors employ to justify the hypothesis. The findings indicate that authors recycle the same set of justifications but not systematically. That state of the literature shields cognitive load lie detection from severe testing in two ways. There is no clear justification to focus on when wanting to nominate or design severe tests. And the justifications contain ambiguities that make it challenging to determine what would count as a severe test of the hypothesis. I illustrate those limitations and discuss the need to make cognitive load lie detection amenable to severe testing.","PeriodicalId":45791,"journal":{"name":"Collabra-Psychology","volume":"1 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.5,"publicationDate":"2022-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"66879324","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
Serious Problems With Interpreting Rubber Hand “Illusion” Experiments 解释橡胶手“错觉”实验的严重问题
IF 2.5 3区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2022-01-01 DOI: 10.1525/collabra.32274
Warrick Roseboom, P. Lush
The rubber hand “illusion” (RHI), in which participants report experiences of ownership over a fake hand, appears to demonstrate that subjective ownership over one’s body can be easily disrupted. It was recently shown that existing methods of controlling for suggestion effects in RHI responding are invalid. It was also shown that propensity to agree with RHI ownership statements is correlated with trait phenomenological control (response to imaginative suggestion). There is currently disagreement regarding the extent to which this relationship may cofound interpretation of RHI measures. Here we present the results of simulated experiments to demonstrate that a relationship between trait phenomenological control and RHI responding of the size reported would fundamentally change the way existing RHI results must be interpreted. Using real participant data, each simulated experiment used a sample biased in selection for trait phenomenological control. We find that using experiment samples comprised only of participants higher in trait phenomenological control almost guarantees that an experiment provides evidence consistent with RHI. By contrast, samples comprised of only participants lower in trait phenomenological control find evidence for RHI only around half the time – and of greater concern, evidence specifically for “ownership” experience just 4% of the time. These findings clearly contradict claims that the magnitude of relationship between phenomenological control and RHI responding is a minor concern, demonstrating that the presence of participants higher in trait phenomenological control in a given RHI experiment sample is critical for finding evidence consistent with RHI. Further study and theorising regarding RHI (and related effects) must take into account the role that trait phenomenological control plays in participant experience and responses during RHI experiments.
在橡胶手“错觉”(RHI)中,参与者报告了对假手的所有权体验,这似乎表明,对一个人身体的主观所有权很容易被破坏。最近的研究表明,现有的控制RHI反应中暗示效应的方法是无效的。研究还表明,认同RHI所有权陈述的倾向与特质现象学控制(对想象建议的反应)相关。目前对于这种关系在多大程度上可能会影响对RHI测量的解释存在分歧。在这里,我们提出了模拟实验的结果,以证明性状现象学控制与报告的RHI反应大小之间的关系将从根本上改变现有RHI结果必须解释的方式。使用真实的参与者数据,每个模拟实验都使用一个样本偏差来选择性状现象学控制。我们发现,使用实验样本仅包括具有较高特质现象学控制的参与者,几乎可以保证实验提供与RHI一致的证据。相比之下,只有特质现象学控制水平较低的参与者的样本中,只有大约一半的时间发现了RHI的证据,而更值得关注的是,只有4%的时间发现了“所有权”经验的证据。这些发现明显反驳了现象学控制与RHI反应之间关系的大小是次要问题的说法,表明在给定的RHI实验样本中,具有较高特质现象学控制的参与者的存在对于找到与RHI一致的证据至关重要。关于RHI(及其相关效应)的进一步研究和理论化必须考虑到特质现象学控制在RHI实验中的参与者体验和反应中所起的作用。
{"title":"Serious Problems With Interpreting Rubber Hand “Illusion” Experiments","authors":"Warrick Roseboom, P. Lush","doi":"10.1525/collabra.32274","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1525/collabra.32274","url":null,"abstract":"The rubber hand “illusion” (RHI), in which participants report experiences of ownership over a fake hand, appears to demonstrate that subjective ownership over one’s body can be easily disrupted. It was recently shown that existing methods of controlling for suggestion effects in RHI responding are invalid. It was also shown that propensity to agree with RHI ownership statements is correlated with trait phenomenological control (response to imaginative suggestion). There is currently disagreement regarding the extent to which this relationship may cofound interpretation of RHI measures. Here we present the results of simulated experiments to demonstrate that a relationship between trait phenomenological control and RHI responding of the size reported would fundamentally change the way existing RHI results must be interpreted. Using real participant data, each simulated experiment used a sample biased in selection for trait phenomenological control. We find that using experiment samples comprised only of participants higher in trait phenomenological control almost guarantees that an experiment provides evidence consistent with RHI. By contrast, samples comprised of only participants lower in trait phenomenological control find evidence for RHI only around half the time – and of greater concern, evidence specifically for “ownership” experience just 4% of the time. These findings clearly contradict claims that the magnitude of relationship between phenomenological control and RHI responding is a minor concern, demonstrating that the presence of participants higher in trait phenomenological control in a given RHI experiment sample is critical for finding evidence consistent with RHI. Further study and theorising regarding RHI (and related effects) must take into account the role that trait phenomenological control plays in participant experience and responses during RHI experiments.","PeriodicalId":45791,"journal":{"name":"Collabra-Psychology","volume":"1 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.5,"publicationDate":"2022-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"66876189","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 5
Rewarding More Is Better for Soliciting Help, Yet More So for Cash Than for Goods: Revisiting and Reframing the Tale of Two Markets With Replications and Extensions of Heyman and Ariely (2004) 对寻求帮助给予更多回报更好,但对现金的回报比对商品的回报更大:通过复制和扩展海曼和艾瑞里的观点,重新审视和重构两个市场的故事
IF 2.5 3区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2022-01-01 DOI: 10.1525/collabra.32572
Hirotaka Imada, Wan Fei Chan, Yuk Ki Ng, Lee Hing Man, Mei Sze Wong, Boley Cheng, Gilad Feldman
Heyman and Ariely (2004) demonstrated that the expected effectiveness of soliciting help varied depending on the “market”, a money market represented by cash rewards versus a social market represented by goods as rewards. They showed that, as cash rewards increase, individuals expected others to be more willing to help, yet, when offering social goods as rewards such as candy, expected willingness to help was insensitive to rewards’ monetary worth. We conducted two pre-registered replication studies (total: N = 3302, MTurk/Prolific) of Study 1 in Heyman and Ariely (2004) and found support for one of their main claims that people are more sensitive to worth when the reward is cash than goods. However, the rewards’ monetary worth impacted expected willingness to help even in social markets, deviating from the original findings. Extensions further compared between-subject and within-subject designs, examined perceived affect (joy and regret), and added a new control condition. We concluded that higher compensation is generally perceived as better when soliciting help, yet more so for the money market cash rewards than for the social market goods rewards. All materials, data, and code are provided on https://osf.io/y9p7u/
Heyman和Ariely(2004)证明,请求帮助的预期有效性取决于“市场”,即以现金奖励为代表的货币市场与以商品作为奖励为代表的社会市场。他们发现,随着现金奖励的增加,个体期望他人更愿意提供帮助,然而,当提供糖果等社会商品作为奖励时,预期的帮助意愿对奖励的货币价值不敏感。我们在Heyman和Ariely(2004)的研究1中进行了两个预先注册的复制研究(总数:N = 3302, MTurk/多产),并发现支持他们的一个主要主张,即当奖励是现金而不是商品时,人们对价值更敏感。然而,即使在社交市场中,奖励的金钱价值也会影响人们的助人意愿,这与最初的研究结果有所不同。扩展进一步比较了受试者之间和受试者内部的设计,检查了感知情感(喜悦和遗憾),并增加了一个新的控制条件。我们的结论是,在寻求帮助时,更高的报酬通常被认为是更好的,但货币市场的现金奖励比社会市场的商品奖励更有效。所有材料、数据和代码都在https://osf.io/y9p7u/上提供
{"title":"Rewarding More Is Better for Soliciting Help, Yet More So for Cash Than for Goods: Revisiting and Reframing the Tale of Two Markets With Replications and Extensions of Heyman and Ariely (2004)","authors":"Hirotaka Imada, Wan Fei Chan, Yuk Ki Ng, Lee Hing Man, Mei Sze Wong, Boley Cheng, Gilad Feldman","doi":"10.1525/collabra.32572","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1525/collabra.32572","url":null,"abstract":"Heyman and Ariely (2004) demonstrated that the expected effectiveness of soliciting help varied depending on the “market”, a money market represented by cash rewards versus a social market represented by goods as rewards. They showed that, as cash rewards increase, individuals expected others to be more willing to help, yet, when offering social goods as rewards such as candy, expected willingness to help was insensitive to rewards’ monetary worth. We conducted two pre-registered replication studies (total: N = 3302, MTurk/Prolific) of Study 1 in Heyman and Ariely (2004) and found support for one of their main claims that people are more sensitive to worth when the reward is cash than goods. However, the rewards’ monetary worth impacted expected willingness to help even in social markets, deviating from the original findings. Extensions further compared between-subject and within-subject designs, examined perceived affect (joy and regret), and added a new control condition. We concluded that higher compensation is generally perceived as better when soliciting help, yet more so for the money market cash rewards than for the social market goods rewards. All materials, data, and code are provided on https://osf.io/y9p7u/","PeriodicalId":45791,"journal":{"name":"Collabra-Psychology","volume":"1 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.5,"publicationDate":"2022-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"66876747","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
期刊
Collabra-Psychology
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1