首页 > 最新文献

Studia Psychologica最新文献

英文 中文
Kontrowersje wokół pojęcia trafności
IF 1.2 4区 心理学 Q3 Psychology Pub Date : 2021-08-04 DOI: 10.21697/sp.2021.21.1.03
J. Rytel
W artykule zrelacjonowano zmieniające się na przestrzeni ponad 100 lat koncepcje pojęcia trafności pomiaru testowego. Aktualnie pojęcie trafności odnosi się do stopnia, w jakim dane empiryczne oraz teoria uzasadniają interpretację wyników testowych w zakładanym kierunku (American Educational Research Association, American Psychological Association, National Council on Measurement in Education [AERA, APA, NCME], 2007, s. 31). Przedstawiono 5 podstawowych źródeł danych dotyczących trafności oraz problemy związane z integracją dowodów na rzecz trafności w spójną argumentację. Podkreślono użyteczność zaproponowanego przez Kane’a podejścia do walidacji opartego na argumentacji, odwołującego się do logiki nieformalnej i struktury argumentu wprowadzonej przez Toulmina. Omówiono także różnice stanowisk zajmowanych przez badaczy w odniesieniu do 2 podstawowych kwestii: czemu przysługuje trafność i jaki jest właściwy sposób jej ustalania?  
{"title":"Kontrowersje wokół pojęcia trafności","authors":"J. Rytel","doi":"10.21697/sp.2021.21.1.03","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.21697/sp.2021.21.1.03","url":null,"abstract":"W artykule zrelacjonowano zmieniające się na przestrzeni ponad 100 lat koncepcje pojęcia trafności pomiaru testowego. Aktualnie pojęcie trafności odnosi się do stopnia, w jakim dane empiryczne oraz teoria uzasadniają interpretację wyników testowych w zakładanym kierunku (American Educational Research Association, American Psychological Association, National Council on Measurement in Education [AERA, APA, NCME], 2007, s. 31). Przedstawiono 5 podstawowych źródeł danych dotyczących trafności oraz problemy związane z integracją dowodów na rzecz trafności w spójną argumentację. Podkreślono użyteczność zaproponowanego przez Kane’a podejścia do walidacji opartego na argumentacji, odwołującego się do logiki nieformalnej i struktury argumentu wprowadzonej przez Toulmina. Omówiono także różnice stanowisk zajmowanych przez badaczy w odniesieniu do 2 podstawowych kwestii: czemu przysługuje trafność i jaki jest właściwy sposób jej ustalania?  ","PeriodicalId":45798,"journal":{"name":"Studia Psychologica","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.2,"publicationDate":"2021-08-04","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"80959206","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
The role of personality, conspiracy mentality, REBT irrational beliefs, and adult attachment in COVID-19 related health behaviors 人格、阴谋心理、REBT非理性信念和成人依恋在COVID-19相关健康行为中的作用
IF 1.2 4区 心理学 Q3 Psychology Pub Date : 2021-06-23 DOI: 10.31234/osf.io/q2nye
S. Stanković, L. Lazarević, G. Knežević
We investigated irrational beliefs defined in rational-emotive cognitive-behavior therapy (REBT), attachment anxiety and avoidance, and conspiracy mentality as mediators of the relationship between HEXACO and Disintegration traits and COVID-19 health behaviors. Structural equation modeling on a sample of 287 participants, showed that Disintegration (D) was related to all mediating variables, highlighting the importance of D in the emergence of irrational beliefs. Conspiracy mentality mediated the effect of D in low adherence to recommended health behaviors - RHB , negative vaccination behavior, and greater use of pseudoscientific practices - PSP . Attachment anxiety mediated the relationship between high D, high Emotionality (E), and low Honesty (H) and lower adherence to RHB. Higher adherence to RHB predicted positive vaccination behavior, whilst there was no relationship between PSP use and vaccination behavior. REBT irrational beliefs and attachment avoidance were not found to be significant mediators of the relationship between personality traits and COVID-19 health behaviors.
我们研究了理性情绪认知行为疗法(REBT)中定义的非理性信念、依恋焦虑和回避以及阴谋心理在HEXACO和解体特征与COVID-19健康行为之间的中介作用。对287名参与者的样本进行结构方程建模,表明解体(D)与所有中介变量相关,突出了D在非理性信念出现中的重要性。阴谋心理介导了D在低遵守推荐健康行为(RHB)、消极疫苗接种行为和更多使用伪科学行为(PSP)中的作用。依恋焦虑在高D、高情绪(E)、低诚实(H)与低依从性之间起中介作用。较高的RHB依从性预测了积极的疫苗接种行为,而PSP使用与疫苗接种行为之间没有关系。REBT非理性信念和依恋回避并不是人格特质与COVID-19健康行为之间关系的显著中介。
{"title":"The role of personality, conspiracy mentality, REBT irrational beliefs, and adult attachment in COVID-19 related health behaviors","authors":"S. Stanković, L. Lazarević, G. Knežević","doi":"10.31234/osf.io/q2nye","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/q2nye","url":null,"abstract":"We investigated irrational beliefs defined in rational-emotive cognitive-behavior therapy (REBT), attachment anxiety and avoidance, and conspiracy mentality as mediators of the relationship between HEXACO and Disintegration traits and COVID-19 health behaviors. Structural equation modeling on a sample of 287 participants, showed that Disintegration (D) was related to all mediating variables, highlighting the importance of D in the emergence of irrational beliefs. Conspiracy mentality mediated the effect of D in low adherence to recommended health behaviors - RHB , negative vaccination behavior, and greater use of pseudoscientific practices - PSP . Attachment anxiety mediated the relationship between high D, high Emotionality (E), and low Honesty (H) and lower adherence to RHB. Higher adherence to RHB predicted positive vaccination behavior, whilst there was no relationship between PSP use and vaccination behavior. REBT irrational beliefs and attachment avoidance were not found to be significant mediators of the relationship between personality traits and COVID-19 health behaviors.","PeriodicalId":45798,"journal":{"name":"Studia Psychologica","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.2,"publicationDate":"2021-06-23","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"86879166","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 7
Does Action vs. State Orientation Really Matter in the Susceptibility to Sunk Cost Fallacy? A Conceptual Replication Study 在沉没成本谬误的易感性中,行动取向和状态取向真的重要吗?概念复制研究
IF 1.2 4区 心理学 Q3 Psychology Pub Date : 2021-06-01 DOI: 10.31577/SP.2021.02.823
Miroslava Galasová, Matúš Grežo
The theory of action versus state orientation suggests that state-oriented people are more susceptible to sunk cost fallacy than action-oriented people because they ruminate about past costs and are reluctant to change their course of actions. However, research on the role of action versus state orientation in sunk cost fallacy is fairly limited. Therefore, the present paper aims to conceptually replicate the seminal study by van Putten et al. (2010) and verify whether action versus state orientation really matters in the susceptibility to sunk cost fallacy. We also examined the role of gender and goal internalization in the susceptibility to sunk cost fallacy. Participants (N = 205) filled an Action Control Scale and solved two sunk cost fallacy tasks in two experimental conditions. In the intrapersonal condition, the sunk costs belonged to a decision-maker. In the intrapersonal condition, an investor was not identical with the decision-maker. Eventually, our study failed to replicate the results of van Putten et al. (2010). Action versus state orientation did not predict the susceptibility to sunk cost fallacy. Moreover, neither gender nor internalization moderated the relationship between action versus state orientation and susceptibility to sunk cost fallacy. We suggest further replications to examine the roles of reluctance to change and rumination in the relationship between action versus state orientation and susceptibility to sunk cost fallacy. Our findings also highlight the importance of high-powered replications that are an essential part of good research practice.
行动与状态导向的理论表明,状态导向的人比行动导向的人更容易受到沉没成本谬论的影响,因为他们反复思考过去的成本,不愿意改变他们的行动方针。然而,关于行动与状态取向在沉没成本谬误中的作用的研究相当有限。因此,本文旨在从概念上复制van Putten等人(2010)的开创性研究,并验证行动与状态取向在沉没成本谬误的易感性中是否真的重要。我们还研究了性别和目标内化在沉没成本谬误易感性中的作用。205名被试在两种实验条件下填写了一份行动控制量表,并解决了两项沉没成本谬误任务。在个人条件下,沉没成本属于决策者。在内省条件下,投资者与决策者并不相同。最终,我们的研究未能复制van Putten et al.(2010)的结果。行动取向与状态取向并不能预测沉没成本谬误的易感性。此外,性别和内化都没有调节行动与状态取向和沉没成本谬误易感性之间的关系。我们建议进一步的重复研究,以检验不愿改变和反思在行动与状态取向和沉没成本谬误易感性之间的关系中的作用。我们的发现还强调了高强度复制的重要性,这是良好研究实践的重要组成部分。
{"title":"Does Action vs. State Orientation Really Matter in the Susceptibility to Sunk Cost Fallacy? A Conceptual Replication Study","authors":"Miroslava Galasová, Matúš Grežo","doi":"10.31577/SP.2021.02.823","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.31577/SP.2021.02.823","url":null,"abstract":"The theory of action versus state orientation suggests that state-oriented people are more susceptible to sunk cost fallacy than action-oriented people because they ruminate about past costs and are reluctant to change their course of actions. However, research on the role of action versus state orientation in sunk cost fallacy is fairly limited. Therefore, the present paper aims to conceptually replicate the seminal study by van Putten et al. (2010) and verify whether action versus state orientation really matters in the susceptibility to sunk cost fallacy. We also examined the role of gender and goal internalization in the susceptibility to sunk cost fallacy. Participants (N = 205) filled an Action Control Scale and solved two sunk cost fallacy tasks in two experimental conditions. In the intrapersonal condition, the sunk costs belonged to a decision-maker. In the intrapersonal condition, an investor was not identical with the decision-maker. Eventually, our study failed to replicate the results of van Putten et al. (2010). Action versus state orientation did not predict the susceptibility to sunk cost fallacy. Moreover, neither gender nor internalization moderated the relationship between action versus state orientation and susceptibility to sunk cost fallacy. We suggest further replications to examine the roles of reluctance to change and rumination in the relationship between action versus state orientation and susceptibility to sunk cost fallacy. Our findings also highlight the importance of high-powered replications that are an essential part of good research practice.","PeriodicalId":45798,"journal":{"name":"Studia Psychologica","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.2,"publicationDate":"2021-06-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"78945040","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Cognitive Predictors of Delay Discounting in Monetary Choices 货币选择中延迟贴现的认知预测因素
IF 1.2 4区 心理学 Q3 Psychology Pub Date : 2021-06-01 DOI: 10.31577/SP.2021.02.817
V. Bačová, Jakub Šrol
Delay discounting, the tendency to choose a smaller-sooner reward over a larger-later reward, has been conceptualized either as a personal preference or as a rational thinking component. In this study (N = 397), the associations between monetary delay discounting – constructed as a rational thinking task – and cognitive individual difference measures were examined. Participants with higher general cognitive ability, cognitive reflection, scientific reasoning, and objective numeracy had a weaker tendency to discount delayed rewards, the opposite was true for those with higher intuitive thinking disposition and bias susceptibility. Bias susceptibility predicted delay discounting over and above all other cognitive predictors. The results partially support the assumption about a common basis of delay discounting and susceptibility to cognitive biases (as a rational thinking indicator). Because of the relatively low explained variance in delay discounting by cognitive variables, however, ample room is left for other potential predictors in the monetary delay discounting tasks.
延迟折扣,即选择较小的较早奖励而不是较大的较晚奖励的倾向,已经被概念化为个人偏好或理性思维的组成部分。在这项研究中(N = 397),货币延迟贴现-构建为理性思维任务-和认知个体差异测量之间的关系进行了检验。一般认知能力、认知反思能力、科学推理能力和客观计算能力较强的参与者对延迟奖励的折扣倾向较弱,而直觉思维倾向和偏见敏感性较高的参与者对延迟奖励的折扣倾向较弱。偏见敏感性预测延迟折扣高于所有其他认知预测。研究结果部分支持了延迟折扣和认知偏差易感性(作为理性思维指标)的共同基础的假设。然而,由于认知变量对延迟贴现的解释方差相对较低,因此在货币延迟贴现任务中,为其他潜在的预测因素留下了充足的空间。
{"title":"Cognitive Predictors of Delay Discounting in Monetary Choices","authors":"V. Bačová, Jakub Šrol","doi":"10.31577/SP.2021.02.817","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.31577/SP.2021.02.817","url":null,"abstract":"Delay discounting, the tendency to choose a smaller-sooner reward over a larger-later reward, has been conceptualized either as a personal preference or as a rational thinking component. In this study (N = 397), the associations between monetary delay discounting – constructed as a rational thinking task – and cognitive individual difference measures were examined. Participants with higher general cognitive ability, cognitive reflection, scientific reasoning, and objective numeracy had a weaker tendency to discount delayed rewards, the opposite was true for those with higher intuitive thinking disposition and bias susceptibility. Bias susceptibility predicted delay discounting over and above all other cognitive predictors. The results partially support the assumption about a common basis of delay discounting and susceptibility to cognitive biases (as a rational thinking indicator). Because of the relatively low explained variance in delay discounting by cognitive variables, however, ample room is left for other potential predictors in the monetary delay discounting tasks.","PeriodicalId":45798,"journal":{"name":"Studia Psychologica","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.2,"publicationDate":"2021-06-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"84894990","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 5
The Analytic Cognitive Style and Conspiracy Mentality as Predictors of Conspiracy Beliefs 分析性认知风格与阴谋心理作为阴谋信念的预测因子
IF 1.2 4区 心理学 Q3 Psychology Pub Date : 2021-06-01 DOI: 10.31577/sp.2021.02.819
Eva Ballová Mikušková
{"title":"The Analytic Cognitive Style and Conspiracy Mentality as Predictors of Conspiracy Beliefs","authors":"Eva Ballová Mikušková","doi":"10.31577/sp.2021.02.819","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.31577/sp.2021.02.819","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":45798,"journal":{"name":"Studia Psychologica","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.2,"publicationDate":"2021-06-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"73044710","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 3
Maladaptive Personality Traits, Religiosity and Spirituality as Predictors of Epistemically Unfounded Beliefs 不适应的人格特征,宗教信仰和灵性作为认识论上没有根据的信仰的预测因素
IF 1.2 4区 心理学 Q3 Psychology Pub Date : 2021-05-19 DOI: 10.31577/SP.2021.02.820
Peter Teličák, P. Halama
The present research focuses on the question whether spirituality, religiosity and maladaptive personality traits, as measured by the PID-5 (antagonism, psychoticism, disinhibition, negative affectivity, detachment), predict epistemologically unfounded beliefs (EUB). The sample included 829 participants recruited through social networks (58% women, mean age 29,98 years). EUB was measured by the Scale of Epistemologically Unfounded Beliefs (Halama, 2019b), which measures three types of EUB: conspiracy, pseudoscientific, and paranormal beliefs. Pathological personality traits were measured by the Short Personality Questionnaire for DSM-5 (PID5-BF, Kruger et al, 2013), spirituality by the Questionnaire of Daily Spirituality (Underwood, 2011) and religiosity by The Religiosity Questionnaire (Storch et al., 2004). The results showed that especially psychoticism is a positive predictor of all EUBs. Spirituality and religiosity predicted only paranormal beliefs. Results confirmed maladaptive personality traits, religiosity, and spirituality can play a significant role in EUB and should be taken into account when considering sources of EUB at the individual level.
本研究主要探讨了由PID-5(对抗性、精神病性、去抑制性、负性情感、超然性)测量的灵性、宗教信仰和适应不良人格特征是否能预测认识论上无根据的信念(EUB)。样本包括通过社交网络招募的829名参与者(58%为女性,平均年龄29岁,98岁)。EUB是通过认识论上没有根据的信仰量表(Halama, 2019b)来衡量的,该量表测量了三种类型的EUB:阴谋、伪科学和超自然信仰。病态人格特征采用DSM-5短人格问卷(PID5-BF, Kruger et al., 2013),精神性采用日常精神性问卷(Underwood, 2011),宗教虔诚度采用宗教虔诚度问卷(Storch et al., 2004)。结果表明,特别是精神病是所有eub的积极预测因子。灵性和宗教信仰只能预言超自然的信仰。结果证实,适应不良的人格特征、宗教信仰和灵性在EUB中起着重要作用,在考虑个体层面的EUB来源时应予以考虑。
{"title":"Maladaptive Personality Traits, Religiosity and Spirituality as Predictors of Epistemically Unfounded Beliefs","authors":"Peter Teličák, P. Halama","doi":"10.31577/SP.2021.02.820","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.31577/SP.2021.02.820","url":null,"abstract":"The present research focuses on the question whether spirituality, religiosity and maladaptive personality traits, as measured by the PID-5 (antagonism, psychoticism, disinhibition, negative affectivity, detachment), predict epistemologically unfounded beliefs (EUB). The sample included 829 participants recruited through social networks (58% women, mean age 29,98 years). EUB was measured by the Scale of Epistemologically Unfounded Beliefs (Halama, 2019b), which measures three types of EUB: conspiracy, pseudoscientific, and paranormal beliefs. Pathological personality traits were measured by the Short Personality Questionnaire for DSM-5 (PID5-BF, Kruger et al, 2013), spirituality by the Questionnaire of Daily Spirituality (Underwood, 2011) and religiosity by The Religiosity Questionnaire (Storch et al., 2004). The results showed that especially psychoticism is a positive predictor of all EUBs. Spirituality and religiosity predicted only paranormal beliefs. Results confirmed maladaptive personality traits, religiosity, and spirituality can play a significant role in EUB and should be taken into account when considering sources of EUB at the individual level.","PeriodicalId":45798,"journal":{"name":"Studia Psychologica","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.2,"publicationDate":"2021-05-19","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"88305864","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 2
Mindware Instantiation as a Predictor of Logical Intuitions in Cognitive Reflection Test 认知反射测试中思维实例化对逻辑直觉的预测作用
IF 1.2 4区 心理学 Q3 Psychology Pub Date : 2021-05-18 DOI: 10.31577/SP.2021.02.822
Roman Burič, Ľubica Konrádová
Following the growing body of evidence suggesting that substantial individual differences in reasoning exist already at early stages of the reasoning process and that reasoners might be able to produce logical intuitions, the model of mindware automatization posits that the mindware acquired to the extent that it is fully automatized can cue the logically correct type 1 response. In this study, we asked participants to solve the Cognitive Reflection Test (CRT) presented under the two-response paradigm. Among individual difference factors, we measured mindware instantiation and conflict detection efficiency. These variables explained approximately 10% of the variance in the accuracy of intuitive answers. We also observed that in more than half of cases, the correct response was already correct at the initial response stage. These results are in line with the theoretical model of mindware automatization to a large extent and raise a question about the main attribute of the CRT.
随着越来越多的证据表明,在推理过程的早期阶段,推理中已经存在实质性的个体差异,推理者可能能够产生逻辑直觉,心智自动化模型假定,获得的心智在完全自动化的程度上可以提示逻辑上正确的第一类反应。在本研究中,我们要求被试解决双反应范式下的认知反思测试(CRT)。在个体差异因素中,我们测量了思维实例化和冲突检测效率。这些变量解释了直觉答案准确度上大约10%的差异。我们还观察到,在超过一半的病例中,正确的反应在初始反应阶段已经是正确的。这些结果在很大程度上符合思维自动化的理论模型,并对CRT的主要属性提出了质疑。
{"title":"Mindware Instantiation as a Predictor of Logical Intuitions in Cognitive Reflection Test","authors":"Roman Burič, Ľubica Konrádová","doi":"10.31577/SP.2021.02.822","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.31577/SP.2021.02.822","url":null,"abstract":"Following the growing body of evidence suggesting that substantial individual differences in reasoning exist already at early stages of the reasoning process and that reasoners might be able to produce logical intuitions, the model of mindware automatization posits that the mindware acquired to the extent that it is fully automatized can cue the logically correct type 1 response. In this study, we asked participants to solve the Cognitive Reflection Test (CRT) presented under the two-response paradigm. Among individual difference factors, we measured mindware instantiation and conflict detection efficiency. These variables explained approximately 10% of the variance in the accuracy of intuitive answers. We also observed that in more than half of cases, the correct response was already correct at the initial response stage. These results are in line with the theoretical model of mindware automatization to a large extent and raise a question about the main attribute of the CRT.","PeriodicalId":45798,"journal":{"name":"Studia Psychologica","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.2,"publicationDate":"2021-05-18","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"88494780","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 5
Everybody Bullshits Sometimes: Relationships of Bullshitting Frequency, Overconfidence and Myside Bias in the Topic of Migration 每个人都有扯淡的时候:在移民话题中,扯淡频率、过度自信和自我偏见的关系
IF 1.2 4区 心理学 Q3 Psychology Pub Date : 2021-05-18 DOI: 10.31577/SP.2021.02.818
V. Čavojová, I. Brezina
Current research on bullshit has shifted its focus from the recipient of bullshit to the producer of bullshit; this trend being reflected in the new Bullshitting frequency scale (Littrell et al., 2020) measuring persuasive and evasive bullshitting. The aim of our study was to validate the scale on the Slovak population and to examine the relationship between persuasive and evasive bullshitting behavior, overconfidence and myside bias in the context of the topic of migration. Six hundred and sixty-six Slovak adults (52.7% men, Mage = 41.84) participated in an online study. The two-factor structure of BFS was confirmed. The results showed that people high in persuasive bullshitting (“persuasive bullshitters”), after controlling for evasive bullshitting, felt they had more knowledge about migration, but they also showed more myside bias. Similarly, people high in evasive bullshitting (“evasive bullshitters”), after controlling for persuasive bullshitting, felt they had less knowledge about migration and tended to underestimate their knowledge. Contrary to our expectations, correlation between overconfidence and persuasive bullshitting disappeared when evasive bullshitting was controlled for, and it seems that negative the correlation was caused by evasive bullshitters being underconfident. Our results further expand the knowledge about cognitive characteristics of bullshitters and support the distinction between the two kinds of bullshitting behavior, which has implications for political debates as well.
目前对屁话的研究已经从屁话的接受者转向了屁话的制造者;这一趋势反映在新的胡扯频率量表(Littrell et al., 2020)中,该量表测量了说服性和回避性的胡扯。我们研究的目的是验证斯洛伐克人口的规模,并检查在移民主题背景下说服和回避扯淡行为,过度自信和我的偏见之间的关系。666名斯洛伐克成年人(52.7%为男性,法师= 41.84)参与了一项在线研究。确定了BFS的双因子结构。结果显示,在控制了闪烁其词的胡扯之后,那些高说服力的人(“有说服力的胡扯者”)觉得他们对移民有更多的了解,但他们也表现出更多的自我偏见。同样,在控制了有说服力的胡扯之后,那些回避性胡扯的人(“逃避性胡扯者”)觉得他们对移民的了解更少,并且倾向于低估他们的知识。与我们的预期相反,当规避性的扯淡被控制后,过度自信与劝导性的扯淡之间的相关性消失了,似乎是由于规避性的扯淡不自信导致了负相关。我们的研究结果进一步扩展了关于扯淡者认知特征的知识,并支持两种扯淡行为之间的区别,这对政治辩论也有影响。
{"title":"Everybody Bullshits Sometimes: Relationships of Bullshitting Frequency, Overconfidence and Myside Bias in the Topic of Migration","authors":"V. Čavojová, I. Brezina","doi":"10.31577/SP.2021.02.818","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.31577/SP.2021.02.818","url":null,"abstract":"Current research on bullshit has shifted its focus from the recipient of bullshit to the producer of bullshit; this trend being reflected in the new Bullshitting frequency scale (Littrell et al., 2020) measuring persuasive and evasive bullshitting. The aim of our study was to validate the scale on the Slovak population and to examine the relationship between persuasive and evasive bullshitting behavior, overconfidence and myside bias in the context of the topic of migration. Six hundred and sixty-six Slovak adults (52.7% men, Mage = 41.84) participated in an online study. The two-factor structure of BFS was confirmed. The results showed that people high in persuasive bullshitting (“persuasive bullshitters”), after controlling for evasive bullshitting, felt they had more knowledge about migration, but they also showed more myside bias. Similarly, people high in evasive bullshitting (“evasive bullshitters”), after controlling for persuasive bullshitting, felt they had less knowledge about migration and tended to underestimate their knowledge. Contrary to our expectations, correlation between overconfidence and persuasive bullshitting disappeared when evasive bullshitting was controlled for, and it seems that negative the correlation was caused by evasive bullshitters being underconfident. Our results further expand the knowledge about cognitive characteristics of bullshitters and support the distinction between the two kinds of bullshitting behavior, which has implications for political debates as well.","PeriodicalId":45798,"journal":{"name":"Studia Psychologica","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.2,"publicationDate":"2021-05-18","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"76468470","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
Paranoid and conspiracy beliefs: The role of anxiety and life satisfaction 偏执和阴谋信念:焦虑和生活满意度的作用
IF 1.2 4区 心理学 Q3 Psychology Pub Date : 2021-04-02 DOI: 10.31234/OSF.IO/ZW65S
Veronika Pekárová
This study aimed to examine the relationship between paranoid and conspiracy beliefs and how these beliefs further relate to anxiety-trait, anxiety disorders, and satisfaction with life. The research was attended by 814 participants who were administered the Paranoia Scale, the Slovak Conspiracy Belief Scale, the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory, and the Satisfaction With Life Scale. The results suggest that paranoid beliefs were associated with conspiracy beliefs, anxiety-trait, and life satisfaction. All of the variables together accounted for almost 40% of the variance of paranoid beliefs. In addition, the presence of anxiety disorders had a relatively strong effect on the level of paranoid beliefs. However, life satisfaction and anxiety-trait were not related to conspiracy beliefs. These results suggest the more attention should be drawn to mental health as both anxiety level and life satisfaction, along with conspiracy beliefs significantly predicted paranoid beliefs.
本研究旨在探讨偏执和阴谋信念之间的关系,以及这些信念如何进一步与焦虑特质、焦虑障碍和生活满意度相关。共有814名参与者参加了研究,他们分别接受了偏执量表、斯洛伐克阴谋信念量表、状态-特质焦虑量表和生活满意度量表。结果表明,偏执信念与阴谋信念、焦虑特质和生活满意度有关。所有这些变量加起来占偏执信念方差的近40%。此外,焦虑障碍的存在对偏执信念的水平有相对较强的影响。然而,生活满意度和焦虑特质与阴谋信念无关。这些结果表明,应该更多地关注心理健康,因为焦虑水平和生活满意度以及阴谋信念都能显著预测偏执信念。
{"title":"Paranoid and conspiracy beliefs: The role of anxiety and life satisfaction","authors":"Veronika Pekárová","doi":"10.31234/OSF.IO/ZW65S","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.31234/OSF.IO/ZW65S","url":null,"abstract":"This study aimed to examine the relationship between paranoid and conspiracy beliefs and how these beliefs further relate to anxiety-trait, anxiety disorders, and satisfaction with life. The research was attended by 814 participants who were administered the Paranoia Scale, the Slovak Conspiracy Belief Scale, the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory, and the Satisfaction With Life Scale. The results suggest that paranoid beliefs were associated with conspiracy beliefs, anxiety-trait, and life satisfaction. All of the variables together accounted for almost 40% of the variance of paranoid beliefs. In addition, the presence of anxiety disorders had a relatively strong effect on the level of paranoid beliefs. However, life satisfaction and anxiety-trait were not related to conspiracy beliefs. These results suggest the more attention should be drawn to mental health as both anxiety level and life satisfaction, along with conspiracy beliefs significantly predicted paranoid beliefs.","PeriodicalId":45798,"journal":{"name":"Studia Psychologica","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.2,"publicationDate":"2021-04-02","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"87623133","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 2
The Role of the Inhibition of Natural Number Based Reasoning and Strategy Switch Cost in a Fraction Comparison Task 基于自然数的推理抑制和策略转换代价在分数比较任务中的作用
IF 1.2 4区 心理学 Q3 Psychology Pub Date : 2021-03-15 DOI: 10.31577/SP.2021.01.814
J. Hoof, E. Ceulemans, W. Dooren
Previous research amply showed the importance of a good fraction understanding but also people’s lack of fraction understanding. It is therefore important to investigate the cognitive processes that underlie reasoning with fractions. The present study investigated the role of inhibition and switch costs in fraction comparison tasks. Participants solved a fraction comparison task that alternated between 4 items congruent and 4 items incongruent with natural number reasoning. This allowed to not only investigate congruency switch effects, but also inhibition, given that inhibition was experimentally increased by the prolonged exposure to incongruent trials. Based on data of seventh graders, the present study showed that inhibition does not only play a role in learners’ general mathematics achievement, but also in specific areas of mathematics, such as fractions. Moreover, a switch cost was found in the lower accuracy rates and higher reaction times needed to correctly solve switch items compared to non-switch items.
以往的研究充分表明了良好的分数理解的重要性,但也表明了人们对分数理解的不足。因此,研究分数推理背后的认知过程是很重要的。本研究探讨了抑制和转换成本在分数比较任务中的作用。参与者解决了一个分数比较任务,在自然数推理的4个项目一致和4个项目不一致之间交替进行。这不仅可以研究一致性转换效应,还可以研究抑制作用,因为在实验中,长时间暴露于不一致的试验会增加抑制作用。本研究基于七年级学生的数据,表明抑制不仅在学习者的一般数学成绩中起作用,而且在特定的数学领域,如分数中也起作用。此外,与非开关项目相比,正确解决开关项目的正确率较低,反应时间较长,这是一个开关成本。
{"title":"The Role of the Inhibition of Natural Number Based Reasoning and Strategy Switch Cost in a Fraction Comparison Task","authors":"J. Hoof, E. Ceulemans, W. Dooren","doi":"10.31577/SP.2021.01.814","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.31577/SP.2021.01.814","url":null,"abstract":"Previous research amply showed the importance of a good fraction understanding but also people’s lack of fraction understanding. It is therefore important to investigate the cognitive processes that underlie reasoning with fractions. The present study investigated the role of inhibition and switch costs in fraction comparison tasks. Participants solved a fraction comparison task that alternated between 4 items congruent and 4 items incongruent with natural number reasoning. This allowed to not only investigate congruency switch effects, but also inhibition, given that inhibition was experimentally increased by the prolonged exposure to incongruent trials. Based on data of seventh graders, the present study showed that inhibition does not only play a role in learners’ general mathematics achievement, but also in specific areas of mathematics, such as fractions. Moreover, a switch cost was found in the lower accuracy rates and higher reaction times needed to correctly solve switch items compared to non-switch items.","PeriodicalId":45798,"journal":{"name":"Studia Psychologica","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.2,"publicationDate":"2021-03-15","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"90673246","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
期刊
Studia Psychologica
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1