首页 > 最新文献

Linguistic Typology最新文献

英文 中文
Object and handling handshapes in 11 sign languages: towards a typology of the iconic use of the hands 11种手语中的对象和处理手势:指向手的标志性使用的类型学
IF 2 2区 文学 0 LANGUAGE & LINGUISTICS Pub Date : 2021-12-10 DOI: 10.1515/lingty-2021-0026
Victoria Nyst, Marta Morgado, Timothy Mac Hadjah, M. Nyarko, Mariana Martins, Lisa van der Mark, Evans Burichani, Tano Angoua, Moustapha Magassouba, Dieydi Sylla, Kidane Admasu, A. Schüller
Abstract This article looks at cross-linguistic variation in lexical iconicity, addressing the question of to what extent and how this variation is patterned. More than in spoken languages, iconicity is highly frequent in the lexicons of sign languages. It is also highly complex, in that often multiple motivated components jointly shape an iconic lexeme. Recent typological research on spoken languages finds tentative iconic patterning in a large number of basic lexical items, underlining once again the significance of iconicity for human language. The uncontested and widespread use of iconicity found in the lexicons of sign languages enables us to take typological research into lexical iconicity to the next level. Indeed, previous studies have shown cross-linguistic variation in: a) the use of embodying and handling handshapes in sign languages (mostly of European origin) and b) the frequency of space-based size depiction in African and European sign languages. The two types of variation may be interrelated, as handling handshapes may use space-based size depiction. In this study, we first replicate earlier studies on the distribution of embodying and handling handshapes, this time in a data set consisting of a relatively large set of sign languages (n = 11), most of which are used in Africa. The results confirm significant variation across these sign languages. These findings are then compared to the use of space-based size depiction, revealing that these patterns independently from the distribution of embodying/handling handshapes. We argue that the results call for expanding typological studies on representational strategies in iconic signs beyond the now relatively well studied instrument/manipulation alternation. Fine-grained analyses on a multitude of iconic features in signs are likely to reveal cross-linguistic variation in iconic tendencies in SL lexicons.
摘要本文着眼于词汇象似性的跨语言变异,探讨了这种变异在多大程度上以及如何形成模式的问题。象似性在手语词典中比在口语中更为常见。它也非常复杂,因为经常有多个动机成分共同形成一个标志性的词位。最近对口语的类型学研究发现,在大量的基本词汇中存在着试探性的象似性模式,再次强调了象似性对人类语言的意义。象似性在手语词典中的广泛使用使我们能够将词汇象似性的类型学研究提升到一个新的水平。事实上,先前的研究表明,在以下方面存在跨语言差异:a)手语(主要源自欧洲)中体现和处理手势的使用;b)非洲和欧洲手语中天基尺寸描述的频率。这两种类型的变化可能是相互关联的,因为处理手型可能使用基于空间的尺寸描述。在这项研究中,我们首先复制了早期关于体现和处理手势分布的研究,这一次是在一个由相对较大的手语集(n=11)组成的数据集中,其中大多数在非洲使用。研究结果证实了这些手语之间的显著差异。然后将这些发现与天基尺寸描述的使用进行了比较,揭示了这些模式独立于体现/处理手形的分布。我们认为,这些结果要求在目前研究相对完善的工具/操作交替之外,扩大对标志性符号中表征策略的类型学研究。对符号中大量标志性特征的细粒度分析可能揭示SL词典中标志性倾向的跨语言变化。
{"title":"Object and handling handshapes in 11 sign languages: towards a typology of the iconic use of the hands","authors":"Victoria Nyst, Marta Morgado, Timothy Mac Hadjah, M. Nyarko, Mariana Martins, Lisa van der Mark, Evans Burichani, Tano Angoua, Moustapha Magassouba, Dieydi Sylla, Kidane Admasu, A. Schüller","doi":"10.1515/lingty-2021-0026","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1515/lingty-2021-0026","url":null,"abstract":"Abstract This article looks at cross-linguistic variation in lexical iconicity, addressing the question of to what extent and how this variation is patterned. More than in spoken languages, iconicity is highly frequent in the lexicons of sign languages. It is also highly complex, in that often multiple motivated components jointly shape an iconic lexeme. Recent typological research on spoken languages finds tentative iconic patterning in a large number of basic lexical items, underlining once again the significance of iconicity for human language. The uncontested and widespread use of iconicity found in the lexicons of sign languages enables us to take typological research into lexical iconicity to the next level. Indeed, previous studies have shown cross-linguistic variation in: a) the use of embodying and handling handshapes in sign languages (mostly of European origin) and b) the frequency of space-based size depiction in African and European sign languages. The two types of variation may be interrelated, as handling handshapes may use space-based size depiction. In this study, we first replicate earlier studies on the distribution of embodying and handling handshapes, this time in a data set consisting of a relatively large set of sign languages (n = 11), most of which are used in Africa. The results confirm significant variation across these sign languages. These findings are then compared to the use of space-based size depiction, revealing that these patterns independently from the distribution of embodying/handling handshapes. We argue that the results call for expanding typological studies on representational strategies in iconic signs beyond the now relatively well studied instrument/manipulation alternation. Fine-grained analyses on a multitude of iconic features in signs are likely to reveal cross-linguistic variation in iconic tendencies in SL lexicons.","PeriodicalId":45834,"journal":{"name":"Linguistic Typology","volume":"26 1","pages":"573 - 604"},"PeriodicalIF":2.0,"publicationDate":"2021-12-10","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"42288378","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"文学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 2
Towards a typology of middle voice systems 走向中间语音系统的类型学
IF 2 2区 文学 0 LANGUAGE & LINGUISTICS Pub Date : 2021-11-29 DOI: 10.1515/lingty-2020-0131
G. Inglese
Abstract The middle voice is a notoriously controversial typological notion. Building on previous work (e.g. Kemmer, Suzanne. 1993. The middle voice. Amsterdam & Philadelphia: John Benjamins), in this paper I propose a new working definition of middle markers as inherently polyfunctional constructions which are partly associated with valency change in opposition to bivalent (or more) verbs and partly lexically obligatory with monovalent verbs. Based on this definition, the paper undertakes a systematic survey of 149 middle voice constructions in a sample of 129 middle-marking languages. Evidence from the sample shows that middle voice systems display a much richer variation in forms and functions than is reported in the literature. This richer empirical evidence challenges some of the mainstream views on middle marking, especially its purported connection with reflexivity and grooming-type events, and calls for an overall rethinking of the typology of the middle voice.
中间声音是一个众所周知的有争议的类型学概念。在先前工作的基础上(例如Kemmer,Suzanne,1993。中间的声音。Amsterdam和Philadelphia:John Benjamins),在这篇论文中,我提出了一个新的工作定义,即中间标记是固有的多功能结构,它部分与二价(或更多)动词的配价变化有关,部分与单价动词的词汇强制性有关。基于这一定义,本文对129种中标记语言中的149种中声结构进行了系统调查。来自样本的证据表明,中间语音系统在形式和功能上比文献中报道的要丰富得多。这一更丰富的经验证据挑战了一些关于中间标记的主流观点,尤其是其与自反性和梳理型事件的联系,并呼吁对中间声音的类型进行全面反思。
{"title":"Towards a typology of middle voice systems","authors":"G. Inglese","doi":"10.1515/lingty-2020-0131","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1515/lingty-2020-0131","url":null,"abstract":"Abstract The middle voice is a notoriously controversial typological notion. Building on previous work (e.g. Kemmer, Suzanne. 1993. The middle voice. Amsterdam & Philadelphia: John Benjamins), in this paper I propose a new working definition of middle markers as inherently polyfunctional constructions which are partly associated with valency change in opposition to bivalent (or more) verbs and partly lexically obligatory with monovalent verbs. Based on this definition, the paper undertakes a systematic survey of 149 middle voice constructions in a sample of 129 middle-marking languages. Evidence from the sample shows that middle voice systems display a much richer variation in forms and functions than is reported in the literature. This richer empirical evidence challenges some of the mainstream views on middle marking, especially its purported connection with reflexivity and grooming-type events, and calls for an overall rethinking of the typology of the middle voice.","PeriodicalId":45834,"journal":{"name":"Linguistic Typology","volume":"26 1","pages":"489 - 531"},"PeriodicalIF":2.0,"publicationDate":"2021-11-29","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"42291786","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"文学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 14
Statistical bias control in typology 类型学中的统计偏差控制
IF 2 2区 文学 0 LANGUAGE & LINGUISTICS Pub Date : 2021-11-17 DOI: 10.1515/lingty-2021-0002
Matías Guzmán Naranjo, Laura Becker
Abstract In this paper, we propose two new statistical controls for genealogical and areal bias in typological samples. Our test case being the effect of VO-order effect on affix position (prefixation vs. suffixation), we show how statistical modeling including a phylogenetic regression term (phylogenetic control) and a two-dimensional Gaussian Process (areal control) can be used to capture genealogical and areal effects in a large but unbalanced sample. We find that, once these biases are controlled for, VO-order has no effect on affix position. Another important finding, which is in line with previous studies, is that areal effects are as important as genealogical effects, emphasizing the importance of areal or contact control in typological studies built on language samples. On the other hand, we also show that strict probability sampling is not required with the statistical controls that we propose, as long as the sample is a variety sample large enough to cover different areas and families. This has the crucial practical consequence that it allows us to include as much of the available information as possible, without the need to artificially restrict the sample and potentially lose otherwise available information.
在本文中,我们提出了两种新的统计控制类型样本的宗谱和地区偏差。我们的测试用例是元音顺序效应对词根位置(前缀与后缀)的影响,我们展示了如何使用统计建模(包括系统发育回归项(系统发育控制)和二维高斯过程(区域控制))来捕获大型但不平衡样本中的谱系和区域效应。我们发现,一旦这些偏差得到控制,词缀顺序对词缀位置没有影响。另一个与先前研究一致的重要发现是,区域效应与系谱效应同样重要,强调了在基于语言样本的类型学研究中区域或接触控制的重要性。另一方面,我们也表明,我们提出的统计控制并不需要严格的概率抽样,只要样本是足够大的品种样本,可以覆盖不同的地区和家庭。这有一个重要的实际结果,它允许我们包含尽可能多的可用信息,而不需要人为地限制样本和潜在地丢失其他可用信息。
{"title":"Statistical bias control in typology","authors":"Matías Guzmán Naranjo, Laura Becker","doi":"10.1515/lingty-2021-0002","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1515/lingty-2021-0002","url":null,"abstract":"Abstract In this paper, we propose two new statistical controls for genealogical and areal bias in typological samples. Our test case being the effect of VO-order effect on affix position (prefixation vs. suffixation), we show how statistical modeling including a phylogenetic regression term (phylogenetic control) and a two-dimensional Gaussian Process (areal control) can be used to capture genealogical and areal effects in a large but unbalanced sample. We find that, once these biases are controlled for, VO-order has no effect on affix position. Another important finding, which is in line with previous studies, is that areal effects are as important as genealogical effects, emphasizing the importance of areal or contact control in typological studies built on language samples. On the other hand, we also show that strict probability sampling is not required with the statistical controls that we propose, as long as the sample is a variety sample large enough to cover different areas and families. This has the crucial practical consequence that it allows us to include as much of the available information as possible, without the need to artificially restrict the sample and potentially lose otherwise available information.","PeriodicalId":45834,"journal":{"name":"Linguistic Typology","volume":"26 1","pages":"605 - 670"},"PeriodicalIF":2.0,"publicationDate":"2021-11-17","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"41789295","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"文学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 9
Similarity of mirative and contrastive focus: three parameters for describing attention markers 幻像焦点和对比焦点的相似性:描述注意标记的三个参数
IF 2 2区 文学 0 LANGUAGE & LINGUISTICS Pub Date : 2021-11-01 DOI: 10.1515/lingty-2020-0134
J. Yliniemi
Abstract This article addresses the heretofore unacknowledged similarity of mirative-like and contrastive-focus-like functions by describing data from Denjongke (Tibetic, sip), Bih (Chamic, Vietnam, ibh) and Nepali (Indo-Aryan, nep). The similarity between mirative-like and contrastive-focus-like functions in the aforementioned languages is captured by the notion of something being brought to the forefront of attention. Mirative-like semantics are shown to be epiphenomenal to attention-oriented phenomena, and the functional domain in which the morphemes operate is shown to be attention rather than knowledge structure. The morphemes in the study are described in terms of three parameters, which are put forward as potentially useful tools for describing similar morphemes in other languages: speaker versus addressee orientation, clausal versus phrasal scope, and anaphoric versus cataphoric use. The first two parameters form a fourfold table in which the heuristically named “mirative-like function” has clausal scope and is speaker-oriented (i.e. speaker signals that something has come to the forefront of their attention). “Contrastive-focus-like function”, on the other hand, has phrasal scope and is addressee-oriented (i.e. speaker intends to bring something to the forefront of the addressee’s attention). Cognitively, contrastive-focus-like function is shown to establish joint attention.
摘要本文通过描述Denjongke(Tibetic,sip)、Bih(Chamic,Vietnam,ibh)和Nepali(Indo-Aryan,nep)的数据,解决了迄今为止未被承认的类幻像和类对比焦点函数的相似性。在上述语言中,类似幻想的功能和类似对比焦点的功能之间的相似性是通过某种东西被带到最前沿的概念来捕捉的。类似幻影的语义被证明是注意力导向现象的副现象,语素在其中运作的功能域被证明是注意而不是知识结构。研究中的语素是根据三个参数来描述的,这三个参数被认为是描述其他语言中类似语素的潜在有用工具:说话人与收件人的取向、从句与短语范围以及回指与回指的使用。前两个参数形成了一个四重表,其中启发式命名的“类幻想函数”具有子句范围,并且是面向说话者的(即说话者发出信号,表示某个东西已经成为他们关注的焦点)。另一方面,“对比焦点式功能”具有短语范围,并以收件人为导向(即说话者打算将某些东西带到收件人的最前沿)。认知上,对比焦点样功能被证明可以建立共同注意力。
{"title":"Similarity of mirative and contrastive focus: three parameters for describing attention markers","authors":"J. Yliniemi","doi":"10.1515/lingty-2020-0134","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1515/lingty-2020-0134","url":null,"abstract":"Abstract This article addresses the heretofore unacknowledged similarity of mirative-like and contrastive-focus-like functions by describing data from Denjongke (Tibetic, sip), Bih (Chamic, Vietnam, ibh) and Nepali (Indo-Aryan, nep). The similarity between mirative-like and contrastive-focus-like functions in the aforementioned languages is captured by the notion of something being brought to the forefront of attention. Mirative-like semantics are shown to be epiphenomenal to attention-oriented phenomena, and the functional domain in which the morphemes operate is shown to be attention rather than knowledge structure. The morphemes in the study are described in terms of three parameters, which are put forward as potentially useful tools for describing similar morphemes in other languages: speaker versus addressee orientation, clausal versus phrasal scope, and anaphoric versus cataphoric use. The first two parameters form a fourfold table in which the heuristically named “mirative-like function” has clausal scope and is speaker-oriented (i.e. speaker signals that something has come to the forefront of their attention). “Contrastive-focus-like function”, on the other hand, has phrasal scope and is addressee-oriented (i.e. speaker intends to bring something to the forefront of the addressee’s attention). Cognitively, contrastive-focus-like function is shown to establish joint attention.","PeriodicalId":45834,"journal":{"name":"Linguistic Typology","volume":"27 1","pages":"77 - 111"},"PeriodicalIF":2.0,"publicationDate":"2021-11-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"49545611","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"文学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 2
Cross-linguistic constraints and lineage-specific developments in the semantics of cutting and breaking in Japonic and Germanic 日语和日耳曼语义学的跨语言限制和特定谱系发展
IF 2 2区 文学 0 LANGUAGE & LINGUISTICS Pub Date : 2021-10-13 DOI: 10.1515/lingty-2021-2090
John L. A. Huisman, R. van Hout, A. Majid
Abstract Semantic variation in the cutting and breaking domain has been shown to be constrained across languages in a previous typological study, but it was unclear whether Japanese was an outlier in this domain. Here we revisit cutting and breaking in the Japonic language area by collecting new naming data for 40 videoclips depicting cutting and breaking events in Standard Japanese, the highly divergent Tohoku dialects, as well as four related Ryukyuan languages (Amami, Okinawa, Miyako and Yaeyama). We find that the Japonic languages recapitulate the same semantic dimensions attested in the previous typological study, confirming that semantic variation in the domain of cutting and breaking is indeed cross-linguistically constrained. We then compare our new Japonic data to previously collected Germanic data and find that, in general, related languages resemble each other more than unrelated languages, and that the Japonic languages resemble each other more than the Germanic languages do. Nevertheless, English resembles all of the Japonic languages more than it resembles Swedish. Together, these findings show that the rate and extent of semantic change can differ between language families, indicating the existence of lineage-specific developments on top of universal cross-linguistic constraints.
在先前的类型学研究中,切割和断裂领域的语义变化在不同语言之间受到限制,但日语是否是该领域的异常值尚不清楚。在这里,我们通过收集40个视频片段的新命名数据来重新审视日本语言领域的切割和切割事件,这些视频片段描述了标准日语、高度分化的东北方言以及四种相关的琉球语(天美语、冲绳语、宫古语和八山语)中的切割和切割事件。我们发现日本语再现了先前类型学研究中证实的相同语义维度,证实了切割和断裂领域的语义变化确实受到跨语言的约束。然后,我们将新的日语数据与之前收集的日耳曼语数据进行比较,发现一般来说,相关语言比不相关语言更相似,日语比日耳曼语言更相似。然而,比起瑞典语,英语更像所有的日语。总之,这些发现表明,语族之间语义变化的速度和程度可能不同,表明在普遍的跨语言限制之上存在谱系特定的发展。
{"title":"Cross-linguistic constraints and lineage-specific developments in the semantics of cutting and breaking in Japonic and Germanic","authors":"John L. A. Huisman, R. van Hout, A. Majid","doi":"10.1515/lingty-2021-2090","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1515/lingty-2021-2090","url":null,"abstract":"Abstract Semantic variation in the cutting and breaking domain has been shown to be constrained across languages in a previous typological study, but it was unclear whether Japanese was an outlier in this domain. Here we revisit cutting and breaking in the Japonic language area by collecting new naming data for 40 videoclips depicting cutting and breaking events in Standard Japanese, the highly divergent Tohoku dialects, as well as four related Ryukyuan languages (Amami, Okinawa, Miyako and Yaeyama). We find that the Japonic languages recapitulate the same semantic dimensions attested in the previous typological study, confirming that semantic variation in the domain of cutting and breaking is indeed cross-linguistically constrained. We then compare our new Japonic data to previously collected Germanic data and find that, in general, related languages resemble each other more than unrelated languages, and that the Japonic languages resemble each other more than the Germanic languages do. Nevertheless, English resembles all of the Japonic languages more than it resembles Swedish. Together, these findings show that the rate and extent of semantic change can differ between language families, indicating the existence of lineage-specific developments on top of universal cross-linguistic constraints.","PeriodicalId":45834,"journal":{"name":"Linguistic Typology","volume":"27 1","pages":"41 - 75"},"PeriodicalIF":2.0,"publicationDate":"2021-10-13","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"42029663","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"文学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
Patterns of persistence and diffusibility in the European lexicon 欧洲词汇中的持续性和扩散性模式
IF 2 2区 文学 0 LANGUAGE & LINGUISTICS Pub Date : 2021-10-13 DOI: 10.1515/lingty-2021-2086
Volker Gast, Maria Koptjevskaja-Tamm
Abstract This article investigates to what extent the semantics and the phonological forms of lexical items are genealogically inherited or acquired through language contact. We focus on patterns of colexification (the encoding of two concepts with the same word) as an aspect of lexical-semantic organization. We test two pairs of hypotheses. The first pair concerns the genealogical stability (persistence) and susceptibility to contact-induced change (diffusibility) of colexification patterns and phonological matter in the 40 most genealogically stable elements of the 100-items Swadesh list, which we call “nuclear vocabulary”. We hypothesize that colexification patterns are (a) less persistent, and (b) more diffusible, than the phonological form of nuclear vocabulary. The second pair of hypotheses concerns degrees of diffusibility in two different sections of the lexicon – “core vocabulary” (all 100 elements of the Swadesh list) and its complement (“non-core/peripheral vocabulary”). We hypothesize that the colexification patterns associated with core vocabulary are (a) more persistent, and (b) less diffusible, than colexification patterns associated with peripheral vocabulary. The four hypotheses are tested using the lexical-semantic data from the CLICS database and independently determined phonological dissimilarity measures. The hypothesis that colexification patterns are less persistent than the phonological matter of nuclear vocabulary receives clear support. The hypothesis that colexification patterns are more diffusible than phonological matter receives some support, but a significant difference can only be observed for unrelated languages. The hypothesis that colexification patterns involving core vocabulary are more genealogically stable than colexification patterns at the periphery of the lexicon cannot be confirmed, but the data seem to indicate a higher degree of diffusibility for colexification patterns at the periphery of the lexicon. While we regard the results of our study as valid, we emphasize the tentativeness of our conclusions and point out some limitations as well as desiderata for future research to enable a better understanding of the genealogical versus areal distribution of linguistic features.
摘要本文研究了词汇的语义和语音形式在多大程度上是通过语言接触而遗传或获得的。我们关注的是作为词汇语义组织的一个方面的colexification模式(用同一个词编码两个概念)。我们检验了两对假设。第一对涉及100个项目Swadesh列表中40个最稳定的系谱元素中的集体诱导模式和语音物质的系谱稳定性(持久性)和接触诱导变化的易感性(扩散性),我们称之为“核心词汇”。我们假设,与核心词汇的语音形式相比,集体诱导模式(a)不那么持久,(b)更容易传播。第二对假设涉及词汇的两个不同部分的扩散程度——“核心词汇”(Swadesh列表的所有100个元素)及其补语(“非核心/外围词汇”)。我们假设,与核心词汇相关的嗜睡症模式比与外围词汇相关的嗜睡症模式(a)更持久,(b)更不易传播。使用CLICS数据库中的词汇语义数据和独立确定的语音相异性测量来测试这四个假设。与核心词汇的音韵学物质相比,集体诱导模式的持久性较差的假说得到了明确的支持。共诱导模式比语音物质更容易传播的假设得到了一些支持,但只有在不相关的语言中才能观察到显著的差异。涉及核心词汇的嗜睡症模式比词汇外围的嗜睡癖模式在谱系上更稳定的假设无法得到证实,但数据似乎表明,嗜睡症在词汇外围的扩散程度更高。虽然我们认为我们的研究结果是有效的,但我们强调了我们结论的暂时性,并指出了一些局限性以及未来研究的必要性,以更好地理解语言特征的谱系分布与区域分布。
{"title":"Patterns of persistence and diffusibility in the European lexicon","authors":"Volker Gast, Maria Koptjevskaja-Tamm","doi":"10.1515/lingty-2021-2086","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1515/lingty-2021-2086","url":null,"abstract":"Abstract This article investigates to what extent the semantics and the phonological forms of lexical items are genealogically inherited or acquired through language contact. We focus on patterns of colexification (the encoding of two concepts with the same word) as an aspect of lexical-semantic organization. We test two pairs of hypotheses. The first pair concerns the genealogical stability (persistence) and susceptibility to contact-induced change (diffusibility) of colexification patterns and phonological matter in the 40 most genealogically stable elements of the 100-items Swadesh list, which we call “nuclear vocabulary”. We hypothesize that colexification patterns are (a) less persistent, and (b) more diffusible, than the phonological form of nuclear vocabulary. The second pair of hypotheses concerns degrees of diffusibility in two different sections of the lexicon – “core vocabulary” (all 100 elements of the Swadesh list) and its complement (“non-core/peripheral vocabulary”). We hypothesize that the colexification patterns associated with core vocabulary are (a) more persistent, and (b) less diffusible, than colexification patterns associated with peripheral vocabulary. The four hypotheses are tested using the lexical-semantic data from the CLICS database and independently determined phonological dissimilarity measures. The hypothesis that colexification patterns are less persistent than the phonological matter of nuclear vocabulary receives clear support. The hypothesis that colexification patterns are more diffusible than phonological matter receives some support, but a significant difference can only be observed for unrelated languages. The hypothesis that colexification patterns involving core vocabulary are more genealogically stable than colexification patterns at the periphery of the lexicon cannot be confirmed, but the data seem to indicate a higher degree of diffusibility for colexification patterns at the periphery of the lexicon. While we regard the results of our study as valid, we emphasize the tentativeness of our conclusions and point out some limitations as well as desiderata for future research to enable a better understanding of the genealogical versus areal distribution of linguistic features.","PeriodicalId":45834,"journal":{"name":"Linguistic Typology","volume":"26 1","pages":"403 - 438"},"PeriodicalIF":2.0,"publicationDate":"2021-10-13","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"47148038","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"文学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 3
Frontmatter Frontmatter
IF 2 2区 文学 0 LANGUAGE & LINGUISTICS Pub Date : 2021-10-01 DOI: 10.1515/lingty-2021-frontmatter3
{"title":"Frontmatter","authors":"","doi":"10.1515/lingty-2021-frontmatter3","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1515/lingty-2021-frontmatter3","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":45834,"journal":{"name":"Linguistic Typology","volume":"1 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.0,"publicationDate":"2021-10-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"47792716","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"文学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Universal and macro-areal patterns in the lexicon 词汇中的普遍模式和宏观模式
IF 2 2区 文学 0 LANGUAGE & LINGUISTICS Pub Date : 2021-09-28 DOI: 10.1515/lingty-2021-2088
T. Georgakopoulos, Eitan Grossman, D. Nikolaev, Stéphane Polis
Abstract This paper investigates universal and areal structures in the lexicon as manifested by colexification patterns in the semantic domains of perception and cognition, based on data from both small and large datasets. Using several methods, including weighted semantic maps, formal concept lattices, correlation analysis, and dimensionality reduction, we identify colexification patterns in the domains in question and evaluate the extent to which these patterns are specific to particular areas. This paper contributes to the methodology of investigating areal patterns in the lexicon, and identifies a number of cross-linguistic regularities and of area-specific properties in the structuring of lexicons.
摘要本文基于小型和大型数据集,研究了词汇中的普遍结构和区域结构,这些结构表现为感知和认知语义领域的共化模式。使用几种方法,包括加权语义图、形式概念格、相关分析和降维,我们确定了问题领域中的共化模式,并评估了这些模式对特定领域的特定程度。本文为研究词汇的地域模式提供了方法,并发现了词汇结构中的一些跨语言规律和地域特征。
{"title":"Universal and macro-areal patterns in the lexicon","authors":"T. Georgakopoulos, Eitan Grossman, D. Nikolaev, Stéphane Polis","doi":"10.1515/lingty-2021-2088","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1515/lingty-2021-2088","url":null,"abstract":"Abstract This paper investigates universal and areal structures in the lexicon as manifested by colexification patterns in the semantic domains of perception and cognition, based on data from both small and large datasets. Using several methods, including weighted semantic maps, formal concept lattices, correlation analysis, and dimensionality reduction, we identify colexification patterns in the domains in question and evaluate the extent to which these patterns are specific to particular areas. This paper contributes to the methodology of investigating areal patterns in the lexicon, and identifies a number of cross-linguistic regularities and of area-specific properties in the structuring of lexicons.","PeriodicalId":45834,"journal":{"name":"Linguistic Typology","volume":"26 1","pages":"439 - 487"},"PeriodicalIF":2.0,"publicationDate":"2021-09-28","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"43424223","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"文学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 14
Areal patterns and colexifications of colour terms in the languages of Africa 非洲语言中颜色术语的区域模式和集合现象
IF 2 2区 文学 0 LANGUAGE & LINGUISTICS Pub Date : 2021-09-27 DOI: 10.1515/lingty-2021-2085
Guillaume Segerer, M. Vanhove
Abstract Of all the semantic domains, colour terms have attracted the largest amount of attention, notably from a typological point of view. However, there is much more to be discovered. A search of the cross-linguistic lexical database of African languages (RefLex) reveals several previously undetected areal colexification patterns and shared lexico-constructional patterns in a genetically balanced sample of 401 languages. In this paper, we illustrate several areal characteristics of colour terms: (i) the spread of an areal feature due to a common extra-linguistic setting (locust bean – Parkia biglobosa – as the lexical source of yellow); (ii) two convergence phenomena, one based on a shared lexico-constructional pattern including a term for water, and one based on shared colexifications (red and ripe vs. green and unripe); and (iii) an areal pattern of lexical diffusion of colour ideophones, a category which has thus far been considered difficult to borrow.
摘要在所有的语义领域中,颜色术语吸引了最多的关注,尤其是从类型学的角度来看。然而,还有更多的东西有待发现。在401种语言的基因平衡样本中,对非洲语言跨语言词汇数据库(RefLex)的搜索揭示了几个以前未被发现的区域共有模式和共享的词典结构模式。在本文中,我们举例说明了颜色术语的几个区域特征:(i)一个区域特征由于一个常见的语言外环境而传播(蝗灾豆——大豆瓣——作为黄色的词汇来源);(ii)两种趋同现象,一种基于包括水的术语在内的共享词典结构模式,另一种基于共享colesiforms(红色和成熟与绿色和未成熟);以及(iii)彩色表意词词汇扩散的区域模式,这一类别迄今为止被认为很难借用。
{"title":"Areal patterns and colexifications of colour terms in the languages of Africa","authors":"Guillaume Segerer, M. Vanhove","doi":"10.1515/lingty-2021-2085","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1515/lingty-2021-2085","url":null,"abstract":"Abstract Of all the semantic domains, colour terms have attracted the largest amount of attention, notably from a typological point of view. However, there is much more to be discovered. A search of the cross-linguistic lexical database of African languages (RefLex) reveals several previously undetected areal colexification patterns and shared lexico-constructional patterns in a genetically balanced sample of 401 languages. In this paper, we illustrate several areal characteristics of colour terms: (i) the spread of an areal feature due to a common extra-linguistic setting (locust bean – Parkia biglobosa – as the lexical source of yellow); (ii) two convergence phenomena, one based on a shared lexico-constructional pattern including a term for water, and one based on shared colexifications (red and ripe vs. green and unripe); and (iii) an areal pattern of lexical diffusion of colour ideophones, a category which has thus far been considered difficult to borrow.","PeriodicalId":45834,"journal":{"name":"Linguistic Typology","volume":"26 1","pages":"247 - 281"},"PeriodicalIF":2.0,"publicationDate":"2021-09-27","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"46106719","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"文学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
The general noun-modifying clause construction beyond Eurasia 欧亚大陆以外的一般名词修饰从句结构
IF 2 2区 文学 0 LANGUAGE & LINGUISTICS Pub Date : 2021-09-22 DOI: 10.1515/lingty-2020-0100
Tongyu Wu
Abstract I aim to provide a typological investigation of the General Noun-modifying Clause Construction (NMCC) in languages other than those of Eurasia. I show that the five properties proposed by Matsumoto et al. as potentially correlating with the General NMCC are rather areal features which are falsified by the data of languages from Africa and Europe. The semantic interpretability condition and the syntactic licensing condition of the General Noun-modifying Clause Construction need reconsidering. Semantically, I argue that the interpretability of the General NMCC depends both on the semantics of the head noun and that of the modifying clause because they show close interaction with each other. Syntactically, I propose three general syntactic properties of the languages with the General NMCC, i.e. (1) no relative pronouns or relative pronouns in competition with a general clause marker, (2) complex subordinate locutions composed of the general clause marker(s) (and a head noun), and (3) unified verb forms in subordination.
摘要我的目的是对欧亚大陆以外的语言中的通用名词修饰从句结构(NMCC)进行类型学研究。我表明,Matsumoto等人提出的可能与一般NMCC相关的五个性质是相当区域性的特征,这些特征被非洲和欧洲语言的数据所伪造。一般名词修饰从句结构的语义可解释性条件和句法许可条件需要重新考虑。从语义上讲,我认为通用NMCC的可解释性既取决于头名词的语义,也取决于修饰从句的语义,因为它们表现出密切的相互作用。在句法上,我提出了通用NMCC语言的三个一般句法特性,即(1)没有关系代词或与通用从句标记竞争的关系代词,(2)由通用从句标记(和一个头名词)组成的复杂从句,以及(3)在从属关系中统一的动词形式。
{"title":"The general noun-modifying clause construction beyond Eurasia","authors":"Tongyu Wu","doi":"10.1515/lingty-2020-0100","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1515/lingty-2020-0100","url":null,"abstract":"Abstract I aim to provide a typological investigation of the General Noun-modifying Clause Construction (NMCC) in languages other than those of Eurasia. I show that the five properties proposed by Matsumoto et al. as potentially correlating with the General NMCC are rather areal features which are falsified by the data of languages from Africa and Europe. The semantic interpretability condition and the syntactic licensing condition of the General Noun-modifying Clause Construction need reconsidering. Semantically, I argue that the interpretability of the General NMCC depends both on the semantics of the head noun and that of the modifying clause because they show close interaction with each other. Syntactically, I propose three general syntactic properties of the languages with the General NMCC, i.e. (1) no relative pronouns or relative pronouns in competition with a general clause marker, (2) complex subordinate locutions composed of the general clause marker(s) (and a head noun), and (3) unified verb forms in subordination.","PeriodicalId":45834,"journal":{"name":"Linguistic Typology","volume":"26 1","pages":"89 - 127"},"PeriodicalIF":2.0,"publicationDate":"2021-09-22","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"49013155","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"文学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Linguistic Typology
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1