首页 > 最新文献

STUDIA LINGUISTICA最新文献

英文 中文
THEORETICAL A‐GRAMMATISM: THE CASE FOR AN ELIMINATIVIST MINIMALISM 理论上的语法主义:消解主义最低限度的理由
IF 0.6 3区 文学 N/A LANGUAGE & LINGUISTICS Pub Date : 2024-07-24 DOI: 10.1111/stul.12245
Claudia Pañeda, Guillermo Lorenzo
This paper explores patterns of cross‐linguistic, intra‐linguistic and individual variation in the acceptability of sentences with extraction from islands, a classic object of attention of generative grammar. It asks which “competence” concept better fits the presented data. We contrast two alternative views: one that ascribes a multilingual competence to individual speakers, in the “rich competence” tradition of acquisition theory, and another one based on a “competence‐free” kind of model, which is framed as an instantiation of the minimalist approach to the language faculty. We conclude that the latter eliminativist approach is better suited to account for variation in the acceptability of island sentences.
本文探讨了从生成语法的经典关注对象--岛屿中提取的句子的可接受性在跨语言、语言内和个体差异方面的模式。本文探讨了哪种 "能力 "概念更符合所提供的数据。我们对比了两种不同的观点:一种是将多语言能力赋予个体说话者,这是习得理论的 "丰富能力 "传统;另一种是基于 "无能力 "模型,这是语言能力最小化方法的具体体现。我们的结论是,后一种消除主义方法更适合解释岛屿句子可接受性的变化。
{"title":"THEORETICAL A‐GRAMMATISM: THE CASE FOR AN ELIMINATIVIST MINIMALISM","authors":"Claudia Pañeda, Guillermo Lorenzo","doi":"10.1111/stul.12245","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1111/stul.12245","url":null,"abstract":"This paper explores patterns of cross‐linguistic, intra‐linguistic and individual variation in the acceptability of sentences with extraction from islands, a classic object of attention of generative grammar. It asks which “competence” concept better fits the presented data. We contrast two alternative views: one that ascribes a multilingual competence to individual speakers, in the “rich competence” tradition of acquisition theory, and another one based on a “competence‐free” kind of model, which is framed as an instantiation of the minimalist approach to the language faculty. We conclude that the latter eliminativist approach is better suited to account for variation in the acceptability of island sentences.","PeriodicalId":46179,"journal":{"name":"STUDIA LINGUISTICA","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.6,"publicationDate":"2024-07-24","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141779289","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"文学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Verb‐echo answers in Japanese do not call for syntactic head movement: Arguments for a pragmatic account* 日语中的动词回声应答并不要求句法头的移动:语用学说的论据*
IF 0.6 3区 文学 N/A LANGUAGE & LINGUISTICS Pub Date : 2024-07-17 DOI: 10.1111/stul.12241
Tomoya Tanabe, Ryoichiro Kobayashi
This paper tackles the issue of whether syntactic head movement exists in Japanese. Sato & Hayashi (2018) and Sato & Maeda (2021) propose that Verb‐Echo Answers (VEAs), an instance of fragment answers, in Japanese are derived via the so‐called Verb‐stranding TP‐Ellipsis (VTPE; i.e., TP‐ellipsis accompanied by verb‐raising to C), thereby claiming that head movement exists in Japanese as a syntactic operation. In response, this paper argues that pro and Argument Ellipsis (AE) in Japanese sufficiently account for the key observations presented in their works. Specifically, a careful examination of the discourse in each question‐answer pair reveals that the seemingly problematic scope patterns in VEAs do not call for the VTPE analysis. We also show that the unacceptability of voice mismatches in VEAs can be explained by a discourse‐based analysis within the Question Under Discussion framework. Further, we provide an extensive discussion on the alleged evidence against the pro/AE analysis concerning adjunct‐inclusive readings. We show that negative scope reversal effects, which Sato & Maeda (2021) argue occur in VTPE, do not occur between adjuncts and negation in the novel data. Given this, we discuss possible ways to account for the availability of adjunct‐inclusive readings in VEAs with no recourse to VTPE, and suggest avenues for future research. The proposed analyses of VEAs shed new light on intriguing aspects of ellipsis phenomena, which involve complex interactions between syntax, semantics, and pragmatics.
本文探讨了日语中是否存在句法头移动的问题。Sato & Hayashi(2018)和Sato & Maeda(2021)提出,日语中的片段回答实例--动词回声回答(VEAs)是通过所谓的 "Verb-stranding TP-Ellipsis"(VTPE;即TP-ellipsis伴随着动词提升到C)派生出来的,从而声称日语中存在作为句法操作的头部移动。作为回应,本文认为日语中的亲切省略(pro)和论据省略(Argument Ellipsis,AE)足以解释其著作中提出的关键观察结果。具体地说,通过仔细研究每一对问答中的话语,我们发现 VEA 中看似有问题的范围模式并不需要 VTPE 分析。我们还表明,在 "问题讨论 "框架内,基于话语的分析可以解释 VEA 中语音不匹配的不可接受性。此外,我们还广泛讨论了有关辅助词包含读法的支持/AE 分析的所谓证据。我们证明,佐藤与amp;前田(2021)认为在 VTPE 中会出现的负范围反转效应,在小说数据中并没有出现在附属词和否定词之间。有鉴于此,我们讨论了在不使用 VTPE 的情况下解释 VEA 中附属词包含读音的可能方法,并提出了未来的研究方向。我们提出的对 VEAs 的分析为省略现象的有趣方面提供了新的启示,省略现象涉及句法、语义和语用之间复杂的相互作用。
{"title":"Verb‐echo answers in Japanese do not call for syntactic head movement: Arguments for a pragmatic account*","authors":"Tomoya Tanabe, Ryoichiro Kobayashi","doi":"10.1111/stul.12241","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1111/stul.12241","url":null,"abstract":"This paper tackles the issue of whether syntactic head movement exists in Japanese. Sato &amp; Hayashi (2018) and Sato &amp; Maeda (2021) propose that <jats:italic>Verb‐Echo Answers</jats:italic> (VEAs), an instance of <jats:italic>fragment answers</jats:italic>, in Japanese are derived via the so‐called <jats:italic>Verb‐stranding TP‐Ellipsis</jats:italic> (VTPE; i.e., TP‐ellipsis accompanied by verb‐raising to C), thereby claiming that head movement exists in Japanese as a syntactic operation. In response, this paper argues that <jats:italic>pro</jats:italic> and <jats:italic>Argument Ellipsis</jats:italic> (AE) in Japanese sufficiently account for the key observations presented in their works. Specifically, a careful examination of the discourse in each question‐answer pair reveals that the seemingly problematic scope patterns in VEAs do not call for the VTPE analysis. We also show that the unacceptability of voice mismatches in VEAs can be explained by a discourse‐based analysis within the Question Under Discussion framework. Further, we provide an extensive discussion on the alleged evidence against the <jats:italic>pro</jats:italic>/AE analysis concerning adjunct‐inclusive readings. We show that negative scope reversal effects, which Sato &amp; Maeda (2021) argue occur in VTPE, do not occur between adjuncts and negation in the novel data. Given this, we discuss possible ways to account for the availability of adjunct‐inclusive readings in VEAs with no recourse to VTPE, and suggest avenues for future research. The proposed analyses of VEAs shed new light on intriguing aspects of ellipsis phenomena, which involve complex interactions between syntax, semantics, and pragmatics.","PeriodicalId":46179,"journal":{"name":"STUDIA LINGUISTICA","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.6,"publicationDate":"2024-07-17","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141744307","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"文学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Bottom Copy Pronunciation in Japanese Passives 日语被动语态中的底仿发音
IF 0.6 3区 文学 N/A LANGUAGE & LINGUISTICS Pub Date : 2024-07-17 DOI: 10.1111/stul.12242
Jun Abe
On the basis of Abe's (2016) proposal that in the case of an A‐chain, any member can be the target for pronunciation, I argue that Japanese passives may have the bottom copies of the produced A‐chains pronounced. When the passivized subjects appear to occupy their original θ‐positions, it is standardly claimed that the relevant word order is derived by scrambling other material sentence‐initially. Contrary to this claim, I demonstrate that there are cases, including those of what Miyagawa (1989) calls the causative‐passive construction, where the passivized subjects actually occupy their original θ‐positions. I then provide evidence that those passivized subjects undergo “covert” A‐movement, hence giving support to my bottom copy pronunciation analysis.
根据 Abe(2016)的提议,即在 A 链的情况下,任何成员都可以是发音的目标,我认为日语被动语态可能会对所产生的 A 链的底层副本进行发音。当被动语态化的主语似乎占据了它们原来的θ-位置时,标准的说法是相关的词序是通过扰乱其他材料的句子初始得来的。与这一说法相反,我证明在有些情况下,包括宫川(1989)所说的因果被动结构,被动化的主语实际上占据了它们原来的θ-位置。然后,我提供了证据,证明这些被钝化的主语进行了 "隐蔽的 "A-移动,从而支持了我的底仿发音分析。
{"title":"Bottom Copy Pronunciation in Japanese Passives","authors":"Jun Abe","doi":"10.1111/stul.12242","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1111/stul.12242","url":null,"abstract":"On the basis of Abe's (2016) proposal that in the case of an A‐chain, any member can be the target for pronunciation, I argue that Japanese passives may have the bottom copies of the produced A‐chains pronounced. When the passivized subjects appear to occupy their original θ‐positions, it is standardly claimed that the relevant word order is derived by scrambling other material sentence‐initially. Contrary to this claim, I demonstrate that there are cases, including those of what Miyagawa (1989) calls the causative‐passive construction, where the passivized subjects actually occupy their original θ‐positions. I then provide evidence that those passivized subjects undergo “covert” A‐movement, hence giving support to my bottom copy pronunciation analysis.","PeriodicalId":46179,"journal":{"name":"STUDIA LINGUISTICA","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.6,"publicationDate":"2024-07-17","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141744341","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"文学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Syntactic Variations in Referential Metonymy 指代性隐喻的句法变化
IF 0.6 3区 文学 N/A LANGUAGE & LINGUISTICS Pub Date : 2024-07-10 DOI: 10.1111/stul.12243
Zhen Wu
This paper explores the syntactic variations of concord in sentences involving referential metonymy in English. Apart from the standard type in which syntactic agreement is shown between the metonymic NP and the verbs or pronouns that follow, this article also discusses and summarizes other types where there are various degrees of disagreement. Moreover, this paper formalizes the principles of Ruiz de Mendoza and Díez (2004) by introducing the metonymic function, and proposes simplification of these principles based on the notion of metonymy as a contingency. Finally, this paper discusses other possible factors – both contextual and non‐contextual – that may lead to these syntactic variations.
本文探讨了英语中涉及指代性隐喻的句子中的句法一致变化。除了隐喻 NP 与后面的动词或代词之间表现出句法一致的标准类型外,本文还讨论并总结了存在不同程度分歧的其他类型。此外,本文通过引入隐喻功能,正式确定了 Ruiz de Mendoza 和 Díez(2004 年)的原则,并根据隐喻是一种或然现象的概念,提出了简化这些原则的建议。最后,本文讨论了可能导致这些句法变化的其他可能因素(包括语境因素和非语境因素)。
{"title":"Syntactic Variations in Referential Metonymy","authors":"Zhen Wu","doi":"10.1111/stul.12243","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1111/stul.12243","url":null,"abstract":"This paper explores the syntactic variations of concord in sentences involving referential metonymy in English. Apart from the standard type in which syntactic agreement is shown between the metonymic NP and the verbs or pronouns that follow, this article also discusses and summarizes other types where there are various degrees of disagreement. Moreover, this paper formalizes the principles of Ruiz de Mendoza and Díez (2004) by introducing the metonymic function, and proposes simplification of these principles based on the notion of metonymy as a contingency. Finally, this paper discusses other possible factors – both contextual and non‐contextual – that may lead to these syntactic variations.","PeriodicalId":46179,"journal":{"name":"STUDIA LINGUISTICA","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.6,"publicationDate":"2024-07-10","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141587892","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"文学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
On the scalarity of nu‐V constructions in Taiwan Mandarin 论台湾普通话中 nu-V 结构的标度性
IF 0.4 3区 文学 N/A LANGUAGE & LINGUISTICS Pub Date : 2024-07-10 DOI: 10.1111/stul.12244
Yi‐Hsun Chen, C.‐T. Tim Chou, Po Ju Shen
This paper investigates three puzzles concerning scalar nu‐V constructions in Taiwan Mandarin: (a) a scalar nu‐V construction is incompatible with state/achievement verbs, irrespective of the presence of a quantity phrase; (b) when combined with activity verbs, the inclusion of a quantity phrase appears obligatory for scalar nu‐V constructions in an out‐of‐the‐blue context; (c) under appropriate contextual support, bare nouns and demonstrative phrases can replace the typically obligatory quantity phrases to form a grammatical scalar nu‐V construction with an activity verb. We argue that the solutions to these puzzles lie in three core meaning components of scalar nu‐V constructions: (a) scalar presupposition; (b) association with focus; (c) volitional requirement. Specifically, we propose that (a) scalar nu is a scalar verbal modifier that forms a verbal compound with its following verb; (b) scalar nu introduces a scalar presupposition (based on the scalar associate) and the volitional requirement in scalar nu‐V constructions. If our analysis is correct, it suggests that the scalarity of a construction may come from a verbal modifier (e.g., scalar nu) in the formation of a compound verb, thereby enhancing our understanding of the compositionality of compound verbs in natural language.
本文研究了台湾普通话中标量 nu-V 结构的三个困惑:(a) 标量 nu-V 结构与状态/成就动词不相容,无论是否有数量短语;(b) 当与活动动词结合时,标量 nu-V 结构在突发语境中似乎必须包含数量短语;(c) 在适当的语境支持下,裸名词和指示词组可以取代典型的强制性数量短语,与活动动词构成语法上的标量 nu-V 结构。我们认为,这些难题的解决之道在于标量 nu-V 结构的三个核心意义成分:(a) 标量预设;(b) 与焦点的关联;(c) 意志要求。具体来说,我们提出:(a) 标量 nu 是标量动词修饰语,与后面的动词构成动词复合词;(b) 标量 nu 在标量 nu-V 结构中引入了标量预设(基于标量关联)和意志要求。如果我们的分析是正确的,那么它表明结构的标量性可能来自复合动词构成中的动词修饰语(如标量 nu),从而加深了我们对自然语言中复合动词构成性的理解。
{"title":"On the scalarity of nu‐V constructions in Taiwan Mandarin","authors":"Yi‐Hsun Chen, C.‐T. Tim Chou, Po Ju Shen","doi":"10.1111/stul.12244","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1111/stul.12244","url":null,"abstract":"This paper investigates three puzzles concerning scalar nu‐V constructions in Taiwan Mandarin: (a) a scalar nu‐V construction is incompatible with state/achievement verbs, irrespective of the presence of a quantity phrase; (b) when combined with activity verbs, the inclusion of a quantity phrase appears obligatory for scalar nu‐V constructions in an out‐of‐the‐blue context; (c) under appropriate contextual support, bare nouns and demonstrative phrases can replace the typically obligatory quantity phrases to form a grammatical scalar nu‐V construction with an activity verb. We argue that the solutions to these puzzles lie in three core meaning components of scalar nu‐V constructions: (a) scalar presupposition; (b) association with focus; (c) volitional requirement. Specifically, we propose that (a) scalar nu is a scalar verbal modifier that forms a verbal compound with its following verb; (b) scalar nu introduces a scalar presupposition (based on the scalar associate) and the volitional requirement in scalar nu‐V constructions. If our analysis is correct, it suggests that the scalarity of a construction may come from a verbal modifier (e.g., scalar nu) in the formation of a compound verb, thereby enhancing our understanding of the compositionality of compound verbs in natural language.","PeriodicalId":46179,"journal":{"name":"STUDIA LINGUISTICA","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.4,"publicationDate":"2024-07-10","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141660749","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"文学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
WE…WITH ANNA: THE INCLUSORY PLURAL PRONOMINAL CONSTRUCTION IN FINNISH AND FENNO‐SWEDISH* 我们......与安娜:芬兰语和芬兰-瑞典语中的包含复数的前置词结构*
IF 0.6 3区 文学 N/A LANGUAGE & LINGUISTICS Pub Date : 2024-06-21 DOI: 10.1111/stul.12233
Klaus Kurki
This article provides a syntactic analysis of the inclusory plural pronominal construction in Fenno‐Swedish and Finnish. In this construction, a plural pronoun has a singular reading: vimed Anna (literally “we …with Anna”) means ‘Anna and I’. In addition to the plural pronoun, the construction includes a comitative PP. Similar constructions can be found in several other languages, especially in the eastern parts of Europe which suggests it is an areal feature. The structural diversity of the construction in Fenno‐Swedish and Finnish seems to require an analysis that differs from previous analyses of other languages. Instead of a derivation involving movements, the study suggests that the analysis must employ an unvalued feature. In some well‐formed examples, the necessary movements would be far too complex for an appealing explanation. Additionally, the Fenno‐Swedish construction seems to allow an insight into a degree of development where the construction has not necessarily been fully grammaticalised.
本文对芬兰语和瑞典语中的包含式复数代词结构进行了句法分析。在该结构中,复数代词具有单数读音:vi ...med Anna(字面意思为 "我们......和 Anna")表示 "Anna 和我"。除了复数代词外,该结构还包含一个连词 PP。类似的结构在其他几种语言中也能找到,尤其是在欧洲东部地区,这表明这是一个地区性特征。芬诺-瑞典语和芬兰语中这种结构的多样性似乎要求我们进行不同于以往对其他语言的分析。研究表明,这种分析必须使用一种无价值的特征,而不是涉及动作的派生。在一些结构良好的例子中,必要的动作过于复杂,无法做出吸引人的解释。此外,Fenno-Swedish 结构似乎可以让人洞察到该结构不一定完全语法化的发展程度。
{"title":"WE…WITH ANNA: THE INCLUSORY PLURAL PRONOMINAL CONSTRUCTION IN FINNISH AND FENNO‐SWEDISH*","authors":"Klaus Kurki","doi":"10.1111/stul.12233","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1111/stul.12233","url":null,"abstract":"This article provides a syntactic analysis of the inclusory plural pronominal construction in Fenno‐Swedish and Finnish. In this construction, a plural pronoun has a singular reading: <jats:italic>vi</jats:italic> …<jats:italic>med Anna</jats:italic> (literally “we …with Anna”) means ‘Anna and I’. In addition to the plural pronoun, the construction includes a comitative PP. Similar constructions can be found in several other languages, especially in the eastern parts of Europe which suggests it is an areal feature. The structural diversity of the construction in Fenno‐Swedish and Finnish seems to require an analysis that differs from previous analyses of other languages. Instead of a derivation involving movements, the study suggests that the analysis must employ an unvalued feature. In some well‐formed examples, the necessary movements would be far too complex for an appealing explanation. Additionally, the Fenno‐Swedish construction seems to allow an insight into a degree of development where the construction has not necessarily been fully grammaticalised.","PeriodicalId":46179,"journal":{"name":"STUDIA LINGUISTICA","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.6,"publicationDate":"2024-06-21","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141510824","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"文学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
CLAUSE TYPING IN MAIN POLAR QUESTIONS: EVIDENCE FROM ITALO‐ROMANCE 主极性问题中的分句类型:来自意大利语-浪漫的证据
IF 0.6 3区 文学 Q2 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2024-06-05 DOI: 10.1111/stul.12239
N. Munaro
This article investigates the morpho‐syntactic marking of main polar questions, which is achieved across Italo‐Romance by crosslinguistically different strategies. I argue that the interpretation of main polar questions is related to the activation of a dedicated functional head that encodes the relevant formal feature within the left‐periphery. In particular, I explore the possibility that the process of clause typing in unembedded yes/no questions is linked in Italo‐Romance to the activation of a functional projection located in the right periphery of the CP‐layer, which I call Polarity Phrase. This hypothesis relies on the intuition that negation and affirmation can be reduced to a more abstract category encoding the open polarity of the sentence, which can be underspecified for either negative or positive value.
本文研究了主极性疑问句的形态句法标记,在意大利-罗马尼亚语中,这种标记是通过跨语言的不同策略实现的。我认为,主极性疑问句的解释与在左外周激活编码相关形式特征的专用功能头有关。我特别探讨了这样一种可能性,即在意大利语-罗曼语中,无嵌入式 "是"/"否 "问题中的分句类型化过程与激活位于CP层右侧外围的功能投射有关,我将其称为 "极性句"。这一假设依赖于这样一种直觉,即否定和肯定可以简化为一个更抽象的类别,它编码句子的开放极性,而句子的开放极性既可以是否定的,也可以是肯定的。
{"title":"CLAUSE TYPING IN MAIN POLAR QUESTIONS: EVIDENCE FROM ITALO‐ROMANCE","authors":"N. Munaro","doi":"10.1111/stul.12239","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1111/stul.12239","url":null,"abstract":"This article investigates the morpho‐syntactic marking of main polar questions, which is achieved across Italo‐Romance by crosslinguistically different strategies. I argue that the interpretation of main polar questions is related to the activation of a dedicated functional head that encodes the relevant formal feature within the left‐periphery. In particular, I explore the possibility that the process of clause typing in unembedded yes/no questions is linked in Italo‐Romance to the activation of a functional projection located in the right periphery of the CP‐layer, which I call Polarity Phrase. This hypothesis relies on the intuition that negation and affirmation can be reduced to a more abstract category encoding the open polarity of the sentence, which can be underspecified for either negative or positive value.","PeriodicalId":46179,"journal":{"name":"STUDIA LINGUISTICA","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.6,"publicationDate":"2024-06-05","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141386923","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"文学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Tonal Phonotactics in Southern Min 闽南语的声调音韵学
IF 0.6 3区 文学 Q2 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2024-05-24 DOI: 10.1111/stul.12235
Yishan Huang
This paper is the first to explore tonal phonotactics in the world's natural languages. Zhangzhou Southern Min is theoretically assumed to have 7320 possible syllables but more than 71% of them are not empirically attested. Each lexical tone is logically possible to generate 915 syllables; however, the attested number only ranges from 98 syllables under tone 8 to 392 under tone 1. This study bases on a large corpus to explore how individual tones behave in the formation of attestable syllables, in what way tonal phonotactics occur and what mechanisms have trigged phonotactic constraints from both synchronic and diachronic factors. This study substantially stretches and advances our knowledge of tonal phonotactics as an important phonology phenomenon in this language. The exploration is supposed to serve as a model for thorough investigations of tonal phonotactics in Sinitic languages, shedding important light on the generalization of areal characteristics in Asia that possess rich and complex tonal contrasts. The study also contributes vital linguistics data to the typology of phonotactics in human languages, while enlightening a research direction of using experimental methods to model phonotactic restrictions in speakers' mental grammar and language practice.
本文首次探讨了世界自然语言中的声调音韵学。理论上,漳州闽南语可能有 7320 个音节,但其中超过 71% 没有得到实证。从逻辑上讲,每个词调可产生 915 个音节,但有据可查的音节数仅从音调 8 下的 98 个到音调 1 下的 392 个不等。本研究以一个大型语料库为基础,探讨了单个声调在可考证音节的形成过程中的表现,声调音位的发生方式,以及同步和非同步因素触发音位制约的机制。这项研究极大地扩展和推进了我们对该语言中一个重要语音现象--声调音位互动的认识。这一探索有望成为深入研究汉族语言中声调音韵学的典范,为亚洲地区拥有丰富而复杂的声调对比的区域特征的普遍化提供重要的启示。该研究还为人类语言的音位战术类型学提供了重要的语言学数据,同时也为使用实验方法模拟说话者心理语法和语言实践中的音位战术限制提供了研究方向上的启示。
{"title":"Tonal Phonotactics in Southern Min","authors":"Yishan Huang","doi":"10.1111/stul.12235","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1111/stul.12235","url":null,"abstract":"This paper is the first to explore tonal phonotactics in the world's natural languages. Zhangzhou Southern Min is theoretically assumed to have 7320 possible syllables but more than 71% of them are not empirically attested. Each lexical tone is logically possible to generate 915 syllables; however, the attested number only ranges from 98 syllables under tone 8 to 392 under tone 1. This study bases on a large corpus to explore how individual tones behave in the formation of attestable syllables, in what way tonal phonotactics occur and what mechanisms have trigged phonotactic constraints from both synchronic and diachronic factors. This study substantially stretches and advances our knowledge of tonal phonotactics as an important phonology phenomenon in this language. The exploration is supposed to serve as a model for thorough investigations of tonal phonotactics in Sinitic languages, shedding important light on the generalization of areal characteristics in Asia that possess rich and complex tonal contrasts. The study also contributes vital linguistics data to the typology of phonotactics in human languages, while enlightening a research direction of using experimental methods to model phonotactic restrictions in speakers' mental grammar and language practice.","PeriodicalId":46179,"journal":{"name":"STUDIA LINGUISTICA","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.6,"publicationDate":"2024-05-24","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141098741","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"文学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
A CORPUS‐DRIVEN STUDY OF THE CORRELATION BETWEEN THE SYNTACTIC COMBINATION STRENGTH AND THE SEMANTIC CONTRIBUTION IN THE ENGLISH “V‐TO‐V” CONSTRUCTION 关于英语 "v-to-v "结构中句法组合强度与语义贡献之间相关性的语料库驱动研究
IF 0.6 3区 文学 Q2 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2024-05-21 DOI: 10.1111/stul.12234
Yalin Zhang, Qingshun He
This paper conducted a corpus‐driven study on the syntactic and semantic features of the first verb and the to‐infinitive in the “V‐to‐V” construction. It was found that the syntactic combination strength of the “V‐to‐V” construction and the semantic contribution of the first verb are in general negatively correlated. The “V‐to‐V” construction can, therefore, be classified into three types: (1) Hypotactic clause complex of expansion, where the two verbs in the “V‐to‐V” construction construe the two processes, with weak syntactic combination and high semantic contribution of the first verb; (2) Single verbal group, where the two verbs construe one process with the first verb being auxiliary, with strong syntactic combination and low semantic contribution of the first verb; (3) Hypotactic clause complex of projection, where the two verbs construe two processes, with flexible syntactic combination and high semantic contribution of the first verb. Such correlation can thus form a cline from hypotactic clause complex of expansion through hypotactic clause complex of projection to single verbal groups.
本文以语料库为驱动,研究了 "V-to-V "结构中第一动词和至动名词的句法和语义特征。研究发现,"V-to-V "结构的句法组合强度和第一动词的语义贡献一般呈负相关。因此,"V-to-V "结构可分为三种类型:(1) "V-to-V "结构中的两个动词构成两个过程,句法结合较弱,第一个动词的语义贡献较高;(2)单动词组,两个动词构成一个过程,第一个动词是助动词,句法结合较强,第一个动词的语义贡献较低;(3) "V-to-V "结构中的两个动词构成两个过程,句法结合灵活,第一个动词的语义贡献较高。因此,这种相关性可以形成一条从扩展假言句群到投射假言句群再到单一动词群的线索。
{"title":"A CORPUS‐DRIVEN STUDY OF THE CORRELATION BETWEEN THE SYNTACTIC COMBINATION STRENGTH AND THE SEMANTIC CONTRIBUTION IN THE ENGLISH “V‐TO‐V” CONSTRUCTION","authors":"Yalin Zhang, Qingshun He","doi":"10.1111/stul.12234","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1111/stul.12234","url":null,"abstract":"This paper conducted a corpus‐driven study on the syntactic and semantic features of the first verb and the to‐infinitive in the “V‐to‐V” construction. It was found that the syntactic combination strength of the “V‐to‐V” construction and the semantic contribution of the first verb are in general negatively correlated. The “V‐to‐V” construction can, therefore, be classified into three types: (1) Hypotactic clause complex of expansion, where the two verbs in the “V‐to‐V” construction construe the two processes, with weak syntactic combination and high semantic contribution of the first verb; (2) Single verbal group, where the two verbs construe one process with the first verb being auxiliary, with strong syntactic combination and low semantic contribution of the first verb; (3) Hypotactic clause complex of projection, where the two verbs construe two processes, with flexible syntactic combination and high semantic contribution of the first verb. Such correlation can thus form a cline from hypotactic clause complex of expansion through hypotactic clause complex of projection to single verbal groups.","PeriodicalId":46179,"journal":{"name":"STUDIA LINGUISTICA","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.6,"publicationDate":"2024-05-21","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141118451","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"文学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
NOMINALIZED CLAUSES AND DISCOURSE‐GIVENNESS: EXPERIMENTAL EVIDENCE FROM RUSSIAN* 名词化从句和话语给定:来自俄语的实验证据*
IF 0.6 3区 文学 Q2 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2024-05-11 DOI: 10.1111/stul.12232
Mikhail Knyazev, Ekaterina Rudaleva
In a seminal paper, Kiparsky & Kiparsky (1970) proposed a two‐way correspondence between presuppositionality of clauses and nominal structure. The proposal remains highly relevant to current research (a.o. Kastner 2015, Bochnak & Hanink 2022), despite the existence of counterexamples in both directions. In this paper, we examine Russian nominalized clauses to show that presuppositionality is indeed neither necessary nor sufficient for nominalization. However, instead of completely discarding the correspondence between presuppositionality and nominalization, we argue for a weaker “preference” hypothesis, whereby presuppositional, or discourse‐given, contexts are associated with a higher likelihood of nominalization compared to discourse‐new contexts. We provide support for the preference hypothesis based on four experimental studies, a forced‐choice and a givenness‐rating study using matrix negation as a proxy for givenness and a forced‐choice and a sentence completion study directly manipulating the discourse context. We suggest a tentative explanation for the preference hypothesis in terms of definiteness/familiarity marking.
Kiparsky & Kiparsky(1970 年)在一篇开创性论文中提出了分句的预设性与名词结构之间的双向对应关系。尽管在两个方向上都存在反例,但这一提议仍然与当前的研究高度相关(a.o. Kastner 2015, Bochnak & Hanink 2022)。在本文中,我们考察了俄语的名词化分句,以证明预设性对于名词化确实既不是必要的,也不是充分的。然而,我们并没有完全抛弃预设性与名词化之间的对应关系,而是提出了一个较弱的 "偏好 "假设,即与话语新语境相比,预设性或话语给定语境与更高的名词化可能性相关联。我们基于四项实验研究为偏好假说提供了支持,其中一项是强迫选择和给定评价研究,使用矩阵否定作为给定的替代;另一项是强迫选择和句子完成研究,直接操纵话语语境。我们建议从定义/熟悉标记的角度对偏好假说进行初步解释。
{"title":"NOMINALIZED CLAUSES AND DISCOURSE‐GIVENNESS: EXPERIMENTAL EVIDENCE FROM RUSSIAN*","authors":"Mikhail Knyazev, Ekaterina Rudaleva","doi":"10.1111/stul.12232","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1111/stul.12232","url":null,"abstract":"In a seminal paper, Kiparsky &amp; Kiparsky (1970) proposed a two‐way correspondence between presuppositionality of clauses and nominal structure. The proposal remains highly relevant to current research (a.o. Kastner 2015, Bochnak &amp; Hanink 2022), despite the existence of counterexamples in both directions. In this paper, we examine Russian nominalized clauses to show that presuppositionality is indeed neither necessary nor sufficient for nominalization. However, instead of completely discarding the correspondence between presuppositionality and nominalization, we argue for a weaker “preference” hypothesis, whereby presuppositional, or discourse‐given, contexts are associated with a higher likelihood of nominalization compared to discourse‐new contexts. We provide support for the preference hypothesis based on four experimental studies, a forced‐choice and a givenness‐rating study using matrix negation as a proxy for givenness and a forced‐choice and a sentence completion study directly manipulating the discourse context. We suggest a tentative explanation for the preference hypothesis in terms of definiteness/familiarity marking.","PeriodicalId":46179,"journal":{"name":"STUDIA LINGUISTICA","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.6,"publicationDate":"2024-05-11","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140926186","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"文学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
STUDIA LINGUISTICA
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1