首页 > 最新文献

I-Perception最新文献

英文 中文
Heading perception from optic flow is affected by heading distribution. 光流的头球感知受头球分布的影响。
IF 1.9 4区 心理学 Q2 Medicine Pub Date : 2022-11-22 eCollection Date: 2022-11-01 DOI: 10.1177/20416695221133406
Qi Sun, Ruifang Yan, Jingyi Wang, Xinyu Li

Recent studies have revealed a central tendency in the perception of physical features. That is, the perceived feature was biased toward the mean of recently experienced features (i.e., previous feature distribution). However, no study explored whether the central tendency was in heading perception or not. In this study, we conducted three experiments to answer this question. The results showed that the perceived heading was not biased toward the mean of the previous heading distribution, suggesting that the central tendency was not in heading perception. However, the perceived headings were overall biased toward the left side, where headings rarely appeared in the right-heavied distribution (Experiment 3), suggesting that heading perception from optic flow was affected by previously seen headings. It indicated that the participants learned the heading distributions and used them to adjust their heading perception. Our study revealed that heading perception from optic flow was not purely perceptual and that postperceptual stages (e.g., attention and working memory) might be involved in the heading perception from optic flow.

最近的研究揭示了对身体特征感知的一种集中倾向。也就是说,感知到的特征偏向于最近经历过的特征的平均值(即,以前的特征分布)。然而,没有研究探讨集中倾向是否存在于头球知觉中。在这项研究中,我们进行了三个实验来回答这个问题。结果表明,头球知觉并不偏向于前一次头球分布的均值,说明头球知觉的集中倾向不存在。然而,感知到的标题总体上偏向左侧,标题很少出现在右重分布中(实验3),这表明光流的标题感知受到先前看到的标题的影响。这表明参与者学习了头球分布,并利用它们来调整他们的头球感知。我们的研究表明,由光流产生的头球知觉不是纯粹的知觉,而后知觉阶段(如注意和工作记忆)可能参与了由光流产生的头球知觉。
{"title":"Heading perception from optic flow is affected by heading distribution.","authors":"Qi Sun,&nbsp;Ruifang Yan,&nbsp;Jingyi Wang,&nbsp;Xinyu Li","doi":"10.1177/20416695221133406","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1177/20416695221133406","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Recent studies have revealed a central tendency in the perception of physical features. That is, the perceived feature was biased toward the mean of recently experienced features (i.e., previous feature distribution). However, no study explored whether the central tendency was in heading perception or not. In this study, we conducted three experiments to answer this question. The results showed that the perceived heading was not biased toward the mean of the previous heading distribution, suggesting that the central tendency was not in heading perception. However, the perceived headings were overall biased toward the left side, where headings rarely appeared in the right-heavied distribution (Experiment 3), suggesting that heading perception from optic flow was affected by previously seen headings. It indicated that the participants learned the heading distributions and used them to adjust their heading perception. Our study revealed that heading perception from optic flow was not purely perceptual and that postperceptual stages (e.g., attention and working memory) might be involved in the heading perception from optic flow.</p>","PeriodicalId":47194,"journal":{"name":"I-Perception","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.9,"publicationDate":"2022-11-22","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC9706071/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"40568495","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 3
The slime hand illusion: Nonproprioceptive ownership distortion specific to the skin region. 粘液手错觉:皮肤区域特有的非本体感觉所有权扭曲。
IF 1.9 4区 心理学 Q2 Medicine Pub Date : 2022-11-15 eCollection Date: 2022-11-01 DOI: 10.1177/20416695221137731
Kenri Kodaka, Yutaro Sato, Kento Imai

The "slime hand illusion" is a simple and robust technique that uses mirror-visual feedback to produce a nonproprioceptive ownership distortion. The illusion can be easily evoked by the participant watching the experimenter pinching and pulling a chunk of slime in a mirror while the participant's hand, hidden behind the mirror, is similarly manipulated. This procedure produces a feeling of one of their fingers or the skin of their whole hand being stretched or deformed in a similar way to the visible slime. A public experiment found that more than 90% of participants reported a strong sense of skin or finger stretching. This report details a laboratory experiment performed to characterize the mechanisms behind the illusion more robustly. It reproduced this result and found that participants experienced a drift in their sense of skin location of approximately 30 cm on average, which is beyond the conventionally accepted range of proprioceptive drift.

“粘液手错觉”是一种简单而有力的技术,它使用镜像视觉反馈来产生非本体感觉的所有权扭曲。当参与者看到实验者在镜子里捏拽一大块黏液,而自己藏在镜子后面的手也被类似地操纵时,这种错觉很容易被唤起。这个过程会产生一种感觉,他们的一个手指或整个手的皮肤被拉伸或变形,与可见的粘液类似。一项公开实验发现,超过90%的参与者报告有强烈的皮肤或手指拉伸感。本报告详细介绍了一项实验室实验,以更有力地表征错觉背后的机制。它再现了这一结果,并发现参与者的皮肤位置感平均漂移了约30厘米,这超出了传统上接受的本体感觉漂移范围。
{"title":"The slime hand illusion: Nonproprioceptive ownership distortion specific to the skin region.","authors":"Kenri Kodaka,&nbsp;Yutaro Sato,&nbsp;Kento Imai","doi":"10.1177/20416695221137731","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1177/20416695221137731","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The \"slime hand illusion\" is a simple and robust technique that uses mirror-visual feedback to produce a nonproprioceptive ownership distortion. The illusion can be easily evoked by the participant watching the experimenter pinching and pulling a chunk of slime in a mirror while the participant's hand, hidden behind the mirror, is similarly manipulated. This procedure produces a feeling of one of their fingers or the skin of their whole hand being stretched or deformed in a similar way to the visible slime. A public experiment found that more than 90% of participants reported a strong sense of skin or finger stretching. This report details a laboratory experiment performed to characterize the mechanisms behind the illusion more robustly. It reproduced this result and found that participants experienced a drift in their sense of skin location of approximately 30 cm on average, which is beyond the conventionally accepted range of proprioceptive drift.</p>","PeriodicalId":47194,"journal":{"name":"I-Perception","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.9,"publicationDate":"2022-11-15","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC9669698/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"40477857","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 2
Individual fixation tendencies in person viewing generalize from images to videos. 从图像到视频,个人观看时的固着倾向具有普遍性。
IF 2.4 4区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2022-11-04 eCollection Date: 2022-11-01 DOI: 10.1177/20416695221128844
Maximilian D Broda, Benjamin de Haas

Fixation behavior toward persons in static scenes varies considerably between individuals. However, it is unclear whether these differences generalize to dynamic stimuli. Here, we examined individual differences in the distribution of gaze across seven person features (i.e. body and face parts) in static and dynamic scenes. Forty-four participants freely viewed 700 complex static scenes followed by eight director-cut videos (28,925 frames). We determined the presence of person features using hand-delineated pixel masks (images) and Deep Neural Networks (videos). Results replicated highly consistent individual differences in fixation tendencies for all person features in static scenes and revealed that these tendencies generalize to videos. Individual fixation behavior for both, images and videos, fell into two anticorrelated clusters representing the tendency to fixate faces versus bodies. These results corroborate a low-dimensional space for individual gaze biases toward persons and show they generalize from images to videos.

在静态场景中,不同个体对人物的固定行为差异很大。然而,这些差异是否会扩展到动态刺激中还不清楚。在此,我们研究了在静态和动态场景中,个体对七个人物特征(即身体和脸部)的注视分布差异。44 名参与者自由观看了 700 个复杂的静态场景和 8 个导演剪辑视频(28925 帧)。我们使用手工划定的像素掩码(图像)和深度神经网络(视频)来确定人物特征的存在。结果表明,在静态场景中,所有人物特征的固定倾向都存在高度一致的个体差异,并且这些倾向在视频中也具有普遍性。图像和视频中的个体固着行为分为两个反相关群组,分别代表固着人脸和身体的倾向。这些结果证实了个人对人物的注视偏向的低维空间,并表明这些偏向会从图像泛化到视频。
{"title":"Individual fixation tendencies in person viewing generalize from images to videos.","authors":"Maximilian D Broda, Benjamin de Haas","doi":"10.1177/20416695221128844","DOIUrl":"10.1177/20416695221128844","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Fixation behavior toward persons in static scenes varies considerably between individuals. However, it is unclear whether these differences generalize to dynamic stimuli. Here, we examined individual differences in the distribution of gaze across seven person features (i.e. body and face parts) in static and dynamic scenes. Forty-four participants freely viewed 700 complex static scenes followed by eight director-cut videos (28,925 frames). We determined the presence of person features using hand-delineated pixel masks (images) and Deep Neural Networks (videos). Results replicated highly consistent individual differences in fixation tendencies for all person features in static scenes and revealed that these tendencies generalize to videos. Individual fixation behavior for both, images and videos, fell into two anticorrelated clusters representing the tendency to fixate faces versus bodies. These results corroborate a low-dimensional space for individual gaze biases toward persons and show they generalize from images to videos.</p>","PeriodicalId":47194,"journal":{"name":"I-Perception","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.4,"publicationDate":"2022-11-04","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC9638695/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"40456671","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Memory decay enhances central bias in time perception. 记忆衰退增强了时间感知的中枢偏差。
IF 1.9 4区 心理学 Q2 Medicine Pub Date : 2022-11-01 DOI: 10.1177/20416695221140428
Natsuki Ueda, Kanji Tanaka, Katsumi Watanabe

Temporal expectations are essential for appropriately interacting with the environment, but they can be biased. This tendency, called central bias, places higher weights on expected rather than actual duration distributions when perceiving incoming sensory stimuli. In particular, the central bias is strengthened in order to decrease total response error when incoming sensory stimuli are unclear. In the present study, we investigated whether the central bias was enhanced via memory decay. For this, we used a delayed reproduction task, manipulating retention periods by introducing delays between the sample interval and the reproduction phase (0.4, 2, 4 s in Experiment 1; 0.4, 2, 8 s in Experiments 2 and 3). Through three experiments, we found the gradual strengthening of the central bias as a function of the retention period (i.e., short-term memory decay). This suggests that the integration of temporal expectation, generated from past trials and stored sensory stimuli, in a current trial occurs in the reproduction phase in the delayed reproduction task.

时间预期对于适当地与环境进行交互至关重要,但它们可能存在偏差。这种倾向被称为中心偏差,当感知到传入的感官刺激时,它更看重预期而不是实际的持续时间分布。特别是,当传入的感觉刺激不清楚时,为了减少总反应误差,中心偏差得到加强。在本研究中,我们研究了中心偏差是否通过记忆衰退而增强。为此,我们使用了延迟复制任务,通过在样本间隔和复制阶段之间引入延迟来操纵保留期(实验1中为0.4、2、4秒;通过三个实验,我们发现中心偏差作为保留时间(即短期记忆衰减)的函数逐渐增强。这表明,在当前的试验中,由过去的试验和储存的感官刺激产生的时间预期的整合发生在延迟繁殖任务的繁殖阶段。
{"title":"Memory decay enhances central bias in time perception.","authors":"Natsuki Ueda,&nbsp;Kanji Tanaka,&nbsp;Katsumi Watanabe","doi":"10.1177/20416695221140428","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1177/20416695221140428","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Temporal expectations are essential for appropriately interacting with the environment, but they can be biased. This tendency, called central bias, places higher weights on expected rather than actual duration distributions when perceiving incoming sensory stimuli. In particular, the central bias is strengthened in order to decrease total response error when incoming sensory stimuli are unclear. In the present study, we investigated whether the central bias was enhanced via memory decay. For this, we used a delayed reproduction task, manipulating retention periods by introducing delays between the sample interval and the reproduction phase (0.4, 2, 4 s in Experiment 1; 0.4, 2, 8 s in Experiments 2 and 3). Through three experiments, we found the gradual strengthening of the central bias as a function of the retention period (i.e., short-term memory decay). This suggests that the integration of temporal expectation, generated from past trials and stored sensory stimuli, in a current trial occurs in the reproduction phase in the delayed reproduction task.</p>","PeriodicalId":47194,"journal":{"name":"I-Perception","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.9,"publicationDate":"2022-11-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC9730004/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10391841","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The aesthetics of frieze patterns: Effects of symmetry, motif, and element size. 雕花图案的美学:对称、图案和元素大小的影响。
IF 1.9 4区 心理学 Q2 Medicine Pub Date : 2022-10-27 eCollection Date: 2022-09-01 DOI: 10.1177/20416695221131112
Jay Friedenberg, Preston Martin, Naomi Uy, Mackenzie Kvapil

Frieze patterns appear frequently in architectural designs and ornamental patterning but their aesthetic qualities have never been studied experimentally. In the first experiment, 39 undergraduates used a seven-point rating scale to assess the perceived beauty of the seven basic frieze types presented at a horizontal orientation. The friezes consisted of individual curved and linear motifs as well as random textures. Friezes that filled the entire pattern region and which contained emergent global features were preferred the most. In a second experiment, we utilized horizontal texture friezes that were completely filled and which varied in size and number of elements. Participants preferred patterns with larger features, probably because they make detection of the symmetric transformations more visible. The frieze with the greatest number of symmetries was preferred most but symmetric complexity by itself could not completely account for the predicted preference ordering. In both studies, friezes containing horizontal mirrors (translation, 180° rotation, horizontal mirror, vertical mirror, and glide reflection and translation, horizontal mirror, and glide reflection) were preferred far more than any other condition. Horizontal symmetry may enhance perceived beauty in these cases because it runs parallel to and so emphasizes the overall frieze orientation.

在建筑设计和装饰图案中频繁出现,但其美学特性从未被实验研究过。在第一个实验中,39名大学生使用一个7分制量表来评估在水平方向上呈现的七种基本frieze类型的感知美。中楣由单独的曲线和线性图案以及随机纹理组成。填充整个图案区域并包含突现的全局特征的条纹最受青睐。在第二个实验中,我们使用了完全填充的水平纹理条纹,其大小和元素数量各不相同。参与者更喜欢具有较大特征的模式,可能是因为它们使对称转换的检测更明显。对称数量最多的饰条是最优先的饰条,但对称复杂性本身不能完全解释预测的优先顺序。在这两项研究中,包含水平反射镜(平移、180°旋转、水平反射镜、垂直反射镜、滑动反射镜和平移、水平反射镜、滑动反射镜)的楣板比任何其他条件都更受欢迎。在这些情况下,水平对称可能会增强感知美感,因为它平行于并因此强调了整个楣板的方向。
{"title":"The aesthetics of frieze patterns: Effects of symmetry, motif, and element size.","authors":"Jay Friedenberg,&nbsp;Preston Martin,&nbsp;Naomi Uy,&nbsp;Mackenzie Kvapil","doi":"10.1177/20416695221131112","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1177/20416695221131112","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Frieze patterns appear frequently in architectural designs and ornamental patterning but their aesthetic qualities have never been studied experimentally. In the first experiment, 39 undergraduates used a seven-point rating scale to assess the perceived beauty of the seven basic frieze types presented at a horizontal orientation. The friezes consisted of individual curved and linear motifs as well as random textures. Friezes that filled the entire pattern region and which contained emergent global features were preferred the most. In a second experiment, we utilized horizontal texture friezes that were completely filled and which varied in size and number of elements. Participants preferred patterns with larger features, probably because they make detection of the symmetric transformations more visible. The frieze with the greatest number of symmetries was preferred most but symmetric complexity by itself could not completely account for the predicted preference ordering. In both studies, friezes containing horizontal mirrors (translation, 180° rotation, horizontal mirror, vertical mirror, and glide reflection and translation, horizontal mirror, and glide reflection) were preferred far more than any other condition. Horizontal symmetry may enhance perceived beauty in these cases because it runs parallel to and so emphasizes the overall frieze orientation.</p>","PeriodicalId":47194,"journal":{"name":"I-Perception","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.9,"publicationDate":"2022-10-27","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC9620142/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"40662550","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Origin of the ease of association of color names: Comparison between humans and AI. 颜色名称容易联想的起源:人类与人工智能的比较。
IF 1.9 4区 心理学 Q2 Medicine Pub Date : 2022-10-26 eCollection Date: 2022-09-01 DOI: 10.1177/20416695221131832
Hidehiko Komatsu, Ami Maeno, Eiji Watanabe

Rapid evolution of artificial intelligence (AI) based on deep neural networks has resulted in artificial systems such as generative pre-trained transformer 3 (GPT-3), which can generate human-like language. Such a system may provide a novel platform for studying how human perception is related to knowledge and the ability of language generation. We compared the frequency distribution of basic color terms in the answers of human subjects and GPT-3 when both were asked similar questions regarding color names associated with the letters of the alphabet. We found that GPT-3 generated basic color terms at a frequency very similar to that of human non-synaesthetes. A similar frequency was observed when color names associated with numerals were tested indicating that simple co-occurrence of alphabet and color word in the trained dataset cannot explain the results. We suggest that the proposed experimental framework using the latest AI models has the potential to explore the mechanisms of human perception.

以深度神经网络为基础的人工智能(AI)迅速发展,产生了可以生成类似人类语言的生成预训练变压器3 (GPT-3)等人工系统。这样的系统可以为研究人类感知与知识和语言生成能力的关系提供一个新的平台。当被问及与字母表中的字母相关的颜色名称时,我们比较了人类受试者和GPT-3的答案中基本颜色术语的频率分布。我们发现GPT-3产生基本颜色项的频率与人类非联觉者非常相似。当测试与数字相关的颜色名称时,也观察到类似的频率,这表明在训练数据集中,字母和颜色词的简单共现不能解释结果。我们认为,使用最新人工智能模型提出的实验框架具有探索人类感知机制的潜力。
{"title":"Origin of the ease of association of color names: Comparison between humans and AI.","authors":"Hidehiko Komatsu,&nbsp;Ami Maeno,&nbsp;Eiji Watanabe","doi":"10.1177/20416695221131832","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1177/20416695221131832","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Rapid evolution of artificial intelligence (AI) based on deep neural networks has resulted in artificial systems such as generative pre-trained transformer 3 (GPT-3), which can generate human-like language. Such a system may provide a novel platform for studying how human perception is related to knowledge and the ability of language generation. We compared the frequency distribution of basic color terms in the answers of human subjects and GPT-3 when both were asked similar questions regarding color names associated with the letters of the alphabet. We found that GPT-3 generated basic color terms at a frequency very similar to that of human non-synaesthetes. A similar frequency was observed when color names associated with numerals were tested indicating that simple co-occurrence of alphabet and color word in the trained dataset cannot explain the results. We suggest that the proposed experimental framework using the latest AI models has the potential to explore the mechanisms of human perception.</p>","PeriodicalId":47194,"journal":{"name":"I-Perception","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.9,"publicationDate":"2022-10-26","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC9623380/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"40453754","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Descriptions of a common belief in an 1813 Japanese beauty handbook regarding the influence of striped clothing on perceived body shape. 1813年一本日本美容手册中关于条纹衣服对体形的影响的描述。
IF 1.9 4区 心理学 Q2 Medicine Pub Date : 2022-10-12 eCollection Date: 2022-09-01 DOI: 10.1177/20416695221130779
Yuki Miyazaki, Kentaro Ishibashi

"Clothes with horizontal (or vertical) stripes are perceived as wider and shorter (slimmer and taller)." This belief is common yet inconsistent with the Helmholtz illusion. It has often attracted attention from researchers of perception. Despite the controversy among empirical studies, it is persistently supported by the general public. This article explores the early appearance of this common belief in Japan in historical records. Consequently, we discovered the descriptions of the common belief in a Japanese beauty handbook titled "Miyako Fuzoku Kewai Den [Cosmetic manners and customs in Edo]," published in 1813. In Japan, this belief was not born in modern times. Instead, it was established over 200 years ago, when vertical striped patterns on clothes were popularized.

“水平(或垂直)条纹的衣服会被认为更宽更短(更瘦更高)。”这种信念很普遍,但与亥姆霍兹错觉不一致。它经常引起知觉研究者的注意。尽管在实证研究中存在争议,但它一直得到公众的支持。本文在历史记载中探讨了这种普遍信仰在日本的早期出现。因此,我们在1813年出版的一本日本美容手册《宫古富佐科wai Den(江户的美容礼仪和习俗)》中发现了对这一共同信念的描述。在日本,这种信仰并非诞生于近代。相反,它是在200多年前建立的,当时衣服上的垂直条纹图案很流行。
{"title":"Descriptions of a common belief in an 1813 Japanese beauty handbook regarding the influence of striped clothing on perceived body shape.","authors":"Yuki Miyazaki,&nbsp;Kentaro Ishibashi","doi":"10.1177/20416695221130779","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1177/20416695221130779","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>\"Clothes with horizontal (or vertical) stripes are perceived as wider and shorter (slimmer and taller).\" This belief is common yet inconsistent with the Helmholtz illusion. It has often attracted attention from researchers of perception. Despite the controversy among empirical studies, it is persistently supported by the general public. This article explores the early appearance of this common belief in Japan in historical records. Consequently, we discovered the descriptions of the common belief in a Japanese beauty handbook titled \"<i>Miyako Fuzoku Kewai Den</i> [Cosmetic manners and customs in Edo],\" published in 1813. In Japan, this belief was not born in modern times. Instead, it was established over 200 years ago, when vertical striped patterns on clothes were popularized.</p>","PeriodicalId":47194,"journal":{"name":"I-Perception","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.9,"publicationDate":"2022-10-12","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC9558892/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"33513922","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Crossmodal correspondences between basic tastes and visual design features: A narrative historical review. 基本品味与视觉设计特征之间的跨模式对应:叙事性历史回顾。
IF 1.9 4区 心理学 Q2 Medicine Pub Date : 2022-10-11 eCollection Date: 2022-09-01 DOI: 10.1177/20416695221127325
Byron P Lee, Charles Spence

People tend to associate abstract visual features with basic taste qualities. This narrative historical review critically evaluates the literature on these associations, often referred to as crossmodal correspondences, between basic tastes and visual design features such as color hue and shape curvilinearity. The patterns, discrepancies, and evolution in the development of the research are highlighted while the mappings that have been reported to date are summarized. The review also reflects on issues of cross-cultural validity and deviations in the matching patterns that are observed when correspondences are assessed with actual tastants versus with verbal stimuli. The various theories that have been proposed to account for different classes of crossmodal correspondence are discussed, among which the statistical and affective (or emotional-mediation) accounts currently appear most promising. Several critical research questions for the future are presented to address the gaps that have been identified in the literature and help validate the popular theories on the origin and operations of visual-taste correspondences.

人们倾向于将抽象的视觉特征与基本的味觉品质联系起来。这篇叙事性的历史回顾批判性地评估了这些关联的文献,这些关联通常被称为跨模式对应,在基本品味和视觉设计特征(如色彩色调和形状曲线)之间。强调了研究发展中的模式、差异和演变,同时总结了迄今为止已报道的映射。这篇综述还反映了跨文化有效性和匹配模式偏差的问题,这些偏差是在实际品味与言语刺激评估对应关系时观察到的。已经提出的各种理论,以解释不同类别的跨模式对应进行了讨论,其中统计和情感(或情感调解)的帐户目前看来最有希望。提出了未来的几个关键研究问题,以解决文献中已确定的差距,并帮助验证关于视觉-味觉对应的起源和操作的流行理论。
{"title":"Crossmodal correspondences between basic tastes and visual design features: A narrative historical review.","authors":"Byron P Lee,&nbsp;Charles Spence","doi":"10.1177/20416695221127325","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1177/20416695221127325","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>People tend to associate abstract visual features with basic taste qualities. This narrative historical review critically evaluates the literature on these associations, often referred to as crossmodal correspondences, between basic tastes and visual design features such as color hue and shape curvilinearity. The patterns, discrepancies, and evolution in the development of the research are highlighted while the mappings that have been reported to date are summarized. The review also reflects on issues of cross-cultural validity and deviations in the matching patterns that are observed when correspondences are assessed with actual tastants versus with verbal stimuli. The various theories that have been proposed to account for different classes of crossmodal correspondence are discussed, among which the statistical and affective (or emotional-mediation) accounts currently appear most promising. Several critical research questions for the future are presented to address the gaps that have been identified in the literature and help validate the popular theories on the origin and operations of visual-taste correspondences.</p>","PeriodicalId":47194,"journal":{"name":"I-Perception","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.9,"publicationDate":"2022-10-11","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC9558874/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"33515416","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 7
The Poggendorff illusion in Ruben's Descent from the Cross in Antwerp: Does the illusion even matter? 鲁本《安特卫普十字架上的降临》中的波根多夫幻觉:这种幻觉重要吗?
IF 1.9 4区 心理学 Q2 Medicine Pub Date : 2022-10-11 eCollection Date: 2022-09-01 DOI: 10.1177/20416695221125879
Olga Daneyko, Natale Stucchi, Daniele Zavagno

Two experiments are described, the purpose of which was to investigate the presence of a misalignment illusion caused by Poggendorff-like conditions in two paintings by Peter Paul Rubens, both depicting the Descent from the Cross, one located in Antwerp (Belgium), the other in Lille (France). The first shows a geometrical misalignment made by Rubens in a minor detail, which is considered proof that the artist observed the Poggendorff illusion. The second painting, instead, shows a perfect geometrical alignment in a similar detail. In experiment 1, participants were asked to align a top segment to a lower one in two types of stimuli: a full-size digitally manipulated reproduction of the painting and a Poggendorff-like configuration that recalled the painting's lines displacement and tilt. Adjustments were performed from two distances, one up close (painting distance) and one from below and far (observation distance). Results confirmed the presence of the Poggendorff illusion, but mean adjustments significantly differed from the misalignment perpetrated by Rubens. Experiment 2 was set up in a similar fashion with the Lille painting. Results confirmed the presence of the Poggendorff illusion also in this painting; however, the alignment by Rubens coincides with the geometrical one. Results from both experiments do not support the claim that Rubens observed the Poggendorff illusion and therefore corrected for it in the Antwerp painting. An alternative account is discussed, which relates to the structural layout of the painting.

描述了两个实验,其目的是调查彼得·保罗·鲁本斯的两幅画中由波根多夫样条件引起的错位错觉的存在,这两幅画都描绘了从十字架上下来,一幅位于安特卫普(比利时),另一幅位于里尔(法国)。第一幅画显示了鲁本斯在一个小细节上的几何错位,这被认为是艺术家观察到波根多夫错觉的证据。相反,第二幅画在相似的细节上表现出完美的几何对齐。在实验1中,参与者被要求在两种类型的刺激下将顶部部分与较低的部分对齐:一种是全尺寸的数字操纵绘画复制品,另一种是波根多夫式的结构,让人回想起绘画线条的位移和倾斜。从两个距离进行调整,一个是近距离(绘画距离),一个是从下方和远处(观察距离)。结果证实了波根多夫错觉的存在,但平均调整明显不同于鲁本斯所犯的错位。实验二以类似里尔画的方式设置。结果证实了这幅画中也存在波根多夫错觉;然而,鲁本斯的排列与几何排列是一致的。两个实验的结果都不支持鲁本斯观察到波根多夫错觉并因此在安特卫普画中纠正它的说法。讨论了另一种说法,这与绘画的结构布局有关。
{"title":"The Poggendorff illusion in Ruben's Descent from the Cross in Antwerp: Does the illusion even matter?","authors":"Olga Daneyko,&nbsp;Natale Stucchi,&nbsp;Daniele Zavagno","doi":"10.1177/20416695221125879","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1177/20416695221125879","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Two experiments are described, the purpose of which was to investigate the presence of a misalignment illusion caused by Poggendorff-like conditions in two paintings by Peter Paul Rubens, both depicting the <i>Descent from the Cross</i>, one located in Antwerp (Belgium), the other in Lille (France). The first shows a geometrical misalignment made by Rubens in a minor detail, which is considered proof that the artist observed the Poggendorff illusion. The second painting, instead, shows a perfect geometrical alignment in a similar detail. In experiment 1, participants were asked to align a top segment to a lower one in two types of stimuli: a full-size digitally manipulated reproduction of the painting and a Poggendorff-like configuration that recalled the painting's lines displacement and tilt. Adjustments were performed from two distances, one up close (painting distance) and one from below and far (observation distance). Results confirmed the presence of the Poggendorff illusion, but mean adjustments significantly differed from the misalignment perpetrated by Rubens. Experiment 2 was set up in a similar fashion with the Lille painting. Results confirmed the presence of the Poggendorff illusion also in this painting; however, the alignment by Rubens coincides with the geometrical one. Results from both experiments do not support the claim that Rubens observed the Poggendorff illusion and therefore corrected for it in the Antwerp painting. An alternative account is discussed, which relates to the structural layout of the painting.</p>","PeriodicalId":47194,"journal":{"name":"I-Perception","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.9,"publicationDate":"2022-10-11","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC9561643/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"33515417","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
An introduction to the special issue: The ecological approach of James J. Gibson: 40 years later. 特刊导言:詹姆斯-J-吉布森的生态学方法:40 年之后。
IF 2.4 4区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2022-10-06 eCollection Date: 2022-09-01 DOI: 10.1177/20416695221123865
H A Sedgwick, Brian Rogers
{"title":"An introduction to the special issue: The ecological approach of James J. Gibson: 40 years later.","authors":"H A Sedgwick, Brian Rogers","doi":"10.1177/20416695221123865","DOIUrl":"10.1177/20416695221123865","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":47194,"journal":{"name":"I-Perception","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.4,"publicationDate":"2022-10-06","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC9549194/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"33501828","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
I-Perception
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1