首页 > 最新文献

I-Perception最新文献

英文 中文
The kinetic Orbison illusion. 动感的野牛幻觉。
IF 1.9 4区 心理学 Q2 Medicine Pub Date : 2023-09-06 eCollection Date: 2023-09-01 DOI: 10.1177/20416695231196979
Hikari Matsunaga, Hiroyuki Ito, Tama Kanematsu

In the typical Orbison illusion, the sides of a square placed on concentric circles appear to be bending toward the center of the circles. We report a motion version of the Orbison illusion (namely, the kinetic Orbison illusion). When a dot moves along a square trajectory against a background of concentric circles, the sides of the trajectory appear to bend toward the center and the corners appear to be sharpened. In the present study, observers adjusted the shape of a comparison stimulus to the shape of the perceived trajectory by bending the sides. The amount of illusion was operationally defined as the largest discrepancy between the square and adjusted shape in the comparison stimulus. It was found that the illusory bending was more than twice the static Orbison illusion and reached a maximum of 7.3% of the length of one side. Experiments including a comparison between fixation and pursuit conditions revealed that the main cause of the kinetic illusion was not motion streaks of the dot crossing background circles. We propose an alternative hypothesis based on induced motion generated by background circle motion, the direction of which is misperceived owing to the aperture problem.

在典型的Orbison错觉中,放置在同心圆上的正方形的侧面似乎向圆的中心弯曲。我们报道了野牛错觉的运动版本(即运动野牛错觉)。当一个点在同心圆的背景下沿着正方形轨迹移动时,轨迹的边看起来向中心弯曲,角看起来更尖锐。在本研究中,观察者通过弯曲侧面,将比较刺激的形状调整为感知轨迹的形状。错觉的量在操作上被定义为比较刺激中正方形和调整后的形状之间的最大差异。研究发现,虚幻的弯曲是静态Orbison错觉的两倍多,最大达到一侧长度的7.3%。包括注视和追逐条件之间的比较在内的实验表明,运动错觉的主要原因不是穿过背景圆的点的运动条纹。我们提出了一种基于背景圆运动产生的诱导运动的替代假设,由于孔径问题,背景圆运动的方向被误解了。
{"title":"The kinetic Orbison illusion.","authors":"Hikari Matsunaga,&nbsp;Hiroyuki Ito,&nbsp;Tama Kanematsu","doi":"10.1177/20416695231196979","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1177/20416695231196979","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>In the typical Orbison illusion, the sides of a square placed on concentric circles appear to be bending toward the center of the circles. We report a motion version of the Orbison illusion (namely, the kinetic Orbison illusion). When a dot moves along a square trajectory against a background of concentric circles, the sides of the trajectory appear to bend toward the center and the corners appear to be sharpened. In the present study, observers adjusted the shape of a comparison stimulus to the shape of the perceived trajectory by bending the sides. The amount of illusion was operationally defined as the largest discrepancy between the square and adjusted shape in the comparison stimulus. It was found that the illusory bending was more than twice the static Orbison illusion and reached a maximum of 7.3% of the length of one side. Experiments including a comparison between fixation and pursuit conditions revealed that the main cause of the kinetic illusion was not motion streaks of the dot crossing background circles. We propose an alternative hypothesis based on induced motion generated by background circle motion, the direction of which is misperceived owing to the aperture problem.</p>","PeriodicalId":47194,"journal":{"name":"I-Perception","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.9,"publicationDate":"2023-09-06","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC10515319/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"41173422","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
When is a disparity not a disparity? Toward an old theory of three-dimensional vision 什么时候差距不是差距?向着三维视觉的旧理论发展
4区 心理学 Q2 Medicine Pub Date : 2023-09-01 DOI: 10.1177/20416695231202726
Brian Rogers
The aims of this paper are twofold: first, to discuss and analyze the concept of binocular disparity and second, to contrast the traditional “air theory” of three-dimensional vision with the much older “ground theory,” first suggested by Ibn al-Haytham more than a thousand years ago. The origins of an “air theory” of perception can be traced back to Descartes and subsequently to the philosopher George Berkeley, who claimed that distance “could not be seen” because points lying along the same line of sight (in an empty space) would all project to the same location on the retina. However, Descartes was also aware that the angle of convergence of the two eyes could solve the problem of the “missing” information for the monocular observer and, since then, most visual scientists have assumed that eye vergence plays an important role both in judging absolute distance and for scaling retinal size and binocular disparities. In contrast, al-Haytham's and Gibson’s “ground theories,” which are based on the geometry of the textured ground plane surface that has surrounded us throughout evolution and during our lifetimes, are not just more ecologically based but they also obviate the need for disparity scaling.
本文的目的有两个:首先,讨论和分析双眼视差的概念;其次,将传统的三维视觉“空气理论”与一千多年前由伊本·海瑟姆(Ibn al-Haytham)首先提出的更古老的“地面理论”进行对比。感知的“空气理论”的起源可以追溯到笛卡尔,随后又追溯到哲学家乔治·伯克利,后者声称距离“无法被看到”,因为位于同一视线(在空白空间中)的点都会投射到视网膜上的同一位置。然而,笛卡儿也意识到两只眼睛的会聚角度可以解决单眼观察者的信息“缺失”问题,从那时起,大多数视觉科学家都认为眼睛的会聚在判断绝对距离、缩放视网膜大小和双眼视差方面都起着重要作用。相比之下,al-Haytham和Gibson的“地面理论”是基于在进化过程中和我们一生中围绕着我们的有纹理的地面平面表面的几何结构,不仅更基于生态学,而且还消除了对差异缩放的需要。
{"title":"When is a disparity not a disparity? Toward an old theory of three-dimensional vision","authors":"Brian Rogers","doi":"10.1177/20416695231202726","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1177/20416695231202726","url":null,"abstract":"The aims of this paper are twofold: first, to discuss and analyze the concept of binocular disparity and second, to contrast the traditional “air theory” of three-dimensional vision with the much older “ground theory,” first suggested by Ibn al-Haytham more than a thousand years ago. The origins of an “air theory” of perception can be traced back to Descartes and subsequently to the philosopher George Berkeley, who claimed that distance “could not be seen” because points lying along the same line of sight (in an empty space) would all project to the same location on the retina. However, Descartes was also aware that the angle of convergence of the two eyes could solve the problem of the “missing” information for the monocular observer and, since then, most visual scientists have assumed that eye vergence plays an important role both in judging absolute distance and for scaling retinal size and binocular disparities. In contrast, al-Haytham's and Gibson’s “ground theories,” which are based on the geometry of the textured ground plane surface that has surrounded us throughout evolution and during our lifetimes, are not just more ecologically based but they also obviate the need for disparity scaling.","PeriodicalId":47194,"journal":{"name":"I-Perception","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2023-09-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"135687758","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Eyes can tell: Assessment of implicit attitudes toward AI art 眼睛可以看出:对人工智能艺术的内在态度的评估
4区 心理学 Q2 Medicine Pub Date : 2023-09-01 DOI: 10.1177/20416695231209846
Yizhen Zhou, Hideaki Kawabata
Advances in artificial intelligence (AI) have significantly improved the abilities of machines. Human-unique abilities, such as art creation, are now being challenged by AI. Recent studies have investigated and compared people's attitudes toward human-made and AI-generated artworks. These results suggest that a negative bias may exist toward the latter. However, none of these previous studies has examined the extent of this bias. In this study, we investigate whether a bias against AI art can be found at an implicit level. Viewers’ attitudes toward AI art were assessed using eye-tracking measures and subjective aesthetic evaluations. Visual attention and aesthetic judgments were compared between artworks categorized as human-made and AI-made. The results showed that although it was difficult for individuals to identify AI-generated artwork, they exhibited an implicit prejudice against AI art. Participants looked longer at paintings that they thought were made by humans. No significant effect of categorization of paintings was found in subjective evaluations. These findings suggest that although human and AI art may be perceived as having similar aesthetic values, an implicit negative bias toward AI art exists. Although AI can now perform creative tasks, artistic creativity is still considered a human prerogative.
人工智能(AI)的进步大大提高了机器的能力。人类独有的能力,比如艺术创作,现在正受到人工智能的挑战。最近的研究调查并比较了人们对人造艺术品和人工智能艺术品的态度。这些结果表明,对后者可能存在负面偏见。然而,这些先前的研究都没有检查过这种偏见的程度。在这项研究中,我们调查了是否可以在隐含的层面上发现对人工智能艺术的偏见。观众对人工智能艺术的态度通过眼球追踪测量和主观审美评价来评估。对比了人造和人工智能艺术作品的视觉注意力和审美判断。结果显示,尽管个人很难识别人工智能生成的艺术品,但他们对人工智能艺术表现出隐性偏见。参与者对他们认为是人类画的画看得更久。绘画分类对主观评价无显著影响。这些发现表明,尽管人类和人工智能艺术可能被认为具有相似的审美价值,但对人工智能艺术存在隐性的负面偏见。虽然人工智能现在可以执行创造性任务,但艺术创造力仍然被认为是人类的特权。
{"title":"Eyes can tell: Assessment of implicit attitudes toward AI art","authors":"Yizhen Zhou, Hideaki Kawabata","doi":"10.1177/20416695231209846","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1177/20416695231209846","url":null,"abstract":"Advances in artificial intelligence (AI) have significantly improved the abilities of machines. Human-unique abilities, such as art creation, are now being challenged by AI. Recent studies have investigated and compared people's attitudes toward human-made and AI-generated artworks. These results suggest that a negative bias may exist toward the latter. However, none of these previous studies has examined the extent of this bias. In this study, we investigate whether a bias against AI art can be found at an implicit level. Viewers’ attitudes toward AI art were assessed using eye-tracking measures and subjective aesthetic evaluations. Visual attention and aesthetic judgments were compared between artworks categorized as human-made and AI-made. The results showed that although it was difficult for individuals to identify AI-generated artwork, they exhibited an implicit prejudice against AI art. Participants looked longer at paintings that they thought were made by humans. No significant effect of categorization of paintings was found in subjective evaluations. These findings suggest that although human and AI art may be perceived as having similar aesthetic values, an implicit negative bias toward AI art exists. Although AI can now perform creative tasks, artistic creativity is still considered a human prerogative.","PeriodicalId":47194,"journal":{"name":"I-Perception","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2023-09-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"135736910","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The zooming-speed illusion: A meta illusion? 变焦速度错觉:元错觉?
IF 1.9 4区 心理学 Q2 Medicine Pub Date : 2023-08-31 eCollection Date: 2023-07-01 DOI: 10.1177/20416695231187800
Hiroshi Ashida, Akiyoshi Kitaoka

A movie taken from the front window of a running train, with zooming in and out, has been popularly acknowledged as a perceptual illusion such that the train motion is perceived as much slower when zoomed in. This is, however, not a real illusion because the image speed varies as a function of the focal length of the lens. This could be a meta-illusion, that is, an illusory sense of illusion, that might reflect a lack of understanding of how zooming changes the geometrical structure of the image.

从行驶中的火车前窗拍摄的电影,放大和缩小,被普遍认为是一种感知错觉,因此当放大时,火车的运动会慢得多。然而,这并不是一种真正的错觉,因为图像速度是镜头焦距的函数。这可能是一种元错觉,也就是一种虚幻的错觉,可能反映出对缩放如何改变图像的几何结构缺乏了解。
{"title":"The zooming-speed illusion: A meta illusion?","authors":"Hiroshi Ashida,&nbsp;Akiyoshi Kitaoka","doi":"10.1177/20416695231187800","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1177/20416695231187800","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>A movie taken from the front window of a running train, with zooming in and out, has been popularly acknowledged as a perceptual illusion such that the train motion is perceived as much slower when zoomed in. This is, however, not a real illusion because the image speed varies as a function of the focal length of the lens. This could be a meta-illusion, that is, an illusory sense of illusion, that might reflect a lack of understanding of how zooming changes the geometrical structure of the image.</p>","PeriodicalId":47194,"journal":{"name":"I-Perception","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.9,"publicationDate":"2023-08-31","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://ftp.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pub/pmc/oa_pdf/0d/dd/10.1177_20416695231187800.PMC10515532.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"41167707","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Visual saltation illusion induced by flashes of subjective contours. 主观轮廓闪烁诱发的视觉盐化幻觉。
IF 2.4 4区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2023-08-11 eCollection Date: 2023-07-01 DOI: 10.1177/20416695231191241
Hiroyuki Ito, Keiya Kubo, Sheryl Anne Manaligod de Jesus

The visual saltation illusion of a Kanizsa-type subjective triangle was demonstrated. After a subjective/real triangle was flashed twice at the same position, another subjective/real triangle was flashed at a displaced position. In a typical case, the second flash was perceived to occur midway between the first and third flash positions. This study showed that the rated illusion strength for the Kanizsa and real triangles largely depended on stimulus onset asynchrony and retinal eccentricity and that the illusion rating was the same between the Kanizsa and real gray triangles when they were presented on black disks (or inducers). When the real triangle was presented in isolation, the illusion rating was lower. Presenting flashes on disks appears to enhance the saltation illusion for both the Kanizsa and real triangles possibly due to a stronger crowding effect or shape changes of inducers enhancing the perception of object appearance and disappearance. Various types of saltation illusions with a Kanizsa triangle are demonstrated in a video.

演示了卡尼兹萨式主观三角形的视觉盐化错觉。一个主观/真实三角形在同一位置闪烁两次后,另一个主观/真实三角形在一个移位位置闪烁。在一个典型的案例中,第二次闪光被认为发生在第一次和第三次闪光位置的中间。这项研究表明,卡尼萨三角形和真实三角形的幻觉强度评分主要取决于刺激开始的异步性和视网膜偏心率,当卡尼萨三角形和真实灰色三角形出现在黑色圆盘(或诱导物)上时,幻觉评分相同。当真实三角形单独出现时,幻觉等级较低。在圆盘上呈现闪光似乎增强了 Kanizsa 和真实三角形的盐化幻觉,这可能是由于更强的拥挤效应或诱导体的形状变化增强了物体出现和消失的感知。视频中演示了使用 Kanizsa 三角形产生的各种盐晕幻觉。
{"title":"Visual saltation illusion induced by flashes of subjective contours.","authors":"Hiroyuki Ito, Keiya Kubo, Sheryl Anne Manaligod de Jesus","doi":"10.1177/20416695231191241","DOIUrl":"10.1177/20416695231191241","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The visual saltation illusion of a Kanizsa-type subjective triangle was demonstrated. After a subjective/real triangle was flashed twice at the same position, another subjective/real triangle was flashed at a displaced position. In a typical case, the second flash was perceived to occur midway between the first and third flash positions. This study showed that the rated illusion strength for the Kanizsa and real triangles largely depended on stimulus onset asynchrony and retinal eccentricity and that the illusion rating was the same between the Kanizsa and real gray triangles when they were presented on black disks (or inducers). When the real triangle was presented in isolation, the illusion rating was lower. Presenting flashes on disks appears to enhance the saltation illusion for both the Kanizsa and real triangles possibly due to a stronger crowding effect or shape changes of inducers enhancing the perception of object appearance and disappearance. Various types of saltation illusions with a Kanizsa triangle are demonstrated in a video.</p>","PeriodicalId":47194,"journal":{"name":"I-Perception","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.4,"publicationDate":"2023-08-11","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC10422910/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10355810","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
At what level is the gap transfer illusion illusory? 在什么程度上差距转移幻觉是虚幻的?
IF 1.9 4区 心理学 Q2 Medicine Pub Date : 2023-07-01 DOI: 10.1177/20416695231194203
Yoshitaka Nakajima, Gerard Bastiaan Remijn

The gap transfer illusion is an auditory phenomenon in which a temporal gap in a longer glide transfers perceptually to a crossing shorter glide, making the longer glide illusorily continuous. This continuity is often considered a variation of classic illusory auditory continuity attributed to auditory peripheral activity, but a new view is given here supported by a series of sound demonstrations indicating that this illusory continuity is purely caused by a higher mechanism of perceptual organization.

间隙转移错觉是一种听觉现象,在这种听觉现象中,长时间滑翔的时间间隙在感知上转移到交叉的短时间滑翔,使长时间滑翔幻觉地连续。这种连续性通常被认为是由听觉外围活动引起的经典错觉听觉连续性的一种变体,但这里提出了一种新的观点,该观点得到了一系列声音证明的支持,表明这种错觉连续性纯粹是由感知组织的更高机制引起的。
{"title":"At what level is the gap transfer illusion illusory?","authors":"Yoshitaka Nakajima,&nbsp;Gerard Bastiaan Remijn","doi":"10.1177/20416695231194203","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1177/20416695231194203","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The gap transfer illusion is an auditory phenomenon in which a temporal gap in a longer glide transfers perceptually to a crossing shorter glide, making the longer glide illusorily continuous. This continuity is often considered a variation of classic illusory auditory continuity attributed to auditory peripheral activity, but a new view is given here supported by a series of sound demonstrations indicating that this illusory continuity is purely caused by a higher mechanism of perceptual organization.</p>","PeriodicalId":47194,"journal":{"name":"I-Perception","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.9,"publicationDate":"2023-07-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC10477769/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10305800","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Change of rapid temporal recalibration magnitude for audiovisual asynchrony with modulation of temporal binding window width: A preliminary investigation. 随时间绑定窗宽变化的视听非同步快速时间再校准幅度的初步研究。
IF 1.9 4区 心理学 Q2 Medicine Pub Date : 2023-07-01 DOI: 10.1177/20416695231193280
Yasuhiro Takeshima

The subjective synchrony perception for audiovisual stimuli is affected by previous temporal information. The point of subjective simultaneity is shifted toward the same asynchronous direction of audiovisual stimuli in a previous trial. This phenomenon is called "rapid temporal recalibration." The factors that modulate the magnitude of rapid temporal recalibration have not been fully investigated. Previously, a positive correlation has been found between the magnitude of rapid temporal recalibration and the width of the temporal binding window (TBW). This preliminary study examined the causal relationship between TBW size and rapid recalibration magnitude using a single experimental group comparison design. In this experiment, the magnitude of rapid recalibration was compared before and after perceptual training, which narrowed the TBW width. The results indicated that the magnitude of rapid recalibration was reduced by perceptual training. Therefore, it was speculated that TBW size determined the magnitude of rapid recalibration. This causal relationship helps elucidate the mechanisms of the adaptation for temporal lags between visual and auditory sensations.

主观对视听刺激的同步性知觉受先前时间信息的影响。主观同时性的点被转移到先前实验中视听刺激的相同的非同步方向。这种现象被称为“快速时间重新校准”。调节快速时间再校准幅度的因素尚未得到充分研究。在此之前,已经发现快速时间再校准的幅度与时间绑定窗口(TBW)的宽度之间存在正相关关系。本初步研究采用单实验组比较设计检验TBW大小与快速再校准幅度之间的因果关系。在本实验中,比较了知觉训练前后快速再校准的幅度,从而缩小了TBW宽度。结果表明,知觉训练降低了快速重新校准的幅度。因此,我们推测TBW的大小决定了快速再校准的幅度。这种因果关系有助于阐明视觉和听觉之间的时间滞后的适应机制。
{"title":"Change of rapid temporal recalibration magnitude for audiovisual asynchrony with modulation of temporal binding window width: A preliminary investigation.","authors":"Yasuhiro Takeshima","doi":"10.1177/20416695231193280","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1177/20416695231193280","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The subjective synchrony perception for audiovisual stimuli is affected by previous temporal information. The point of subjective simultaneity is shifted toward the same asynchronous direction of audiovisual stimuli in a previous trial. This phenomenon is called \"rapid temporal recalibration.\" The factors that modulate the magnitude of rapid temporal recalibration have not been fully investigated. Previously, a positive correlation has been found between the magnitude of rapid temporal recalibration and the width of the temporal binding window (TBW). This preliminary study examined the causal relationship between TBW size and rapid recalibration magnitude using a single experimental group comparison design. In this experiment, the magnitude of rapid recalibration was compared before and after perceptual training, which narrowed the TBW width. The results indicated that the magnitude of rapid recalibration was reduced by perceptual training. Therefore, it was speculated that TBW size determined the magnitude of rapid recalibration. This causal relationship helps elucidate the mechanisms of the adaptation for temporal lags between visual and auditory sensations.</p>","PeriodicalId":47194,"journal":{"name":"I-Perception","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.9,"publicationDate":"2023-07-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC10439762/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10651369","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Up is best. 向上是最好的。
IF 1.9 4区 心理学 Q2 Medicine Pub Date : 2023-07-01 DOI: 10.1177/20416695231190236
Stuart Anstis, Patrick Cavanagh
Ambiguous patterns have a tendency to appear to point up. This bias makes sense as most objects are on the ground, pointing up. However, we discover that the source of the up bias is the preference for seeing depth receding from the lower to the upper visual field.
模棱两可的图案有向上指向的趋势。这种偏差是有道理的,因为大多数物体都在地面上,指向上方。然而,我们发现,上偏的来源是看到深度的偏好从较低的视野后退到较高的视野。
{"title":"Up is best.","authors":"Stuart Anstis,&nbsp;Patrick Cavanagh","doi":"10.1177/20416695231190236","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1177/20416695231190236","url":null,"abstract":"Ambiguous patterns have a tendency to appear to point up. This bias makes sense as most objects are on the ground, pointing up. However, we discover that the source of the up bias is the preference for seeing depth receding from the lower to the upper visual field.","PeriodicalId":47194,"journal":{"name":"I-Perception","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.9,"publicationDate":"2023-07-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC10404385/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10302952","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The Influences of gradient color on the weight perception and stability perception: A preliminary study. 渐变色对体重知觉和稳定性知觉影响的初步研究。
IF 1.9 4区 心理学 Q2 Medicine Pub Date : 2023-07-01 DOI: 10.1177/20416695231197797
Xing Xu, Jieying Zhang, Qi Zhu, Tiansheng Xia

Gradient colors are widely used in product design. The variation of gradient colors muting a color as a series of steps from bright to dull creates a soft and gradual impression while also affecting people's perceptions. This study manipulates the types of gradient colors to explore the relationship between color gradients and perception of stability to determine whether weight perception plays a role. In the case of controlling for aesthetic differences, the study manipulated two types of color gradients (dark colors fading upward from the bottom versus downward from the top) and measured the perceptions of product stability. In the same hue, an upward gradient gives a stronger perception of stability. In addition, gradient colors significantly influence women's perception of stability more than men's. The study also investigated the mediating effect of weight perception: participants evaluated color fading-upward products with less weight relative to fading-downward colors. Furthermore, dark colors fading upward from the bottom lead to a stronger perception of weight, increasing the stability perception of the object. Finally, to aid future research, we discuss the practical implications of the current findings for areas such as sensory marketing, as well as possible directions for future research.

渐变色在产品设计中应用广泛。渐变颜色的变化使颜色从明亮到暗淡的一系列步骤变得柔和,给人一种柔和渐变的印象,同时也影响着人们的感知。本研究通过操纵渐变颜色的类型来探索颜色渐变与稳定性感知之间的关系,以确定体重感知是否起作用。在控制审美差异的情况下,该研究操纵了两种类型的颜色梯度(深色从底部向上褪色与从顶部向下褪色),并测量了对产品稳定性的感知。在相同的色调中,向上的渐变会给人一种更强的稳定感。此外,渐变色对女性稳定性感知的影响显著大于男性。该研究还调查了体重感知的中介作用:参与者评估了相对于褪色向下的颜色,重量较小的颜色褪色向上的产品。此外,深色从底部向上逐渐消失,导致更强的重量感知,增加了物体的稳定性感知。最后,为了帮助未来的研究,我们讨论了当前研究结果对感官营销等领域的实际意义,以及未来研究的可能方向。
{"title":"The Influences of gradient color on the weight perception and stability perception: A preliminary study.","authors":"Xing Xu,&nbsp;Jieying Zhang,&nbsp;Qi Zhu,&nbsp;Tiansheng Xia","doi":"10.1177/20416695231197797","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1177/20416695231197797","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Gradient colors are widely used in product design. The variation of gradient colors muting a color as a series of steps from bright to dull creates a soft and gradual impression while also affecting people's perceptions. This study manipulates the types of gradient colors to explore the relationship between color gradients and perception of stability to determine whether weight perception plays a role. In the case of controlling for aesthetic differences, the study manipulated two types of color gradients (dark colors fading upward from the bottom versus downward from the top) and measured the perceptions of product stability. In the same hue, an upward gradient gives a stronger perception of stability. In addition, gradient colors significantly influence women's perception of stability more than men's. The study also investigated the mediating effect of weight perception: participants evaluated color fading-upward products with less weight relative to fading-downward colors. Furthermore, dark colors fading upward from the bottom lead to a stronger perception of weight, increasing the stability perception of the object. Finally, to aid future research, we discuss the practical implications of the current findings for areas such as sensory marketing, as well as possible directions for future research.</p>","PeriodicalId":47194,"journal":{"name":"I-Perception","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.9,"publicationDate":"2023-07-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC10472828/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10669335","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Addendum to “Proprioceptive art: How should it be defined, and why has it become so popular?” “本体感觉艺术:它应该如何定义,为什么它变得如此受欢迎?”
IF 1.9 4区 心理学 Q2 Medicine Pub Date : 2023-07-01 DOI: 10.1177/20416695231191624
The author wishes to acknowledge that some of the ideas around the concept of ‘proprioceptive art’ developed in the paper are also shared by Prof. M. Schrenk (https://proprioceptive.art/#:∼:text=(6)% 20In%20proprioceptive%20art%2C,his%20or%20her%20exclusive%20audience) who was not explicitly mentioned by name in the article. While the acknowledgments to the paper thanked the organizers of the Prop Art workshop, which had proved very germane to the ideas developed in this chapter, Prof. Schrenk’s work was not mentioned by name, for which the author wishes to apologize. Those readers wishing to find out more about Schrenk’s thinking on the theme of proprioceptive art are directed to his 2014 book chapter (Schrenk, 2014).
作者希望承认,M.Schrenk教授也分享了论文中关于“本体感觉艺术”概念的一些观点(https://proprioceptive.art/#:~:text=(6)%20In%20本体感觉%20art%2C,his%20or%20her%20独占%20受众),文章中没有明确提到他的名字。虽然对论文的致谢感谢道具艺术研讨会的组织者,事实证明,该研讨会与本章中提出的想法非常密切,但Schrenk教授的作品没有被提及,作者对此表示歉意。那些希望了解更多关于施伦克关于本体感觉艺术主题的思考的读者可以参考他2014年出版的书中的章节(施伦克,2014)。
{"title":"Addendum to “Proprioceptive art: How should it be defined, and why has it become so popular?”","authors":"","doi":"10.1177/20416695231191624","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1177/20416695231191624","url":null,"abstract":"The author wishes to acknowledge that some of the ideas around the concept of ‘proprioceptive art’ developed in the paper are also shared by Prof. M. Schrenk (https://proprioceptive.art/#:∼:text=(6)% 20In%20proprioceptive%20art%2C,his%20or%20her%20exclusive%20audience) who was not explicitly mentioned by name in the article. While the acknowledgments to the paper thanked the organizers of the Prop Art workshop, which had proved very germane to the ideas developed in this chapter, Prof. Schrenk’s work was not mentioned by name, for which the author wishes to apologize. Those readers wishing to find out more about Schrenk’s thinking on the theme of proprioceptive art are directed to his 2014 book chapter (Schrenk, 2014).","PeriodicalId":47194,"journal":{"name":"I-Perception","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.9,"publicationDate":"2023-07-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"45612244","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
期刊
I-Perception
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1