首页 > 最新文献

I-Perception最新文献

英文 中文
Encoding of object location in a scrolling display. 滚动显示中对象位置的编码。
IF 2.4 4区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-09-29 eCollection Date: 2024-09-01 DOI: 10.1177/20416695241281172
Yumiko Fujii, Hiromi Morita

There are two main characteristics of visual information processing when viewing an image by scrolling on a small screen: viewing the image sequentially, section by section, owing to the limited visible area, and moving the image to view the desired section of the image. In this study, we investigated the effects of these characteristics on the encoding of object location. The participants were required to observe an image containing 10 objects under three viewing conditions without a time limit and to recall the location of the target object. The viewing conditions were a scrolling condition, a moving-window condition in which a fixed image was viewed by moving the window, and a no-window condition in which the entire image was viewed without a window. The results showed that although the recall accuracy did not differ among the conditions, the observation time increased in the order of scrolling, moving-window, and no-window conditions. These results indicate that in a scrolling view, the object location can be encoded with the same accuracy as that in a full view; however, more time is required for encoding. This finding suggests that viewing the image sequentially and moving the image degrade the encoding of object location.

在小屏幕上滚动观看图像时,视觉信息处理有两个主要特点:由于可视区域有限,因此要按顺序逐段观看图像;移动图像以观看所需的图像部分。在这项研究中,我们研究了这些特征对物体位置编码的影响。被试需要在三种无时间限制的观看条件下观察包含 10 个物体的图像,并回忆目标物体的位置。观看条件包括滚动条件、移动窗口条件(通过移动窗口观看固定图像)和无窗口条件(在无窗口条件下观看整个图像)。结果表明,虽然不同条件下的记忆准确率没有差异,但观察时间却依次按照滚动、移动窗口和无窗口条件的顺序增加。这些结果表明,在滚动视图中,物体位置的编码准确度与全视图中的编码准确度相同,但编码所需的时间更长。这一结果表明,连续观看图像和移动图像会降低对物体位置的编码。
{"title":"Encoding of object location in a scrolling display.","authors":"Yumiko Fujii, Hiromi Morita","doi":"10.1177/20416695241281172","DOIUrl":"10.1177/20416695241281172","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>There are two main characteristics of visual information processing when viewing an image by scrolling on a small screen: viewing the image sequentially, section by section, owing to the limited visible area, and moving the image to view the desired section of the image. In this study, we investigated the effects of these characteristics on the encoding of object location. The participants were required to observe an image containing 10 objects under three viewing conditions without a time limit and to recall the location of the target object. The viewing conditions were a scrolling condition, a moving-window condition in which a fixed image was viewed by moving the window, and a no-window condition in which the entire image was viewed without a window. The results showed that although the recall accuracy did not differ among the conditions, the observation time increased in the order of scrolling, moving-window, and no-window conditions. These results indicate that in a scrolling view, the object location can be encoded with the same accuracy as that in a full view; however, more time is required for encoding. This finding suggests that viewing the image sequentially and moving the image degrade the encoding of object location.</p>","PeriodicalId":47194,"journal":{"name":"I-Perception","volume":"15 5","pages":"20416695241281172"},"PeriodicalIF":2.4,"publicationDate":"2024-09-29","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11526158/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142559137","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Characterization of human lightness discrimination thresholds for independent spectral variations. 独立光谱变化的人类亮度辨别阈值的特征。
IF 2.4 4区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-09-26 eCollection Date: 2024-09-01 DOI: 10.1177/20416695241274662
Devin Reynolds, Vijay Singh

The lightness of an object is an intrinsic property that depends on its surface reflectance spectrum. The visual system estimates an object's lightness from the light reflected off its surface. However, the reflected light also depends on object extrinsic properties of the scene, such as the light source. For stable perception, the visual system needs to discount the variations due to the object extrinsic properties. We characterize this perceptual stability for variation in two spectral properties of the scene: the reflectance spectra of background objects and the intensity of light sources. We measure human observers' thresholds of discriminating computer-generated images of 3D scenes based on the lightness of a spherical target object in the scene. We measured change in discrimination thresholds as we varied the reflectance spectra of the objects and the intensity of the light sources in the scene, both individually and simultaneously. For small amounts of extrinsic variations, the discrimination thresholds remained nearly constant indicating that the thresholds were dominated by observers' intrinsic representation of lightness. As extrinsic variation increased, it started affecting observers' lightness judgment and the thresholds increased. We estimated that the effects of extrinsic variations were comparable to observers' intrinsic variation in the representation of object lightness. Moreover, for simultaneous variation of these spectral properties, the increase in threshold squared compared to the no-variation condition was a linear sum of the corresponding increase in threshold squared for the individual properties, indicating that the variations from these independent sources combine linearly.

物体的亮度是一种内在属性,取决于其表面反射光谱。视觉系统通过物体表面的反射光来估计物体的亮度。然而,反射光也取决于物体在场景中的外在属性,如光源。为了获得稳定的感知,视觉系统需要忽略物体外在属性所带来的变化。我们针对场景中两种光谱属性的变化来描述这种感知稳定性:背景物体的反射光谱和光源的强度。我们根据场景中球形目标物体的亮度,测量了人类观察者辨别计算机生成的三维场景图像的阈值。我们测量了当我们单独或同时改变场景中物体的反射光谱和光源强度时辨别阈值的变化。对于少量的外在变化,分辨阈值几乎保持不变,这表明阈值受观察者对亮度的内在表征所支配。随着外在变化的增加,它开始影响观察者的亮度判断,阈值也随之增加。我们估计,外在变化对物体亮度表征的影响与观察者内在变化的影响相当。此外,当这些光谱特性同时发生变化时,与无变化条件相比,阈值平方的增加是各个特性阈值平方相应增加的线性总和,这表明这些独立来源的变化是线性结合的。
{"title":"Characterization of human lightness discrimination thresholds for independent spectral variations.","authors":"Devin Reynolds, Vijay Singh","doi":"10.1177/20416695241274662","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1177/20416695241274662","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The lightness of an object is an intrinsic property that depends on its surface reflectance spectrum. The visual system estimates an object's lightness from the light reflected off its surface. However, the reflected light also depends on object extrinsic properties of the scene, such as the light source. For stable perception, the visual system needs to discount the variations due to the object extrinsic properties. We characterize this perceptual stability for variation in two spectral properties of the scene: the reflectance spectra of background objects and the intensity of light sources. We measure human observers' thresholds of discriminating computer-generated images of 3D scenes based on the lightness of a spherical target object in the scene. We measured change in discrimination thresholds as we varied the reflectance spectra of the objects and the intensity of the light sources in the scene, both individually and simultaneously. For small amounts of extrinsic variations, the discrimination thresholds remained nearly constant indicating that the thresholds were dominated by observers' intrinsic representation of lightness. As extrinsic variation increased, it started affecting observers' lightness judgment and the thresholds increased. We estimated that the effects of extrinsic variations were comparable to observers' intrinsic variation in the representation of object lightness. Moreover, for simultaneous variation of these spectral properties, the increase in threshold squared compared to the no-variation condition was a linear sum of the corresponding increase in threshold squared for the individual properties, indicating that the variations from these independent sources combine linearly.</p>","PeriodicalId":47194,"journal":{"name":"I-Perception","volume":"15 5","pages":"20416695241274662"},"PeriodicalIF":2.4,"publicationDate":"2024-09-26","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11475098/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142477514","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Can we estimate which colors our participants see? Comparing results from different gamma correction methods 我们能估计出参与者看到的颜色吗?比较不同伽玛校正方法的结果
IF 1.9 4区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-09-17 DOI: 10.1177/20416695241278562
Déborah Epicoco, Domicele Jonauskaite, Christine Mohr, C. Alejandro Parraga
In experimental color research, one must ensure that color is displayed and described reliably. When monitors are involved, colors are displayed through device-dependent color systems. However, these values must be translated into device-independent color systems to interpret what people perceive, often involving techniques such as gamma correction. We sought to explore the feasibility of estimating gamma instead of relying on direct gamma measurements, which typically require specialized equipment like a chromameter. Potential solutions include a computerized perception-based gamma estimation task or adopting the industry-standard gamma value of 2.2. We compared these two solutions against the chromameter measurements in the context of a color-matching task. Thirty-nine participants visually matched red, yellow, green, and blue physical objects using a computerized color picker. Starting from these color choices, we applied two RGB-to-CIE Lab color conversion methods: one using a perception-based gamma estimation and another using the industry-standard gamma. Color values obtained with the chromameter differed from the other two methods by 6–15 JNDs. Small differences existed between the results obtained using the perception-based task and the industry-standard gamma. Thus, we conclude that when standard viewing conditions cannot be assumed, adopting a gamma value of 2.2 should suffice.
在色彩实验研究中,必须确保可靠地显示和描述色彩。当涉及显示器时,色彩是通过与设备相关的色彩系统来显示的。然而,这些数值必须转换成与设备无关的色彩系统,以解释人们的感知,这通常涉及伽马校正等技术。我们试图探索估算伽马值的可行性,而不是依赖直接伽马值测量,后者通常需要色度计等专业设备。潜在的解决方案包括基于感知的计算机化伽马估算任务或采用行业标准伽马值 2.2。我们将这两种解决方案与色度计测量方法在色彩匹配任务中进行了比较。39 名参与者使用电脑选色器对红色、黄色、绿色和蓝色实物进行视觉配色。从这些颜色选择开始,我们采用了两种 RGB 到 CIE Lab 颜色转换方法:一种是基于感知的伽玛估计法,另一种是行业标准伽玛法。使用色度计获得的颜色值与其他两种方法相差 6-15 JND。使用基于感知的任务和行业标准伽马值得出的结果差异很小。因此,我们得出结论:当无法假设标准观看条件时,采用 2.2 的伽玛值就足够了。
{"title":"Can we estimate which colors our participants see? Comparing results from different gamma correction methods","authors":"Déborah Epicoco, Domicele Jonauskaite, Christine Mohr, C. Alejandro Parraga","doi":"10.1177/20416695241278562","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1177/20416695241278562","url":null,"abstract":"In experimental color research, one must ensure that color is displayed and described reliably. When monitors are involved, colors are displayed through device-dependent color systems. However, these values must be translated into device-independent color systems to interpret what people perceive, often involving techniques such as gamma correction. We sought to explore the feasibility of estimating gamma instead of relying on direct gamma measurements, which typically require specialized equipment like a chromameter. Potential solutions include a computerized perception-based gamma estimation task or adopting the industry-standard gamma value of 2.2. We compared these two solutions against the chromameter measurements in the context of a color-matching task. Thirty-nine participants visually matched red, yellow, green, and blue physical objects using a computerized color picker. Starting from these color choices, we applied two RGB-to-CIE Lab color conversion methods: one using a perception-based gamma estimation and another using the industry-standard gamma. Color values obtained with the chromameter differed from the other two methods by 6–15 JNDs. Small differences existed between the results obtained using the perception-based task and the industry-standard gamma. Thus, we conclude that when standard viewing conditions cannot be assumed, adopting a gamma value of 2.2 should suffice.","PeriodicalId":47194,"journal":{"name":"I-Perception","volume":"51 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":1.9,"publicationDate":"2024-09-17","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142266224","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Varieties of pictorial vision. 图像视觉的多样性
IF 2.4 4区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-09-02 eCollection Date: 2024-09-01 DOI: 10.1177/20416695241267371
Jan Koenderink, Andrea van Doorn, Johan Wagemans

Pictorial awareness is addressed through experimental phenomenology involving over 90 naïve participants. Since one can't look at the "same" picture twice the study uses one-shot trials. The participant's fascination for the duration of a session is held through the artistic principle of theme and variation. Six variations focus on the theme of pictorial geometry, both two-dimensional and three-dimensional. Major findings are: Idiosyncratic deviations from veridical are huge as compared to common textbook "effects." Observers wield arbitrary heuristics for tasks that are "formally related." The assumption of a common formal framework is apparently unsound. The notion of "inverse optics" is misleading. A fair fraction of the population appears to lack monocular stereopsis as intuitive awareness. It suggests an as-yet unrecognized, but perhaps common variety of aphantasia.

图片意识是通过 90 多名天真的参与者参与的实验现象学来解决的。由于 "同一幅 "图片不可能看两次,因此研究采用了一次性试验。通过主题和变体的艺术原则,让参与者在整个实验过程中保持对图片的痴迷。六种变体以图形几何为主题,包括二维和三维图形。主要发现有与教科书上常见的 "效果 "相比,随意偏离真实的情况非常严重。对于 "形式相关 "的任务,观察者会使用任意的启发式方法。对共同形式框架的假设显然是不可靠的。逆光学 "的概念具有误导性。相当一部分人似乎缺乏单眼立体视觉的直觉意识。这表明有一种尚未被认识的、但也许是常见的幻视。
{"title":"Varieties of pictorial vision.","authors":"Jan Koenderink, Andrea van Doorn, Johan Wagemans","doi":"10.1177/20416695241267371","DOIUrl":"10.1177/20416695241267371","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Pictorial awareness is addressed through experimental phenomenology involving over 90 naïve participants. Since one can't look at the \"same\" picture twice the study uses one-shot trials. The participant's fascination for the duration of a session is held through the artistic principle of theme and variation. Six variations focus on the theme of pictorial geometry, both two-dimensional and three-dimensional. Major findings are: Idiosyncratic deviations from veridical are huge as compared to common textbook \"effects.\" Observers wield arbitrary heuristics for tasks that are \"formally related.\" The assumption of a common formal framework is apparently unsound. The notion of \"inverse optics\" is misleading. A fair fraction of the population appears to lack monocular stereopsis as intuitive awareness. It suggests an as-yet unrecognized, but perhaps common variety of aphantasia.</p>","PeriodicalId":47194,"journal":{"name":"I-Perception","volume":"15 5","pages":"20416695241267371"},"PeriodicalIF":2.4,"publicationDate":"2024-09-02","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11372779/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142141399","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Bilateral rubber hand illusion induced by unilateral visuotactile stimulation. 单侧视觉触觉刺激诱发的双侧橡胶手幻觉
IF 2.4 4区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-09-02 eCollection Date: 2024-09-01 DOI: 10.1177/20416695241278277
Shinya Yamamoto

The rubber hand illusion involves the sense of body ownership of a fake hand. We showed that concurrent visuotactile stimuli to unilateral rubber and real hands can induce the embodiment of bilateral rubber hands when both rubber hands are positioned on the table. This phenomenon indicates that the brain has an integrated representation of the sense of body ownership for both hands.

橡皮手错觉涉及假手的身体拥有感。我们的研究表明,当两只橡皮手都放在桌子上时,同时对单侧橡皮手和真实手的视觉触觉刺激可以诱发双侧橡皮手的化身。这一现象表明,大脑对两只手的身体拥有感有一个综合表征。
{"title":"Bilateral rubber hand illusion induced by unilateral visuotactile stimulation.","authors":"Shinya Yamamoto","doi":"10.1177/20416695241278277","DOIUrl":"10.1177/20416695241278277","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The rubber hand illusion involves the sense of body ownership of a fake hand. We showed that concurrent visuotactile stimuli to <i>unilateral</i> rubber and real hands can induce the embodiment of <i>bilateral</i> rubber hands when both rubber hands are positioned on the table. This phenomenon indicates that the brain has an integrated representation of the sense of body ownership for both hands.</p>","PeriodicalId":47194,"journal":{"name":"I-Perception","volume":"15 5","pages":"20416695241278277"},"PeriodicalIF":2.4,"publicationDate":"2024-09-02","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11372775/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142141398","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The well-tempered color circle: A chromatic Gestalt. 调和的色环:色度格式塔
IF 2.4 4区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-08-22 eCollection Date: 2024-07-01 DOI: 10.1177/20416695241269314
Jan J Koenderink, Andrea J van Doorn, Doris I Braun

The "Color Circle" is an important chromatic Gestalt in the visual arts. There is not really a formal equivalent in conventional colorimetry. The fact that the hues can be linearly ordered and that such an order is necessarily periodic was intuited by artists in the early 19th century, but only formally explained by Ostwald and later Schrödinger a century later. As with musical keys, various metrical orders are in common use. Is there such a thing as a "well tempered" order? We consider this an issue for experimental phenomenology. We discuss an attempt based on observations by 30 (nonartist) observers.

色环 "是视觉艺术中一种重要的色度格式塔。在传统的色度学中,并没有与之相对应的形式。色调可以线性排序,而且这种排序必然是周期性的,这是 19 世纪初艺术家们的直觉,但直到一个世纪后,奥斯特瓦尔德和薛定谔才对此做出正式解释。与音调一样,各种音阶也被广泛使用。是否存在 "调和 "的音阶?我们认为这是一个实验现象学的问题。我们讨论了根据 30 位(非艺术家)观察者的观察所做的尝试。
{"title":"The well-tempered color circle: A chromatic Gestalt.","authors":"Jan J Koenderink, Andrea J van Doorn, Doris I Braun","doi":"10.1177/20416695241269314","DOIUrl":"10.1177/20416695241269314","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The \"Color Circle\" is an important chromatic Gestalt in the visual arts. There is not really a formal equivalent in conventional colorimetry. The fact that the hues can be linearly ordered and that such an order is necessarily periodic was intuited by artists in the early 19th century, but only formally explained by Ostwald and later Schrödinger a century later. As with musical keys, various metrical orders are in common use. Is there such a thing as a \"well tempered\" order? We consider this an issue for experimental phenomenology. We discuss an attempt based on observations by 30 (nonartist) observers.</p>","PeriodicalId":47194,"journal":{"name":"I-Perception","volume":"15 4","pages":"20416695241269314"},"PeriodicalIF":2.4,"publicationDate":"2024-08-22","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11342333/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142056833","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The effect of shape on visual size perception. 形状对视觉大小感知的影响
IF 2.4 4区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-08-21 eCollection Date: 2024-07-01 DOI: 10.1177/20416695241270301
Xiaolin Zhang, Shujie Li, Keli Yin

The Delboeuf illusion occurs when two circles (test figures) of equal radius are placed side by side and surrounded by concentric circles (inducers) of varying radii, resulting in the test figure being misestimated depending on the size of the surrounding inducer. This study conducted three experiments to explore the impact of shape and the contour attraction and parallel attraction on the Delboeuf illusion for different shapes. In Experiment 1 (n = 64), the test figures remained as circles while the inducers varied in shape. Experiment 2 (n = 64) involved simultaneous changes in the shape of both the test figures and the inducers. Experiment 3 (n = 64) replicated Experiment 2, with the exception that the areas of the inducers were equal and the distances between the inducers and the test figures were also equal. We conclude that the shape of the inducer and the test figure had an impact on the visual size perception, and in the magnitude of the Delboeuf illusion, varied depending on contour attraction. Configurations with circles or shapes resembling circles exhibit contour attraction, while configurations with shapes possessing longer parallel lines shift toward parallel attraction, both attractions enhance the perceived magnitude of the Delboeuf illusion.

当两个半径相等的圆(测试图形)并排放置,并被半径不同的同心圆(诱导物)包围时,就会产生德尔博夫错觉,导致测试图形因周围诱导物的大小不同而被误估。本研究进行了三项实验,以探讨不同形状、轮廓吸引力和平行吸引力对德尔博夫错觉的影响。在实验 1(n = 64)中,测试图形仍为圆形,而诱导体的形状则各不相同。实验 2(n = 64)中,测试图形和诱导物的形状同时发生变化。实验 3(n = 64)重复了实验 2,但诱导物的面积相等,诱导物与测试图形之间的距离也相等。我们的结论是,诱导物和测试图形的形状对视觉大小感知有影响,而且德尔博夫错觉的程度也因轮廓吸引力的不同而不同。圆形或类似圆形的形状会表现出轮廓吸引力,而拥有较长平行线的形状会转向平行吸引力,这两种吸引力都会增强德尔博夫幻觉的感知幅度。
{"title":"The effect of shape on visual size perception.","authors":"Xiaolin Zhang, Shujie Li, Keli Yin","doi":"10.1177/20416695241270301","DOIUrl":"10.1177/20416695241270301","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The Delboeuf illusion occurs when two circles (test figures) of equal radius are placed side by side and surrounded by concentric circles (inducers) of varying radii, resulting in the test figure being misestimated depending on the size of the surrounding inducer. This study conducted three experiments to explore the impact of shape and the contour attraction and parallel attraction on the Delboeuf illusion for different shapes. In Experiment 1 (<i>n </i>= 64), the test figures remained as circles while the inducers varied in shape. Experiment 2 (<i>n </i>= 64) involved simultaneous changes in the shape of both the test figures and the inducers. Experiment 3 (<i>n </i>= 64) replicated Experiment 2, with the exception that the areas of the inducers were equal and the distances between the inducers and the test figures were also equal. We conclude that the shape of the inducer and the test figure had an impact on the visual size perception, and in the magnitude of the Delboeuf illusion, varied depending on contour attraction. Configurations with circles or shapes resembling circles exhibit contour attraction, while configurations with shapes possessing longer parallel lines shift toward parallel attraction, both attractions enhance the perceived magnitude of the Delboeuf illusion.</p>","PeriodicalId":47194,"journal":{"name":"I-Perception","volume":"15 4","pages":"20416695241270301"},"PeriodicalIF":2.4,"publicationDate":"2024-08-21","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11342332/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142056832","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Looking with or without seeing in an individual with age-related macular degeneration impairing central vision. 患有老年性黄斑变性、中心视力受损的人,看东西时有或没有视力。
IF 2.4 4区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-08-14 eCollection Date: 2024-07-01 DOI: 10.1177/20416695241265821
Li Zhaoping

Looking leads gaze to objects; seeing recognizes them. Visual crowding makes seeing difficult or impossible before looking brings objects to the fovea. Looking before seeing can be guided by saliency mechanisms in the primary visual cortex (V1). We have proposed that looking and seeing are mainly supported by peripheral and central vision, respectively. This proposal is tested in an observer with central vision loss due to macular degeneration, using a visual search task that can be accomplished solely through looking, but is actually impeded through seeing. The search target is an uniquely oriented, salient, bar among identically shaped bars. Each bar, including the target, is part of an " " X " shape. The target's " X is identical to, although rotated from, the other " X 's in the image, which normally causes confusion. However, this observer exhibits no such confusion, presumably because she cannot see the " X 's shape, but can look towards the target. This result demonstrates a critical dichotomy between central and peripheral vision.

注视会将目光引向物体,而观看则能识别物体。在看将物体带到眼窝之前,视觉拥挤会使看变得困难或不可能。初级视觉皮层(V1)的显著性机制可以引导先看后看。我们提出,"看 "和 "看 "主要分别由周边视觉和中心视觉支持。我们在一名因黄斑变性而丧失中心视力的观察者身上对这一提议进行了测试,测试使用的是一项视觉搜索任务,该任务可以完全通过 "看 "来完成,但 "看 "实际上会阻碍 "看"。搜索目标是形状相同的条形图中一个方向独特、突出的条形图。包括目标在内的每个条形都是 "X "形状的一部分。目标的 "X "与图像中的其他 "X "虽然是旋转的,但却完全相同,这通常会造成混淆。然而,这位观察者却没有表现出这种混淆,这可能是因为她看不到 " X "的形状,但却可以看向目标。这一结果表明,中心视力和周边视力之间存在关键的二分法。
{"title":"Looking with or without seeing in an individual with age-related macular degeneration impairing central vision.","authors":"Li Zhaoping","doi":"10.1177/20416695241265821","DOIUrl":"10.1177/20416695241265821","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Looking leads gaze to objects; seeing recognizes them. Visual crowding makes seeing difficult or impossible before looking brings objects to the fovea. Looking before seeing can be guided by saliency mechanisms in the primary visual cortex (V1). We have proposed that looking and seeing are mainly supported by peripheral and central vision, respectively. This proposal is tested in an observer with central vision loss due to macular degeneration, using a visual search task that can be accomplished solely through looking, but is actually impeded through seeing. The search target is an uniquely oriented, salient, bar among identically shaped bars. Each bar, including the target, is part of an \" <math><mo>\"</mo> <mi>X</mi></math> \" shape. The target's <math><mo>\"</mo> <mi>X</mi></math> is identical to, although rotated from, the other <math><mo>\"</mo> <mi>X</mi></math> 's in the image, which normally causes confusion. However, this observer exhibits no such confusion, presumably because she cannot see the <math><mo>\"</mo> <mi>X</mi></math> 's shape, but can look towards the target. This result demonstrates a critical dichotomy between central and peripheral vision.</p>","PeriodicalId":47194,"journal":{"name":"I-Perception","volume":"15 4","pages":"20416695241265821"},"PeriodicalIF":2.4,"publicationDate":"2024-08-14","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11325320/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141989125","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Individual factors and vection in younger and older adults: How sex, field dependence, personality, and visual attention do (or do not) affect illusory self-motion. 年轻人和老年人的个体因素和视线:性别、场地依赖性、性格和视觉注意力如何影响(或不影响)幻觉自我运动。
IF 2.4 4区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-08-12 eCollection Date: 2024-07-01 DOI: 10.1177/20416695241270302
Brandy Murovec, Julia Spaniol, Behrang Keshavarz

An important aspect to an immersive experience in Virtual Reality is vection, defined as the illusion of self-motion. Much of the literature to date has explored strategies to maximize vection through manipulations of the visual stimulus (e.g., increasing speed) or the experimental context (e.g., framing of the study instructions). However, the role of individual differences (e.g., age, biological sex) in vection susceptibility has received little attention. The objective of the current study was to investigate the influence of individual-difference factors on vection perception in younger and older adults. Forty-six younger adults (M age = 25.1) and 39 older adults (M age = 72.4) completed assessments of personality traits, field dependence, and visual attention prior to observing a moving visual stimulus aimed at inducing circular vection. Vection was measured using self-reports of onset latency, duration, and intensity. Results indicated that, in both age groups, females experienced longer-lasting vection compared to males. Additionally, the level of field dependence was related to vection intensity and duration in males but not in females. Variability in vection intensity was best explained by a mixture of biological, perceptual, cognitive, and personality variables. Taken together, these findings suggest that individual factors are important for understanding differences in vection susceptibility.

虚拟现实沉浸式体验的一个重要方面是矢量,即自我运动的错觉。迄今为止,许多文献都在探讨通过操纵视觉刺激(如提高速度)或实验环境(如研究说明的框架)来最大限度地提高牵引力的策略。然而,个体差异(如年龄、生理性别)在静脉曲张易感性中的作用却很少受到关注。本研究的目的是调查个体差异因素对年轻和年长成年人静脉注射感知的影响。46 名年轻人(平均年龄为 25.1 岁)和 39 名老年人(平均年龄为 72.4 岁)在观察旨在诱发圆形脉动的移动视觉刺激物之前完成了人格特质、视野依赖性和视觉注意力的评估。通过对起始潜伏期、持续时间和强度的自我报告来测量脉动。结果表明,与男性相比,两个年龄组的女性都会出现持续时间更长的牵引。此外,在男性中,场依赖程度与脉动强度和持续时间有关,而在女性中则无关。生物、知觉、认知和个性变量的混合体最能解释脉动强度的变化。综上所述,这些研究结果表明,个体因素对于理解静脉曲张易感性的差异非常重要。
{"title":"Individual factors and vection in younger and older adults: How sex, field dependence, personality, and visual attention do (or do not) affect illusory self-motion.","authors":"Brandy Murovec, Julia Spaniol, Behrang Keshavarz","doi":"10.1177/20416695241270302","DOIUrl":"10.1177/20416695241270302","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>An important aspect to an immersive experience in Virtual Reality is vection, defined as the illusion of self-motion. Much of the literature to date has explored strategies to maximize vection through manipulations of the visual stimulus (e.g., increasing speed) or the experimental context (e.g., framing of the study instructions). However, the role of individual differences (e.g., age, biological sex) in vection susceptibility has received little attention. The objective of the current study was to investigate the influence of individual-difference factors on vection perception in younger and older adults. Forty-six younger adults (<i>M</i> <sub>age </sub>= 25.1) and 39 older adults (<i>M</i> <sub>age </sub>= 72.4) completed assessments of personality traits, field dependence, and visual attention prior to observing a moving visual stimulus aimed at inducing circular vection. Vection was measured using self-reports of onset latency, duration, and intensity. Results indicated that, in both age groups, females experienced longer-lasting vection compared to males. Additionally, the level of field dependence was related to vection intensity and duration in males but not in females. Variability in vection intensity was best explained by a mixture of biological, perceptual, cognitive, and personality variables. Taken together, these findings suggest that individual factors are important for understanding differences in vection susceptibility.</p>","PeriodicalId":47194,"journal":{"name":"I-Perception","volume":"15 4","pages":"20416695241270302"},"PeriodicalIF":2.4,"publicationDate":"2024-08-12","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11320702/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141976877","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The mechanism for the specificity of gaze direction: Inhibiting background location. 凝视方向特异性的机制:抑制背景位置
IF 2.4 4区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-08-12 eCollection Date: 2024-07-01 DOI: 10.1177/20416695241270303
Airui Chen, Weixia Han, Wei Wang, Bo Dong

The experiment combined the spatial Stroop paradigm to examine the effect of background location on the perception of arrow or gaze direction in the vertical dimension by manipulating the congruence between the target direction and background location, and to validate a possible cognitive mechanism for gaze direction specificity - inhibiting background location. The results showed that when subjects were required to identify the target direction in a Stroop task (Experiment 1), the gaze cue failed to induce the Stroop effect. However, when subjects were required to judge the congruence between the target direction and the background location (Experiment 2), the gaze cue and the arrow cue both induced the Stroop effect. This suggests that " inhibiting background location" is responsible for the elimination of the spatial Stroop effect by gaze direction, which may one of the mechanisms for gaze direction specificity.

实验结合空间斯特罗普范式,通过操纵目标方向与背景位置的一致性,考察背景位置对垂直维度上箭头或注视方向感知的影响,并验证注视方向特异性的可能认知机制--抑制背景位置。结果表明,当要求被试在 Stroop 任务中识别目标方向时(实验 1),凝视线索无法诱发 Stroop 效应。然而,当要求被试判断目标方向与背景位置是否一致时(实验 2),凝视线索和箭头线索都能诱发 Stroop 效应。这表明,"抑制背景位置 "是凝视方向消除空间斯特罗普效应的原因,这可能是凝视方向特异性的机制之一。
{"title":"The mechanism for the specificity of gaze direction: Inhibiting background location.","authors":"Airui Chen, Weixia Han, Wei Wang, Bo Dong","doi":"10.1177/20416695241270303","DOIUrl":"10.1177/20416695241270303","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The experiment combined the spatial Stroop paradigm to examine the effect of background location on the perception of arrow or gaze direction in the vertical dimension by manipulating the congruence between the target direction and background location, and to validate a possible cognitive mechanism for gaze direction specificity - inhibiting background location. The results showed that when subjects were required to identify the target direction in a Stroop task (Experiment 1), the gaze cue failed to induce the Stroop effect. However, when subjects were required to judge the congruence between the target direction and the background location (Experiment 2), the gaze cue and the arrow cue both induced the Stroop effect. This suggests that \" inhibiting background location\" is responsible for the elimination of the spatial Stroop effect by gaze direction, which may one of the mechanisms for gaze direction specificity.</p>","PeriodicalId":47194,"journal":{"name":"I-Perception","volume":"15 4","pages":"20416695241270303"},"PeriodicalIF":2.4,"publicationDate":"2024-08-12","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11320694/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141976878","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
I-Perception
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1