Pub Date : 2024-08-01Epub Date: 2024-08-10DOI: 10.1007/s10339-024-01214-z
Christoph von der Malsburg
A programmatically described solution to the segmentation problem is taken as opportunity to dicuss the neural architecture problem of vision. At the center of this problem is the formation of holistic entities (the Gestalt phenomenon) out of masses of neurons (the binding problem). As formulated in the Dynamic Net Architecture (DNA), neurons can become part of a (short-term) stable state only if supported inside a coherent network ('net'). Integration into nets is the basis for global treatment of structures, such as recognition as rigid body or projection to an invariant model.
{"title":"How are segmentation and binding computed and represented in the brain?","authors":"Christoph von der Malsburg","doi":"10.1007/s10339-024-01214-z","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s10339-024-01214-z","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>A programmatically described solution to the segmentation problem is taken as opportunity to dicuss the neural architecture problem of vision. At the center of this problem is the formation of holistic entities (the Gestalt phenomenon) out of masses of neurons (the binding problem). As formulated in the Dynamic Net Architecture (DNA), neurons can become part of a (short-term) stable state only if supported inside a coherent network ('net'). Integration into nets is the basis for global treatment of structures, such as recognition as rigid body or projection to an invariant model.</p>","PeriodicalId":47638,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Processing","volume":" ","pages":"67-72"},"PeriodicalIF":1.7,"publicationDate":"2024-08-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141914204","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
Pub Date : 2024-08-01Epub Date: 2024-08-10DOI: 10.1007/s10339-024-01211-2
Tina Iachini
How do we mentally represent the world out there? Psychology, philosophy and neuroscience have given two classical answers: as a living space where we act and perceive, dependent on our bodies; as an enduring physical space with its feature, independent of our bodily interactions. The first would be based on egocentric frames of reference anchored to the body, while the second on allocentric frames of reference centred on the environment itself or on objects. This raises some questions concerning how deep the reliance on the body and the environment is when using these reference frames, and whether they are affected differently by the duration of time and the scale (small or large) of space. To answer these questions, I have brought empirical evidence of the effect of motor interference, blindness, environmental characteristics and temporal factors on egocentric and allocentric spatial representational capacity. The results suggest that egocentric representations are deeply rooted in the body, with its sensory and motor properties, and are closely linked to acting now in small-scale or peripersonal space. Allocentric representations are more influenced by environmental than by bodily characteristics, by visual than by motor properties, and seem particularly related to large-scale or extrapersonal space. In line with neurophysiological evidence and a Kantian perspective, it appears that we are endowed with an internal spatial representation system ready to structure environmental information for our purposes. To what extent this system is innate and pervasive in cognition and what is its relationship to the neural 'positioning' substrate discovered by O'Keefe and colleagues requires further scientific investigation.
{"title":"Spatial memory and frames of reference: How deeply do we rely on the body and the environment?","authors":"Tina Iachini","doi":"10.1007/s10339-024-01211-2","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s10339-024-01211-2","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>How do we mentally represent the world out there? Psychology, philosophy and neuroscience have given two classical answers: as a living space where we act and perceive, dependent on our bodies; as an enduring physical space with its feature, independent of our bodily interactions. The first would be based on egocentric frames of reference anchored to the body, while the second on allocentric frames of reference centred on the environment itself or on objects. This raises some questions concerning how deep the reliance on the body and the environment is when using these reference frames, and whether they are affected differently by the duration of time and the scale (small or large) of space. To answer these questions, I have brought empirical evidence of the effect of motor interference, blindness, environmental characteristics and temporal factors on egocentric and allocentric spatial representational capacity. The results suggest that egocentric representations are deeply rooted in the body, with its sensory and motor properties, and are closely linked to acting now in small-scale or peripersonal space. Allocentric representations are more influenced by environmental than by bodily characteristics, by visual than by motor properties, and seem particularly related to large-scale or extrapersonal space. In line with neurophysiological evidence and a Kantian perspective, it appears that we are endowed with an internal spatial representation system ready to structure environmental information for our purposes. To what extent this system is innate and pervasive in cognition and what is its relationship to the neural 'positioning' substrate discovered by O'Keefe and colleagues requires further scientific investigation.</p>","PeriodicalId":47638,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Processing","volume":" ","pages":"79-83"},"PeriodicalIF":1.7,"publicationDate":"2024-08-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11364679/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141914258","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
Pub Date : 2024-08-01Epub Date: 2024-05-07DOI: 10.1007/s10339-024-01197-x
S Sivaranjini, C M Sujatha
Cognitive impairment in Parkinson's disease (PD) is associated with changes in the brain anatomical structures. The objective of this study, is to identify the atrophy patterns based on the severity of cognitive decline and evaluate the disease progression. In this study, gray matter alterations are analysed in 135 PD subjects under 3 cognitive domains (91 Cognitively normal PD (NC-PD), 25 PD with Mild Cognitive Impairment (PD-MCI) and 19 PD with Dementia (PD-D)) by comparing them with 58 Healthy Control (HC) subjects. Voxel Based Morphometry (VBM) is used to segment the gray matter regions in magnetic resonance images and analyse the atrophy patterns statistically. Significant patterns of gray matter variations observed in the middle temporal and medial frontal region differentiate between HC and PD subject groups based on the severity of cognitive decline. Abnormalities in gray matter is substantiated through radiomic features extracted from the significant gray matter clusters. Significant radiomic features of the clusters are able to differentiate between the HC and PD-D subjects with an accuracy of 81.82%. Higher atrophy levels identified in PD-D subjects compared to NC-PD and PD-MCI group enables early diagnosis and treatment procedures. The combined and comprehensive analysis of gray matter alterations through VBM and radiomic features gives better assessment of cognitive impairment in PD.
{"title":"Analysis of cognitive dysfunction in Parkinson's disease using voxel based morphometry and radiomics.","authors":"S Sivaranjini, C M Sujatha","doi":"10.1007/s10339-024-01197-x","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s10339-024-01197-x","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Cognitive impairment in Parkinson's disease (PD) is associated with changes in the brain anatomical structures. The objective of this study, is to identify the atrophy patterns based on the severity of cognitive decline and evaluate the disease progression. In this study, gray matter alterations are analysed in 135 PD subjects under 3 cognitive domains (91 Cognitively normal PD (NC-PD), 25 PD with Mild Cognitive Impairment (PD-MCI) and 19 PD with Dementia (PD-D)) by comparing them with 58 Healthy Control (HC) subjects. Voxel Based Morphometry (VBM) is used to segment the gray matter regions in magnetic resonance images and analyse the atrophy patterns statistically. Significant patterns of gray matter variations observed in the middle temporal and medial frontal region differentiate between HC and PD subject groups based on the severity of cognitive decline. Abnormalities in gray matter is substantiated through radiomic features extracted from the significant gray matter clusters. Significant radiomic features of the clusters are able to differentiate between the HC and PD-D subjects with an accuracy of 81.82%. Higher atrophy levels identified in PD-D subjects compared to NC-PD and PD-MCI group enables early diagnosis and treatment procedures. The combined and comprehensive analysis of gray matter alterations through VBM and radiomic features gives better assessment of cognitive impairment in PD.</p>","PeriodicalId":47638,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Processing","volume":" ","pages":"521-532"},"PeriodicalIF":1.7,"publicationDate":"2024-08-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140877583","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
Pub Date : 2024-08-01Epub Date: 2024-08-10DOI: 10.1007/s10339-024-01212-1
Barbara Landau
Theories of spatial term meanings often focus on geometric properties of objects and locations as the key to understanding meaning. For example, in English, "The cat is on the mat" might engage geometric properties characterizing the figure ('cat', a point) and the ground ('mat', a plane) as well as the geometric relationship between the two objects ('on', + vertical, 0 distance from ground object). However, substantial literature suggests that geometric properties are far from sufficient to capture the meanings of many spatial expressions, and that instead, force-dynamic properties of objects that afford containment or support relationships may be crucial to the meanings of those expressions. I will argue that both approaches are needed to understand the variety of spatial terms that appear in language and further, that spatial terms fall into two distinct sets, one represented by geometric properties of figure and ground and their spatial relationships, and the other by the force-dynamic properties of objects and their relationships. This division of labor within spatial terms has many consequences, with the two types differing in the nature of the acquisition problem and likely learning mechanisms, the extent and kind of cross-linguistic variation that has been observed, and the application of pragmatic principles to spatial terms. Speculatively, the two types may also be rooted in different cognitive systems and their neural substrates.
空间词义理论通常把物体和位置的几何属性作为理解词义的关键。例如,在英语中,"The cat is on the mat"("猫在垫子上")可能涉及到图形("cat",一个点)和地面("mat",一个平面)的几何属性,以及两个物体之间的几何关系("on",+ 垂直,与地面物体的距离为 0)。然而,大量文献表明,几何属性远不足以捕捉许多空间表达的含义,相反,物体的力-动态属性(提供容纳或支撑关系)可能对这些表达的含义至关重要。我将论证,要理解语言中出现的各种空间术语,这两种方法都是必要的;此外,空间术语可分为两组不同的术语,一组代表图形和地面的几何属性及其空间关系,另一组代表物体的力-动态属性及其关系。空间术语的这种分工产生了许多后果,两种类型在习得问题的性质和可能的学习机制、已观察到的跨语言变化的程度和种类以及对空间术语的语用原则的应用等方面各不相同。据推测,这两种类型也可能植根于不同的认知系统及其神经底层。
{"title":"Are spatial terms rooted in geometry or force-dynamics? Yes.","authors":"Barbara Landau","doi":"10.1007/s10339-024-01212-1","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s10339-024-01212-1","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Theories of spatial term meanings often focus on geometric properties of objects and locations as the key to understanding meaning. For example, in English, \"The cat is on the mat\" might engage geometric properties characterizing the figure ('cat', a point) and the ground ('mat', a plane) as well as the geometric relationship between the two objects ('on', + vertical, 0 distance from ground object). However, substantial literature suggests that geometric properties are far from sufficient to capture the meanings of many spatial expressions, and that instead, force-dynamic properties of objects that afford containment or support relationships may be crucial to the meanings of those expressions. I will argue that both approaches are needed to understand the variety of spatial terms that appear in language and further, that spatial terms fall into two distinct sets, one represented by geometric properties of figure and ground and their spatial relationships, and the other by the force-dynamic properties of objects and their relationships. This division of labor within spatial terms has many consequences, with the two types differing in the nature of the acquisition problem and likely learning mechanisms, the extent and kind of cross-linguistic variation that has been observed, and the application of pragmatic principles to spatial terms. Speculatively, the two types may also be rooted in different cognitive systems and their neural substrates.</p>","PeriodicalId":47638,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Processing","volume":" ","pages":"85-90"},"PeriodicalIF":1.7,"publicationDate":"2024-08-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141914203","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
Pub Date : 2024-08-01Epub Date: 2024-03-25DOI: 10.1007/s10339-024-01182-4
Francisco Molins, Nour Ben-Hassen Jemni, Dolores Garrote-Petisco, Miguel Ángel Serrano
In risky contexts, autism spectrum disorder (ASD) individuals exhibit more logical consistency and non-emotional decisions than do typical adults (TAs). This way of deciding could be also prevailing in social contexts, leading to maladaptive decisions. This evidence is scarce and inconsistent, and further research is needed. Recent developments in computational modeling allow analysis of decisional subcomponents that could provide valuable information to understand the decision-making and help address inconsistencies. Twenty-seven individuals with ASD and 25 TAs were submitted to a framing-task and the ultimatum game (UG). The Rescorla-Wagner computational model was used to analyze UG decisions. Results showed that in the UG, the ASD group exhibited a higher utilitarianism, characterized by lower aversion to unfairness and higher acceptance of offers. Moreover, this way of deciding was predicted by the higher economic rationality found in the framing task, where people with ASD did not manifest emotional biases such as framing effect. These results could suggest an atypical decision making, highly logical and non-emotional, as a robust feature of ASD.
在有风险的情况下,自闭症谱系障碍(ASD)患者比典型成人(TAs)表现出更多的逻辑一致性和非情绪化决策。这种决策方式在社会环境中也可能普遍存在,从而导致不适应性决策。这方面的证据很少且不一致,需要进一步研究。计算模型的最新发展允许对决策子组件进行分析,这可以为理解决策提供有价值的信息,并帮助解决不一致的问题。研究人员让 27 名 ASD 患者和 25 名 TA 进行了框架任务和最后通牒游戏(UG)。Rescorla-Wagner 计算模型用于分析 UG 决策。结果显示,在UG中,ASD组表现出较高的功利主义,其特点是对不公平的厌恶程度较低,对提议的接受程度较高。此外,在框架任务中发现的较高经济理性也预示了这种决策方式,在框架任务中,ASD 患者没有表现出框架效应等情感偏差。这些结果表明,高度逻辑性和非情感性的非典型决策是自闭症的一个显著特征。
{"title":"Highly logical and non-emotional decisions in both risky and social contexts: understanding decision making in autism spectrum disorder through computational modeling.","authors":"Francisco Molins, Nour Ben-Hassen Jemni, Dolores Garrote-Petisco, Miguel Ángel Serrano","doi":"10.1007/s10339-024-01182-4","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s10339-024-01182-4","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>In risky contexts, autism spectrum disorder (ASD) individuals exhibit more logical consistency and non-emotional decisions than do typical adults (TAs). This way of deciding could be also prevailing in social contexts, leading to maladaptive decisions. This evidence is scarce and inconsistent, and further research is needed. Recent developments in computational modeling allow analysis of decisional subcomponents that could provide valuable information to understand the decision-making and help address inconsistencies. Twenty-seven individuals with ASD and 25 TAs were submitted to a framing-task and the ultimatum game (UG). The Rescorla-Wagner computational model was used to analyze UG decisions. Results showed that in the UG, the ASD group exhibited a higher utilitarianism, characterized by lower aversion to unfairness and higher acceptance of offers. Moreover, this way of deciding was predicted by the higher economic rationality found in the framing task, where people with ASD did not manifest emotional biases such as framing effect. These results could suggest an atypical decision making, highly logical and non-emotional, as a robust feature of ASD.</p>","PeriodicalId":47638,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Processing","volume":" ","pages":"503-512"},"PeriodicalIF":1.7,"publicationDate":"2024-08-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11269346/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140289238","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
Pub Date : 2024-08-01Epub Date: 2024-03-25DOI: 10.1007/s10339-024-01184-2
Xiujie Yang, Jon R Star, Xiangyi Zhu, Rong Wang, Yan Zhang, Jiajin Tong, Zhonghui He
The present study investigated how phonological awareness and rapid automatized naming (hereafter, RAN), simultaneously contributed to Chinese reading and arithmetic fluency. Specifically, we proposed a new hypothesized mechanism that processing speed would mediate the relations of RAN with Chinese reading and arithmetic fluency. One hundred and forty-five Chinese children at the fifth grade were administered with a battery of measures, including three phonological processing measures, character reading, and whole number computation, as well as nonverbal IQ, and vocabulary knowledge. Path analyses revealed that phonological awareness and RAN were uniquely related to character reading and arithmetic fluency, while phonological memory was not significantly correlated to either character reading or arithmetic fluency, after controlling for age, nonverbal IQ, and vocabulary knowledge. Further analysis indicated that processing speed demonstrated a mediating effect on the importance of RAN in character reading, rather than in arithmetic fluency. Results underscore the potential importance of phonological awareness and RAN in character reading and arithmetic fluency, and the mediating role of processing speed in RAN to promote Chinese character reading fluency.
本研究探讨了语音意识和快速自动命名(以下简称 "RAN")如何同时促进中文阅读和算术流利性。具体而言,我们提出了一个新的假设机制,即处理速度将介导 RAN 与中文阅读和算术流利性的关系。我们对 145 名五年级中国儿童进行了一系列测量,包括三种语音处理测量、汉字阅读和整数计算,以及非语言智商和词汇知识。路径分析显示,在控制了年龄、非语言智商和词汇知识之后,语音意识和RAN与汉字阅读和算术流利性有独特的相关性,而语音记忆与汉字阅读或算术流利性没有显著的相关性。进一步的分析表明,处理速度对 RAN 在字符阅读中的重要性有中介作用,而不是在算术流利性中。研究结果强调了语音意识和RAN在汉字阅读和算术流利性中的潜在重要性,以及处理速度在RAN促进汉字阅读流利性中的中介作用。
{"title":"Phonological awareness and RAN contribute to Chinese reading and arithmetic for different reasons.","authors":"Xiujie Yang, Jon R Star, Xiangyi Zhu, Rong Wang, Yan Zhang, Jiajin Tong, Zhonghui He","doi":"10.1007/s10339-024-01184-2","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s10339-024-01184-2","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The present study investigated how phonological awareness and rapid automatized naming (hereafter, RAN), simultaneously contributed to Chinese reading and arithmetic fluency. Specifically, we proposed a new hypothesized mechanism that processing speed would mediate the relations of RAN with Chinese reading and arithmetic fluency. One hundred and forty-five Chinese children at the fifth grade were administered with a battery of measures, including three phonological processing measures, character reading, and whole number computation, as well as nonverbal IQ, and vocabulary knowledge. Path analyses revealed that phonological awareness and RAN were uniquely related to character reading and arithmetic fluency, while phonological memory was not significantly correlated to either character reading or arithmetic fluency, after controlling for age, nonverbal IQ, and vocabulary knowledge. Further analysis indicated that processing speed demonstrated a mediating effect on the importance of RAN in character reading, rather than in arithmetic fluency. Results underscore the potential importance of phonological awareness and RAN in character reading and arithmetic fluency, and the mediating role of processing speed in RAN to promote Chinese character reading fluency.</p>","PeriodicalId":47638,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Processing","volume":" ","pages":"443-455"},"PeriodicalIF":1.7,"publicationDate":"2024-08-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140289239","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
Pub Date : 2024-08-01Epub Date: 2024-08-10DOI: 10.1007/s10339-024-01213-0
Steven J Luck, John E Kiat
Visual working memory is a fundamental cognitive process that people use thousands of times each day as they engage in visually guided behavior. Thus, it is important to understand how the natural visual input-which consists of complex, spatially organized, continuously varying features-is represented in visual working memory. However, most research has used arrays of discrete, artificial objects defined by simple features, and existing formal models of visual working memory cannot be applied to natural scenes. In this paper, we identify 3 key aspects of natural scenes that are not captured by existing formal models of visual working memory, along with 2 distinct types of attention that must be considered. The goal is to clearly define the challenges and opportunities for moving models of visual working memory from arrays of artificial objects to natural scenes.
{"title":"Visual working memory for natural scenes: challenges and opportunities.","authors":"Steven J Luck, John E Kiat","doi":"10.1007/s10339-024-01213-0","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s10339-024-01213-0","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Visual working memory is a fundamental cognitive process that people use thousands of times each day as they engage in visually guided behavior. Thus, it is important to understand how the natural visual input-which consists of complex, spatially organized, continuously varying features-is represented in visual working memory. However, most research has used arrays of discrete, artificial objects defined by simple features, and existing formal models of visual working memory cannot be applied to natural scenes. In this paper, we identify 3 key aspects of natural scenes that are not captured by existing formal models of visual working memory, along with 2 distinct types of attention that must be considered. The goal is to clearly define the challenges and opportunities for moving models of visual working memory from arrays of artificial objects to natural scenes.</p>","PeriodicalId":47638,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Processing","volume":" ","pages":"73-78"},"PeriodicalIF":1.7,"publicationDate":"2024-08-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11365787/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141914261","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
Pub Date : 2024-08-01Epub Date: 2024-08-10DOI: 10.1007/s10339-024-01215-y
Mohan Matthen
{"title":"Object perception: four arguments from philosophy.","authors":"Mohan Matthen","doi":"10.1007/s10339-024-01215-y","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s10339-024-01215-y","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":47638,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Processing","volume":" ","pages":"105-111"},"PeriodicalIF":1.7,"publicationDate":"2024-08-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141914205","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
Pub Date : 2024-08-01Epub Date: 2024-05-07DOI: 10.1007/s10339-024-01193-1
Agnieszka Fusinska-Korpik, Michal Gacek
Decision-making capability is essential in fulfilling the need for autonomy of people with intellectual disability. In this study we aimed to examine decision-making capability regarding important social situations in people with intellectual disability at different stages of decision-making process. We studied 80 vocational school students with mild intellectual disability and 80 students of a similar age from mass vocation schools. We assessed decision-making with Important Life Decisions Task (ILDT). Students with intellectual disability obtained significantly lower scores than controls for each of the stories in ILDT as in each stage and overall final score in the decision-making process. The magnitude of difference in scores between groups varied in different stages of decision-making process. The most notable difficulties in decision-making regarding important social situations in people with intellectual disability are related to the evaluation of alternatives stage. Pattern of differences obtained in our study may be related to the content of decision-making problems.
{"title":"Decision-making regarding social situations in people with intellectual disability at different stages of the decision-making process.","authors":"Agnieszka Fusinska-Korpik, Michal Gacek","doi":"10.1007/s10339-024-01193-1","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s10339-024-01193-1","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Decision-making capability is essential in fulfilling the need for autonomy of people with intellectual disability. In this study we aimed to examine decision-making capability regarding important social situations in people with intellectual disability at different stages of decision-making process. We studied 80 vocational school students with mild intellectual disability and 80 students of a similar age from mass vocation schools. We assessed decision-making with Important Life Decisions Task (ILDT). Students with intellectual disability obtained significantly lower scores than controls for each of the stories in ILDT as in each stage and overall final score in the decision-making process. The magnitude of difference in scores between groups varied in different stages of decision-making process. The most notable difficulties in decision-making regarding important social situations in people with intellectual disability are related to the evaluation of alternatives stage. Pattern of differences obtained in our study may be related to the content of decision-making problems.</p>","PeriodicalId":47638,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Processing","volume":" ","pages":"491-501"},"PeriodicalIF":1.7,"publicationDate":"2024-08-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11269429/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140877584","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}