首页 > 最新文献

Cognitive Processing最新文献

英文 中文
Autistic and non-autistic adults use discourse context to determine a speaker's intention to request. 自闭症成人和非自闭症成人都会使用话语语境来判断说话者的请求意图。
IF 1.7 4区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-09-24 DOI: 10.1007/s10339-024-01229-6
Faith Frost, Marisa Nagano, Emily Zane

The current study focuses on how autistic adults utilize context to determine whether ambiguous utterances (e.g., "I'm thirsty") are intended as indirect requests or as literal comment/questions. Two questions are addressed: (1) How do autistic adults compare to neurotypical adults in using context to interpret an utterance's intention as either literal or a request? (2) What cognitive mechanisms correlate with indirect request interpretation, and are these different for participants in each group? Twenty-six autistic and 26 neurotypical college students participated, engaging in an online experiment where they read narratives that ended with utterances open to literal or request interpretations, based on context. After each narrative, participants selected the best paraphrase of the utterance from two options, literal versus request. Following this task, participants completed two mentalizing measures (a false belief and emotion-identification task) and several executive functioning tests. The best model for predicting paraphrase choice included scores on the emotion-identification task and context as main effects, along with the interaction between both. Participants with higher emotion-identification test scores were more likely to provide correct paraphrases. Models including group as a main effect and/or interaction were not better at fitting the data, nor were any models that included executive functioning measures as main effects or interactions. Emotion-identification test scores, but not autism diagnosis, predict how adults infer whether an utterance is a request. Findings suggest that autistic adults use context similarly to neurotypical adults when interpreting requests, and that similar processes underlie performance for each group.

本研究的重点是自闭症成人如何利用语境来判断模棱两可的话语(如 "我渴了")是间接请求还是字面评论/提问。研究涉及两个问题:(1) 自闭症成人与神经畸形成人相比,如何利用语境将语句的意图解释为字面意思或请求?(2)与间接请求解释相关的认知机制是什么?26 名患有自闭症的大学生和 26 名患有神经症的大学生参与了一项在线实验,在实验中,他们阅读了一些叙述,这些叙述的结尾是可以根据上下文进行字面解释或请求解释的语句。每篇叙述结束后,参与者从字面解释和请求解释两个选项中选出对语句的最佳解释。完成这项任务后,受试者还要完成两项心理测量(错误信念和情绪识别任务)和几项执行功能测试。预测转述选择的最佳模型包括作为主效应的情感识别任务得分和情境,以及两者之间的交互作用。情感识别测试得分较高的参与者更有可能提供正确的转述。将组别作为主效应和/或交互作用的模型并不能更好地拟合数据,将执行功能测量作为主效应或交互作用的模型也不能更好地拟合数据。情感识别测试得分(而非自闭症诊断)可以预测成人如何推断一个语句是否是请求。研究结果表明,自闭症成人在解释请求时使用的语境与神经典型成人相似,而且每个群体的表现都有相似的过程。
{"title":"Autistic and non-autistic adults use discourse context to determine a speaker's intention to request.","authors":"Faith Frost, Marisa Nagano, Emily Zane","doi":"10.1007/s10339-024-01229-6","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1007/s10339-024-01229-6","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The current study focuses on how autistic adults utilize context to determine whether ambiguous utterances (e.g., \"I'm thirsty\") are intended as indirect requests or as literal comment/questions. Two questions are addressed: (1) How do autistic adults compare to neurotypical adults in using context to interpret an utterance's intention as either literal or a request? (2) What cognitive mechanisms correlate with indirect request interpretation, and are these different for participants in each group? Twenty-six autistic and 26 neurotypical college students participated, engaging in an online experiment where they read narratives that ended with utterances open to literal or request interpretations, based on context. After each narrative, participants selected the best paraphrase of the utterance from two options, literal versus request. Following this task, participants completed two mentalizing measures (a false belief and emotion-identification task) and several executive functioning tests. The best model for predicting paraphrase choice included scores on the emotion-identification task and context as main effects, along with the interaction between both. Participants with higher emotion-identification test scores were more likely to provide correct paraphrases. Models including group as a main effect and/or interaction were not better at fitting the data, nor were any models that included executive functioning measures as main effects or interactions. Emotion-identification test scores, but not autism diagnosis, predict how adults infer whether an utterance is a request. Findings suggest that autistic adults use context similarly to neurotypical adults when interpreting requests, and that similar processes underlie performance for each group.</p>","PeriodicalId":47638,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Processing","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.7,"publicationDate":"2024-09-24","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142308777","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Testing the dual-memory framework: individual differences in the magnitude of the retrieval practice effect and fluid intelligence 检验双重记忆框架:检索练习效应和流体智力大小的个体差异
IF 1.7 4区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-09-17 DOI: 10.1007/s10339-024-01228-7
Marcos Felipe Rodrigues de Lima, Luciano Grüdtner Buratto

Retrieving information from memory enhances long-term retention. In this manuscript, we describe the dual-memory framework, which makes interval-scale predictions of the magnitude of this retrieval practice effect. After outlining the framework, we use data from our laboratory—both at the group level and at the distribution level—to fit the equations from the dual-memory framework. Overall, we successfully fitted the model predictions to the observed average data. In addition, we compared the predicted and the observed distributions of performance in the retrieval practice condition. More importantly, we introduce a useful approach to simulate empirical scenarios and test the relationship between individual-difference variables and the retrieval practice effect. We illustrate the application of this approach using data from a study that measured fluid intelligence. Future studies may benefit from contrasting different strength-based frameworks.

从记忆中检索信息能增强长期记忆。在本手稿中,我们描述了双重记忆框架,该框架对这种检索练习效应的大小做出了区间尺度的预测。在概述了该框架后,我们利用实验室的数据--包括小组层面和分布层面的数据--拟合了双重记忆框架的方程。总体而言,我们成功地将模型预测与观察到的平均数据进行了拟合。此外,我们还比较了在检索练习条件下预测的成绩分布和观察到的成绩分布。更重要的是,我们引入了一种有用的方法来模拟经验场景,并测试个体差异变量与检索练习效应之间的关系。我们用一项测量流体智力的研究数据来说明这种方法的应用。未来的研究可能会从对比不同的基于强度的框架中获益。
{"title":"Testing the dual-memory framework: individual differences in the magnitude of the retrieval practice effect and fluid intelligence","authors":"Marcos Felipe Rodrigues de Lima, Luciano Grüdtner Buratto","doi":"10.1007/s10339-024-01228-7","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1007/s10339-024-01228-7","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Retrieving information from memory enhances long-term retention. In this manuscript, we describe the dual-memory framework, which makes interval-scale predictions of the magnitude of this <i>retrieval practice effect</i>. After outlining the framework, we use data from our laboratory—both at the group level and at the distribution level—to fit the equations from the dual-memory framework. Overall, we successfully fitted the model predictions to the observed average data. In addition, we compared the predicted and the observed distributions of performance in the retrieval practice condition. More importantly, we introduce a useful approach to simulate empirical scenarios and test the relationship between individual-difference variables and the retrieval practice effect. We illustrate the application of this approach using data from a study that measured fluid intelligence. Future studies may benefit from contrasting different strength-based frameworks.</p>","PeriodicalId":47638,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Processing","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.7,"publicationDate":"2024-09-17","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142252649","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The effect of cognitive intervention program on intelligence scores in preschool 认知干预计划对学龄前儿童智力分数的影响
IF 1.7 4区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-09-11 DOI: 10.1007/s10339-024-01224-x
Hatice Kübra Koçak

Dynamic assessment is an approach that aims to improve student performance through interventions. One of the important application areas of dynamic assessment is the assessment of intelligence. Within the scope of the study, after a Cognitive intervention program (CIP) was developed, its effects on intelligence scores were examined with a quasi-experimental research method. The CIP was prepared by adopting a dynamic assessment approach to improve the performance of students with expert support. This improvement would be provided by the clues and feedback given during the intervention within the scope of the CIP. The sample of the study included 173 students in the 5–6 age group (83 experimental group, 90 control group). The CIP developed by the researcher consisted of 54 worksheets and was applied to the experimental group for 9 weeks. The implementation of the worksheets was supervised by classroom teachers. The digital application of the Anadolu-Sak Intelligence Scale (d-ASIS) and Raven’s colored progressive matrices (RCPM) were applied to both the experimental and control groups as pretests and posttests. The increases in the intelligence scores of the experimental and control groups were analyzed by MANOVA. The analysis showed that the intelligence levels of the experimental group increased significantly more than the control group in terms of both d-ASIS and RCPM total gain scores (posttest to pretest). This result indicated that the CIP, which was developed by adopting a dynamic assessment approach, supported cognitive development.

动态评估是一种旨在通过干预来提高学生成绩的方法。智力评估是动态评估的重要应用领域之一。在本研究范围内,在制定了认知干预计划(CIP)后,采用准实验研究方法考察了该计划对智力分数的影响。认知干预计划是通过采用动态评估方法来准备的,目的是在专家的支持下提高学生的成绩。在 CIP 范围内,通过干预过程中提供的线索和反馈来提高成绩。研究样本包括 173 名 5-6 岁年龄组的学生(实验组 83 人,对照组 90 人)。研究人员开发的 CIP 包括 54 张工作表,在实验组中使用了 9 周。工作表的实施由任课教师监督。阿纳多卢-萨克智力测验量表(d-ASIS)和瑞文彩色渐进矩阵(RCPM)的数字应用作为前测和后测分别应用于实验组和对照组。通过 MANOVA 分析了实验组和对照组智力分数的增长情况。分析结果表明,就 d-ASIS 和 RCPM 总增益分数(后测与前测)而言,实验组的智力水平明显高于对照组。这一结果表明,采用动态评估方法开发的 CIP 支持了认知发展。
{"title":"The effect of cognitive intervention program on intelligence scores in preschool","authors":"Hatice Kübra Koçak","doi":"10.1007/s10339-024-01224-x","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1007/s10339-024-01224-x","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Dynamic assessment is an approach that aims to improve student performance through interventions. One of the important application areas of dynamic assessment is the assessment of intelligence. Within the scope of the study, after a Cognitive intervention program (CIP) was developed, its effects on intelligence scores were examined with a quasi-experimental research method. The CIP was prepared by adopting a dynamic assessment approach to improve the performance of students with expert support. This improvement would be provided by the clues and feedback given during the intervention within the scope of the CIP. The sample of the study included 173 students in the 5–6 age group (83 experimental group, 90 control group). The CIP developed by the researcher consisted of 54 worksheets and was applied to the experimental group for 9 weeks. The implementation of the worksheets was supervised by classroom teachers. The digital application of the Anadolu-Sak Intelligence Scale (d-ASIS) and Raven’s colored progressive matrices (RCPM) were applied to both the experimental and control groups as pretests and posttests. The increases in the intelligence scores of the experimental and control groups were analyzed by MANOVA. The analysis showed that the intelligence levels of the experimental group increased significantly more than the control group in terms of both d-ASIS and RCPM total gain scores (posttest to pretest). This result indicated that the CIP, which was developed by adopting a dynamic assessment approach, supported cognitive development.</p>","PeriodicalId":47638,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Processing","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.7,"publicationDate":"2024-09-11","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142178728","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Choosing between bad and worse: investigating choice in moral dilemmas through the lens of control. 在坏与更坏之间做出选择:从控制的角度研究道德困境中的选择。
IF 1.7 4区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-08-31 DOI: 10.1007/s10339-024-01226-9
Revati Shivnekar, Narayanan Srinivasan

People's preferences for the utilitarian outcome in sacrificial moral dilemmas, where a larger group of individuals are saved at the cost of a few, have been argued to be influenced by various factors. Taking expected utility (EU) theory into consideration, we investigate whether the expected effectiveness of actions elucidate certain inconsistencies in moral judgments. Additionally, we also explore whether participants' role in the dilemma as the executor or a superior who merely makes a decision, which is carried out by a subordinate, influences judgments-a factor generally overlooked by classical EU models. We test these hypotheses using a modified moral dilemma paradigm with a choice between two actions, one highly successful and the other more likely to fail. Both actions are either expected to result in a favorable outcome of saving five individuals by sacrificing one or an unfavorable outcome of sacrificing five to save one. When the efficient action is anticipated to lead to a favorable outcome, in line with EU models, people almost invariably choose the efficient action. However, in conditions where the EUs associated with efficient and inefficient actions are close to each other, people's choice for favored outcome is above chance when they act as agents themselves. We discuss the implications of our results for existing theories of moral judgments.

在牺牲型道德困境中,人们对功利性结果的偏好(即以少数人的牺牲为代价来拯救更多的个人)被认为受到各种因素的影响。考虑到预期效用(EU)理论,我们研究了行动的预期效用是否能阐明道德判断中的某些不一致性。此外,我们还探讨了参与者在两难中的角色是执行者还是仅仅做出决定并由下属执行的上级是否会影响判断--这是经典的欧盟模型通常忽略的一个因素。我们使用了一个改良的道德两难范式来验证这些假设,在两个行动之间进行选择,一个是非常成功的行动,另一个是更有可能失败的行动。这两种行动要么会导致牺牲一人拯救五人的有利结果,要么会导致牺牲五人拯救一人的不利结果。当预期有效行动会带来有利结果时,根据欧盟模型,人们几乎总是选择有效行动。然而,在与高效和低效行动相关的 EU 值彼此接近的条件下,当人们自己充当代理人时,他们对有利结果的选择会高于偶然性。我们将讨论我们的结果对现有道德判断理论的影响。
{"title":"Choosing between bad and worse: investigating choice in moral dilemmas through the lens of control.","authors":"Revati Shivnekar, Narayanan Srinivasan","doi":"10.1007/s10339-024-01226-9","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1007/s10339-024-01226-9","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>People's preferences for the utilitarian outcome in sacrificial moral dilemmas, where a larger group of individuals are saved at the cost of a few, have been argued to be influenced by various factors. Taking expected utility (EU) theory into consideration, we investigate whether the expected effectiveness of actions elucidate certain inconsistencies in moral judgments. Additionally, we also explore whether participants' role in the dilemma as the executor or a superior who merely makes a decision, which is carried out by a subordinate, influences judgments-a factor generally overlooked by classical EU models. We test these hypotheses using a modified moral dilemma paradigm with a choice between two actions, one highly successful and the other more likely to fail. Both actions are either expected to result in a favorable outcome of saving five individuals by sacrificing one or an unfavorable outcome of sacrificing five to save one. When the efficient action is anticipated to lead to a favorable outcome, in line with EU models, people almost invariably choose the efficient action. However, in conditions where the EUs associated with efficient and inefficient actions are close to each other, people's choice for favored outcome is above chance when they act as agents themselves. We discuss the implications of our results for existing theories of moral judgments.</p>","PeriodicalId":47638,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Processing","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.7,"publicationDate":"2024-08-31","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142113438","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The impact of cognitive flexibility on prospective EFL teachers' critical thinking disposition: the mediating role of self-efficacy. 认知灵活性对准 EFL 教师批判性思维倾向的影响:自我效能感的中介作用。
IF 1.7 4区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-08-31 DOI: 10.1007/s10339-024-01227-8
Şenol Orakcı, Tahmineh Khalili

Critical thinking as one of the key skills for success in the 21st-century has been considered by many scholars in teacher education. This study tries to examine the interaction of critical thinking disposition with two other key characteristics of successful teachers: cognitive flexibility and self-efficacy. To this end, a sample of pre-service English as a Foreign Language (EFL) teachers was selected for this study. Based on the findings, a positive and strong relationship between cognitive flexibility and critical thinking disposition, and a positive and robust correlation between self-efficacy and critical thinking disposition were observed. Hence, it can be suggested that teacher-educationists can use this link for designing teacher-training courses with tailored tasks for both in and pre-service teachers. The main contribution of the findings might be beneficial for homogenizing teacher-training courses around the globe with the 21st-century trends. In addition, this line of research can be followed by empirical studies for checking the effectiveness of tailored tasks for provoking teachers' critical thinking dispositions, cognitive flexibility, and self-efficacy in teaching activities.

批判性思维作为 21 世纪成功教师的关键技能之一,已被许多教师教育学者所重视。本研究试图探讨批判性思维倾向与成功教师的另外两个关键特征:认知灵活性和自我效能感之间的相互作用。为此,本研究选取了职前英语作为外语(EFL)教师作为样本。研究结果表明,认知灵活性与批判性思维倾向之间存在正相关和强相关,自我效能感与批判性思维倾向之间存在正相关和强相关。因此,教师教育工作者可以利用这一联系,为在职教师和职前教师设计具有针对性任务的教师培训课程。研究结果的主要贡献可能有助于全球教师培训课程与 21 世纪的发展趋势保持一致。此外,还可以通过实证研究来检验定制任务在激发教师批判性思维、认知灵活性和教学活动自我效能感方面的有效性。
{"title":"The impact of cognitive flexibility on prospective EFL teachers' critical thinking disposition: the mediating role of self-efficacy.","authors":"Şenol Orakcı, Tahmineh Khalili","doi":"10.1007/s10339-024-01227-8","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1007/s10339-024-01227-8","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Critical thinking as one of the key skills for success in the 21st-century has been considered by many scholars in teacher education. This study tries to examine the interaction of critical thinking disposition with two other key characteristics of successful teachers: cognitive flexibility and self-efficacy. To this end, a sample of pre-service English as a Foreign Language (EFL) teachers was selected for this study. Based on the findings, a positive and strong relationship between cognitive flexibility and critical thinking disposition, and a positive and robust correlation between self-efficacy and critical thinking disposition were observed. Hence, it can be suggested that teacher-educationists can use this link for designing teacher-training courses with tailored tasks for both in and pre-service teachers. The main contribution of the findings might be beneficial for homogenizing teacher-training courses around the globe with the 21st-century trends. In addition, this line of research can be followed by empirical studies for checking the effectiveness of tailored tasks for provoking teachers' critical thinking dispositions, cognitive flexibility, and self-efficacy in teaching activities.</p>","PeriodicalId":47638,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Processing","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.7,"publicationDate":"2024-08-31","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142113441","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Immorality backward, morality forward? Metaphorical morality in Chinese-English bilinguals. 不道德向后,道德向前?汉英双语者的隐喻道德。
IF 1.7 4区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-08-30 DOI: 10.1007/s10339-024-01225-w
Huilan Yang, Neng Yang

This study explored whether instructing participants to make forward or backward joystick movements in response to morality words is consistent with the conceptual metaphor MORAL IS MOVING FORWARD/IMMORAL IS MOVING BACKWARD in Chinese-English bilinguals' first and second languages. Chinese-English bilinguals were instructed to categorize words as moral or immoral while moving a joystick in a compatible block (moral-forward, immoral-backward) and an incompatible block (moral-backward, immoral-forward). The results revealed three main conclusions: First, participants showed faster categorization of immoral words when their responses involved backward joystick movements compared to forward joystick movements. Second, participants also demonstrated a slightly faster categorization of moral words when their responses involved backward joystick movements compared to forward joystick movements. Third, Chinese morality words were categorized faster overall than English morality words. However, despite a slightly larger effect size observed in L1, the action compatibility effects for morality words exhibited a similar pattern across both languages. In sum, bilinguals showed shared access to the IMMORAL IS MOVING BACKWARD conceptual metaphor across both L1 and L2, but they did not access the MORAL IS MOVING FORWARD conceptual metaphor in either L1 or L2. This study provides new evidence supporting the conceptual metaphor theory.

本研究探讨了让被试在回答道德词语时向前或向后移动操纵杆是否与汉英双语者第一语言和第二语言中的概念隐喻 "道德向前移动/不道德向后移动 "相一致。在兼容区块(道德向前,不道德向后)和不兼容区块(道德向后,不道德向前)中移动操纵杆时,指导汉英双语者将词语归类为道德或不道德。结果显示了三个主要结论:首先,与向前移动操纵杆相比,当参与者的反应涉及向后移动操纵杆时,他们对不道德词语的分类速度更快。其次,与操纵杆向前移动相比,参与者在操纵杆向后移动时对道德词语的分类速度也略快。第三,总体而言,中文道德词语的分类速度快于英文道德词语。然而,尽管在第一语言中观察到的效应大小略大,但道德词语的动作相容性效应在两种语言中表现出相似的模式。总之,双语者在第一语言和第二语言中都能共同使用 "不道德就是向后移动 "这一概念隐喻,但他们在第一语言和第二语言中都不能使用 "道德就是向前移动 "这一概念隐喻。本研究提供了支持概念隐喻理论的新证据。
{"title":"Immorality backward, morality forward? Metaphorical morality in Chinese-English bilinguals.","authors":"Huilan Yang, Neng Yang","doi":"10.1007/s10339-024-01225-w","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1007/s10339-024-01225-w","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>This study explored whether instructing participants to make forward or backward joystick movements in response to morality words is consistent with the conceptual metaphor MORAL IS MOVING FORWARD/IMMORAL IS MOVING BACKWARD in Chinese-English bilinguals' first and second languages. Chinese-English bilinguals were instructed to categorize words as moral or immoral while moving a joystick in a compatible block (moral-forward, immoral-backward) and an incompatible block (moral-backward, immoral-forward). The results revealed three main conclusions: First, participants showed faster categorization of immoral words when their responses involved backward joystick movements compared to forward joystick movements. Second, participants also demonstrated a slightly faster categorization of moral words when their responses involved backward joystick movements compared to forward joystick movements. Third, Chinese morality words were categorized faster overall than English morality words. However, despite a slightly larger effect size observed in L1, the action compatibility effects for morality words exhibited a similar pattern across both languages. In sum, bilinguals showed shared access to the IMMORAL IS MOVING BACKWARD conceptual metaphor across both L1 and L2, but they did not access the MORAL IS MOVING FORWARD conceptual metaphor in either L1 or L2. This study provides new evidence supporting the conceptual metaphor theory.</p>","PeriodicalId":47638,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Processing","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.7,"publicationDate":"2024-08-30","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142113440","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Food for thought: the enhanced recall of metaphorical food sentences independent of hunger. 思考的食物:与饥饿无关的隐喻性食物句子的强化记忆。
IF 1.7 4区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-08-30 DOI: 10.1007/s10339-024-01222-z
Catherine Audrin, Géraldine Coppin

Metaphorical sentences are assumed to be related to more costly processes than their literal counterparts. However, given their frequent use in our daily lives, metaphorical sentences "must come with a benefit" (Noveck et al. Metaphor Symb 16:109-121. https://doi.org/10.1080/10926488.2001.9678889 , 2001). In this paper, we investigated whether metaphorical sentences were better remembered than their literal counterparts. In addition, we were interested in assessing whether the relevance of the metaphors impacted this recall. Anchoring this hypothesis in the appraisal theory, we hypothesized that food-related metaphorical sentences may be particularly relevant when one is hungry, and consequently, be better remembered in that particular physiological state. Participants were presented with randomized metaphorical sentences and their literal counterparts and were later asked to remember the missing word in both metaphorical and literal sentences. General mixed model analyses revealed that metaphorical sentences were better remembered. However, there was no significant effect of hunger. We discuss these results in relation to (1) the metaphor literature and (2) the appraisal theory of emotion.

与字面意义的句子相比,隐喻句子被认为与成本更高的过程有关。然而,鉴于隐喻句在我们日常生活中的频繁使用,它们 "一定会带来好处"(Noveck et al. Metaphor Symb 16:109-121. https://doi.org/10.1080/10926488.2001.9678889 , 2001)。在本文中,我们研究了隐喻句子是否比字面句子更好记。此外,我们还想评估隐喻的相关性是否会影响记忆效果。根据评价理论,我们假设与食物有关的隐喻句子在人饥饿时可能特别相关,因此在这种特殊的生理状态下记忆效果会更好。我们随机向受试者展示了隐喻句子和与之对应的字面句子,随后要求受试者记住隐喻句子和字面句子中缺少的单词。一般混合模型分析表明,隐喻句子的记忆效果更好。然而,饥饿感并没有明显的影响。我们将结合(1)隐喻文献和(2)情绪评价理论来讨论这些结果。
{"title":"Food for thought: the enhanced recall of metaphorical food sentences independent of hunger.","authors":"Catherine Audrin, Géraldine Coppin","doi":"10.1007/s10339-024-01222-z","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1007/s10339-024-01222-z","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Metaphorical sentences are assumed to be related to more costly processes than their literal counterparts. However, given their frequent use in our daily lives, metaphorical sentences \"must come with a benefit\" (Noveck et al. Metaphor Symb 16:109-121. https://doi.org/10.1080/10926488.2001.9678889 , 2001). In this paper, we investigated whether metaphorical sentences were better remembered than their literal counterparts. In addition, we were interested in assessing whether the relevance of the metaphors impacted this recall. Anchoring this hypothesis in the appraisal theory, we hypothesized that food-related metaphorical sentences may be particularly relevant when one is hungry, and consequently, be better remembered in that particular physiological state. Participants were presented with randomized metaphorical sentences and their literal counterparts and were later asked to remember the missing word in both metaphorical and literal sentences. General mixed model analyses revealed that metaphorical sentences were better remembered. However, there was no significant effect of hunger. We discuss these results in relation to (1) the metaphor literature and (2) the appraisal theory of emotion.</p>","PeriodicalId":47638,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Processing","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.7,"publicationDate":"2024-08-30","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142113439","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Emotional text messages affect the early processing of emoticons depending on their emotional congruence: evidence from the N170 and EPN event related potentials. 情感文本信息根据其情感一致性影响表情符号的早期处理:来自 N170 和 EPN 事件相关电位的证据。
IF 1.7 4区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-08-24 DOI: 10.1007/s10339-024-01223-y
Nerea Aldunate, Vladimir López, Felipe Rojas-Thomas, Mario Villena-González, Ismael Palacios, Claudio Artigas, Eugenio Rodríguez, Conrado A Bosman

Emoticons have been considered pragmatic cues that enhance emotional expressivity during computer-mediated communication. Yet, it is unclear how emoticons are processed in ambiguous text-based communication due to incongruences between the emoticon's emotional valence and its context. In this study, we investigated the electrophysiological correlates of contextual influence on the early emotional processing of emoticons, during an emotional congruence judgment task. Participants were instructed to judge the congruence between a text message expressing an emotional situation (positive or negative), and a subsequent emoticon expressing positive or negative emotions. We analyzed early event-related potentials elicited by emoticons related to face processing (N170) and emotional salience in visual perception processing (Early Posterior Negativity, EPN). Our results show that accuracy and Reaction Times depend on the interaction between the emotional valence of the context and the emoticon. Negative emoticons elicited a larger N170, suggesting that the emotional information of the emoticon is integrated at the early stages of the perceptual process. During emoticon processing, a valence effect was observed with enhanced EPN amplitudes in occipital areas for emoticons representing negative valences. Moreover, we observed a congruence effect in parieto-temporal sites within the same time-window, with larger amplitudes for the congruent condition. We conclude that, similar to face processing, emoticons are processed differently according to their emotional content and the context in which they are embedded. A congruent context might enhance the emotional salience of the emoticon (and therefore, its emotional expression) during the early stages of their processing.

表情符号一直被认为是计算机辅助交流中增强情感表达的实用线索。然而,由于表情符号的情感价位与其语境不一致,人们还不清楚表情符号在模棱两可的文本交流中是如何被处理的。在这项研究中,我们研究了在情绪一致性判断任务中,语境对表情符号早期情绪处理影响的电生理学相关性。参与者被要求判断一条表达情绪状况(积极或消极)的短信与随后一个表达积极或消极情绪的表情符号之间的一致性。我们分析了表情符号引起的早期事件相关电位,这些电位与人脸处理(N170)和视觉感知处理中的情绪显著性(早期后负性,EPN)有关。我们的研究结果表明,准确率和反应时间取决于情境和表情符号的情绪价值之间的相互作用。负面表情符号会引起较大的 N170,这表明表情符号的情绪信息在知觉过程的早期阶段就被整合了。在表情符号处理过程中,我们观察到了一种情绪效应,即代表负面情绪的表情符号在枕叶区的 EPN 振幅增强。此外,我们还在同一时间窗内的顶颞部位观察到了一致效应,一致条件下的振幅更大。我们的结论是,与脸部处理类似,表情符号也会根据其情感内容和所处的情境而得到不同的处理。在表情符号处理的早期阶段,一致的情境可能会增强表情符号的情绪显著性(因此也会增强其情绪表达)。
{"title":"Emotional text messages affect the early processing of emoticons depending on their emotional congruence: evidence from the N170 and EPN event related potentials.","authors":"Nerea Aldunate, Vladimir López, Felipe Rojas-Thomas, Mario Villena-González, Ismael Palacios, Claudio Artigas, Eugenio Rodríguez, Conrado A Bosman","doi":"10.1007/s10339-024-01223-y","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s10339-024-01223-y","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Emoticons have been considered pragmatic cues that enhance emotional expressivity during computer-mediated communication. Yet, it is unclear how emoticons are processed in ambiguous text-based communication due to incongruences between the emoticon's emotional valence and its context. In this study, we investigated the electrophysiological correlates of contextual influence on the early emotional processing of emoticons, during an emotional congruence judgment task. Participants were instructed to judge the congruence between a text message expressing an emotional situation (positive or negative), and a subsequent emoticon expressing positive or negative emotions. We analyzed early event-related potentials elicited by emoticons related to face processing (N170) and emotional salience in visual perception processing (Early Posterior Negativity, EPN). Our results show that accuracy and Reaction Times depend on the interaction between the emotional valence of the context and the emoticon. Negative emoticons elicited a larger N170, suggesting that the emotional information of the emoticon is integrated at the early stages of the perceptual process. During emoticon processing, a valence effect was observed with enhanced EPN amplitudes in occipital areas for emoticons representing negative valences. Moreover, we observed a congruence effect in parieto-temporal sites within the same time-window, with larger amplitudes for the congruent condition. We conclude that, similar to face processing, emoticons are processed differently according to their emotional content and the context in which they are embedded. A congruent context might enhance the emotional salience of the emoticon (and therefore, its emotional expression) during the early stages of their processing.</p>","PeriodicalId":47638,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Processing","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.7,"publicationDate":"2024-08-24","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142047307","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The differential illusion memory for high-associated abstract concepts (DIM-HA) effect. 高关联抽象概念的差异幻觉记忆(DIM-HA)效应。
IF 1.7 4区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-08-17 DOI: 10.1007/s10339-024-01220-1
Alejandro Marín-Gutiérrez, Emiliano Díez Villoria, Ana María González Martín

A vast body of evidence has shown that concrete concepts are processed faster and more accurately than abstract concepts in a variety of cognitive tasks. This phenomenon is widely known as the concreteness effect, and explanations for its occurrence seem to reflect differences in processing and organization for both types of representations. While there is considerable evidence to support this concreteness effect, the nature of these differences is still controversial. In developing an explanation, we have proposed a relatively different approach from a false memory perspective using the Deese-Roediger-McDermott paradigm. To explore the implications of the association in creating false memories, we explore behavioral and electrophysiologically the false memory effect, where targets were manipulated according to their association strength and their concreteness. Results showed that false recognition rates differed significantly between concrete and abstract critical words when they were associated strongly with their respective lists, which led to a higher proportion of abstract false alarms both in behavioral and electrophysiological experiments. The principal outcome, which was called the DIM-HA effect, was discussed in terms of theories of associative activation and qualitatively different representation.

大量证据表明,在各种认知任务中,具体概念比抽象概念处理得更快、更准确。这种现象被广泛称为 "具体性效应"(concreteness effect),对其发生的解释似乎反映了两种表征类型在处理和组织上的差异。虽然有大量证据支持这种具体性效应,但这些差异的性质仍存在争议。我们从虚假记忆的角度出发,利用 Deese-Roediger-McDermott 范式提出了一种相对不同的解释方法。为了探索联想在产生错误记忆中的影响,我们从行为学和电生理学角度探讨了错误记忆效应,即根据目标的联想强度和具体程度对其进行操作。结果表明,当具体和抽象的关键词语与各自的列表强关联时,错误识别率会有显著差异,这导致在行为学和电生理学实验中抽象错误警报的比例更高。主要结果被称为 DIM-HA 效应,并从联想激活理论和质变表征理论的角度进行了讨论。
{"title":"The differential illusion memory for high-associated abstract concepts (DIM-HA) effect.","authors":"Alejandro Marín-Gutiérrez, Emiliano Díez Villoria, Ana María González Martín","doi":"10.1007/s10339-024-01220-1","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1007/s10339-024-01220-1","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>A vast body of evidence has shown that concrete concepts are processed faster and more accurately than abstract concepts in a variety of cognitive tasks. This phenomenon is widely known as the concreteness effect, and explanations for its occurrence seem to reflect differences in processing and organization for both types of representations. While there is considerable evidence to support this concreteness effect, the nature of these differences is still controversial. In developing an explanation, we have proposed a relatively different approach from a false memory perspective using the Deese-Roediger-McDermott paradigm. To explore the implications of the association in creating false memories, we explore behavioral and electrophysiologically the false memory effect, where targets were manipulated according to their association strength and their concreteness. Results showed that false recognition rates differed significantly between concrete and abstract critical words when they were associated strongly with their respective lists, which led to a higher proportion of abstract false alarms both in behavioral and electrophysiological experiments. The principal outcome, which was called the DIM-HA effect, was discussed in terms of theories of associative activation and qualitatively different representation.</p>","PeriodicalId":47638,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Processing","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.7,"publicationDate":"2024-08-17","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141996680","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The effect of an embodied intervention on responsibility: put a load on one's shoulder. 身临其境的干预对责任感的影响:把重担放在肩上。
IF 1.7 4区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-08-05 DOI: 10.1007/s10339-024-01207-y
Sara Shahabifar, Aryan Yazdanpanah, Abdol-Hossein Vahabie

Responsibility is an essential part of our social life. Although responsibility is an abstract concept, it can be represented with concrete ideas through conceptual metaphor. Expressions like "carry a lot of responsibility," "shoulder the responsibility" shows that responsibility can be understood as a load on shoulder that one has to carry. Accordingly, this study tests the question that does putting a burden on one's shoulder makes him/her more responsible or not. In order to investigate this, on each trial, we asked participants to decide between risky situations that vary in magnitude, probability of win/lose, and the ambiguity level in two conditions: "self" and 'group." Each subject wears a vest with a load on each shoulder in half of the trials. As expected, Most of participants choose to defer on the group trials more than on the self-trials. This difference between numbers of deferring in group and self conditions is called responsibility aversion. Results indicate that responsibility aversion scores are lower (responsibility-taking was greater) in the state of wearing the vest than in the form of not wearing the vest significantly. We provided evidence that the abstract concept of responsibility is linked to bodily experiences of feeling load on the shoulder consistent with an embodied cognition theory.

责任是我们社会生活的重要组成部分。责任虽然是一个抽象的概念,但可以通过概念隐喻的方式用具体的观念来表现。诸如 "背负着很多责任"、"肩负着责任 "等表达方式表明,责任可以理解为肩上必须扛起的重担。因此,本研究对 "肩上的担子是否会使一个人更有责任感 "这一问题进行了检验。为了探究这个问题,在每次试验中,我们都要求参与者在两种条件下,在不同程度、输赢概率和模糊程度的风险情境中做出决定:"自我 "和 "团体":自我 "和 "团体"。在一半的试验中,每个受试者都会穿上一件肩负重物的背心。不出所料,大多数受试者在 "集体 "试验中比在 "自我 "试验中更多地选择了 "推迟"。这种在小组和自我条件下推迟次数的差异被称为责任厌恶。结果表明,穿背心时的责任厌恶得分明显低于不穿背心时(承担责任的程度更高)。我们提供的证据表明,抽象的责任概念与感觉肩上负担的身体体验有关,这与具身认知理论是一致的。
{"title":"The effect of an embodied intervention on responsibility: put a load on one's shoulder.","authors":"Sara Shahabifar, Aryan Yazdanpanah, Abdol-Hossein Vahabie","doi":"10.1007/s10339-024-01207-y","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1007/s10339-024-01207-y","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Responsibility is an essential part of our social life. Although responsibility is an abstract concept, it can be represented with concrete ideas through conceptual metaphor. Expressions like \"carry a lot of responsibility,\" \"shoulder the responsibility\" shows that responsibility can be understood as a load on shoulder that one has to carry. Accordingly, this study tests the question that does putting a burden on one's shoulder makes him/her more responsible or not. In order to investigate this, on each trial, we asked participants to decide between risky situations that vary in magnitude, probability of win/lose, and the ambiguity level in two conditions: \"self\" and 'group.\" Each subject wears a vest with a load on each shoulder in half of the trials. As expected, Most of participants choose to defer on the group trials more than on the self-trials. This difference between numbers of deferring in group and self conditions is called responsibility aversion. Results indicate that responsibility aversion scores are lower (responsibility-taking was greater) in the state of wearing the vest than in the form of not wearing the vest significantly. We provided evidence that the abstract concept of responsibility is linked to bodily experiences of feeling load on the shoulder consistent with an embodied cognition theory.</p>","PeriodicalId":47638,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Processing","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.7,"publicationDate":"2024-08-05","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141890466","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Cognitive Processing
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1