首页 > 最新文献

Cognitive Processing最新文献

英文 中文
Be kind, don't rewind: trait rumination may hinder the effects of self-compassion on health behavioral intentions after a body image threat. 善待自己,不要倒带:特质反刍可能会在身体形象受到威胁后阻碍自我同情对健康行为意向的影响。
IF 1.7 4区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-11-15 DOI: 10.1007/s10339-024-01249-2
Amsela Hodzic, Abigail R Flynn, Jean M Lamont, Min Khin, Alexandria Grubbs

Many individuals encounter situations that may elicit body-related concerns and impact how they think and feel about their weight, daily habits, and physical attractiveness. Research shows body image threats can predict poor health behaviors, but approaching those difficult moments with self-compassion-being kind, forgiving, and nonjudgmental-may reduce the impact of that threat and promote engagement in positive health behaviors. However, trait rumination, or tending to perseverate on negative events, may both dampen the benefits of a self-compassionate state and predict poor health behaviors. The present study examined whether a brief self-compassion writing exercise, after recalling a negative body-related event, predicted intent to perform health-promoting behaviors, and whether trait rumination attenuated this relationship. Participants (N = 217) completed a measure of trait rumination, underwent a body image threat, and were randomly assigned to cope with self-compassion or a distraction. Subsequently, participants completed a measure of health behavioral intentions. Analyses revealed participants in the self-compassion condition reported greater health-promoting behavioral intentions compared to control, with no significant main effect of trait rumination. However, a condition-by-rumination interaction emerged, suggesting the self-compassion condition was associated with higher health behavioral intentions, but only for participants with low trait rumination levels. These effects washed out when controlling for participants' self-rated health. The findings suggest that a self-compassion practice can help mitigate the adverse effects of a body image threat and facilitate health-promoting behavioral intentions, although its efficacy may depend on individual levels of trait rumination and perceived health.

许多人都会遇到一些情况,这些情况可能会引起与身体有关的担忧,并影响他们对自己体重、日常习惯和身体吸引力的看法和感受。研究表明,身体形象威胁可以预示不良的健康行为,但以自我同情的态度--善意、宽容和不做评判--来对待这些困难时刻,可能会减少威胁的影响,并促进参与积极的健康行为。然而,特质反刍或倾向于坚持负面事件,可能会削弱自我同情状态的益处,并预测不良的健康行为。本研究考察了在回忆与身体有关的负面事件后,进行简短的自我同情写作练习是否会预示着采取促进健康行为的意愿,以及特质反刍是否会削弱这种关系。参与者(217 人)完成了特质反刍测量,接受了身体形象威胁,并被随机分配到以自我同情或转移注意力的方式来应对。随后,参与者完成了一项健康行为意向测量。分析表明,与对照组相比,自我同情条件下的参与者报告了更多促进健康的行为意向,特质反刍没有显著的主效应。然而,出现了条件与反刍的交互作用,表明自我同情条件与较高的健康行为意向相关,但只针对特质反刍水平较低的参与者。在对参与者的自我健康评价进行控制后,这些影响就会消失。研究结果表明,自我同情练习有助于减轻身体形象威胁的不利影响,促进健康行为意向,但其效果可能取决于个人的特质反刍水平和感知健康状况。
{"title":"Be kind, don't rewind: trait rumination may hinder the effects of self-compassion on health behavioral intentions after a body image threat.","authors":"Amsela Hodzic, Abigail R Flynn, Jean M Lamont, Min Khin, Alexandria Grubbs","doi":"10.1007/s10339-024-01249-2","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1007/s10339-024-01249-2","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Many individuals encounter situations that may elicit body-related concerns and impact how they think and feel about their weight, daily habits, and physical attractiveness. Research shows body image threats can predict poor health behaviors, but approaching those difficult moments with self-compassion-being kind, forgiving, and nonjudgmental-may reduce the impact of that threat and promote engagement in positive health behaviors. However, trait rumination, or tending to perseverate on negative events, may both dampen the benefits of a self-compassionate state and predict poor health behaviors. The present study examined whether a brief self-compassion writing exercise, after recalling a negative body-related event, predicted intent to perform health-promoting behaviors, and whether trait rumination attenuated this relationship. Participants (N = 217) completed a measure of trait rumination, underwent a body image threat, and were randomly assigned to cope with self-compassion or a distraction. Subsequently, participants completed a measure of health behavioral intentions. Analyses revealed participants in the self-compassion condition reported greater health-promoting behavioral intentions compared to control, with no significant main effect of trait rumination. However, a condition-by-rumination interaction emerged, suggesting the self-compassion condition was associated with higher health behavioral intentions, but only for participants with low trait rumination levels. These effects washed out when controlling for participants' self-rated health. The findings suggest that a self-compassion practice can help mitigate the adverse effects of a body image threat and facilitate health-promoting behavioral intentions, although its efficacy may depend on individual levels of trait rumination and perceived health.</p>","PeriodicalId":47638,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Processing","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":1.7,"publicationDate":"2024-11-15","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142638895","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Analysis of the impact of different background colors in VR environments on risk preferences. 分析 VR 环境中不同背景颜色对风险偏好的影响。
IF 1.7 4区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-11-13 DOI: 10.1007/s10339-024-01248-3
Yoko Shikata, Tatsunori Matsui

Metaverse virtual reality (VR) technology offers an environment that provides a sense of presence that cannot be achieved through traditional online interactions. Although this technology is used in education and industry, challenges remain to be overcome for further penetration into society. In this study, we conducted an experiment using a risk preference task in blue and red spaces to verify color impressions and their effects in VR environments. Consistent with previous studies, high calmness was observed in the blue space. However, contrary to expectations, the risk preference task results showed risk-averse behavior in red space but not in blue space. Therefore, conventional color psychology may not be applicable to constructing the same space in a virtual environment. Previous studies have shown consistent experimental results regarding emotional responses to color stimuli; however, no consistent outcomes have been reported regarding cognitive performance. This is because the effect of color impressions is thought to depend not only on the color itself but also on what is recalled. The results of this study suggest that red in a VR environment may be interpreted as a warning color, preventing people from losing focus.

Metaverse 虚拟现实(VR)技术提供了一种环境,它所带来的临场感是传统在线互动无法实现的。虽然这种技术已被用于教育和工业领域,但要进一步渗透到社会中,仍需克服各种挑战。在本研究中,我们使用蓝色和红色空间中的风险偏好任务进行了一项实验,以验证 VR 环境中的色彩印象及其影响。与之前的研究一致,在蓝色空间中观察到了较高的平静度。然而,与预期相反的是,风险偏好任务的结果显示,在红色空间中存在规避风险的行为,而在蓝色空间中却没有。因此,传统的色彩心理学可能并不适用于在虚拟环境中构建相同的空间。以往的研究表明,关于对色彩刺激的情绪反应的实验结果是一致的;但是,关于认知表现的实验结果却不一致。这是因为人们认为颜色印象的效果不仅取决于颜色本身,还取决于所回忆的内容。本研究的结果表明,VR 环境中的红色可能会被解释为一种警告色,防止人们失去注意力。
{"title":"Analysis of the impact of different background colors in VR environments on risk preferences.","authors":"Yoko Shikata, Tatsunori Matsui","doi":"10.1007/s10339-024-01248-3","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1007/s10339-024-01248-3","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Metaverse virtual reality (VR) technology offers an environment that provides a sense of presence that cannot be achieved through traditional online interactions. Although this technology is used in education and industry, challenges remain to be overcome for further penetration into society. In this study, we conducted an experiment using a risk preference task in blue and red spaces to verify color impressions and their effects in VR environments. Consistent with previous studies, high calmness was observed in the blue space. However, contrary to expectations, the risk preference task results showed risk-averse behavior in red space but not in blue space. Therefore, conventional color psychology may not be applicable to constructing the same space in a virtual environment. Previous studies have shown consistent experimental results regarding emotional responses to color stimuli; however, no consistent outcomes have been reported regarding cognitive performance. This is because the effect of color impressions is thought to depend not only on the color itself but also on what is recalled. The results of this study suggest that red in a VR environment may be interpreted as a warning color, preventing people from losing focus.</p>","PeriodicalId":47638,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Processing","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":1.7,"publicationDate":"2024-11-13","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142630071","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Decision-making during training of a Swedish navy command and control team: a quantitative study of workload effects. 瑞典海军指挥与控制小组培训期间的决策制定:对工作量影响的定量研究。
IF 1.7 4区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-11-13 DOI: 10.1007/s10339-024-01242-9
Marie Hindorf, Denise Bäckström, Carl-Oscar Jonson, Anders Jonsson, Peter Berggren

The study compared two simulation environments for training of Swedish naval Command and Control teams by using indirect measures, including workload, combat readiness, and situation awareness. The literature explains simulation-based training as providing a safe avenue to practice relevant scenarios. Fidelity, the degree of realism in the simulation, and workload, the equilibrium between demands and assigned tasks, are crucial factors examined in this study of low- and high-fidelity naval simulations. This study was conducted to better understand the effects of various training methods. An experimental design with repeated measures was used with three consecutive escalating parts. The subjective, multidimensional assessment tool, NASA-Task Load Index was used to rate perceived workload. Combat readiness of the ship and mental demand yielded significant results. For combat readiness of the ship, there was a difference between the low and the high-fidelity setting, for the initial part of the scenario p = 0.037 and for the second part p = 0.028. Mental demand was experienced as higher in the low-fidelity setting, p = 0.036. Notably, the simulated internal battle training for onboard command teams in a low-fidelity setting was found to induce a level of stress comparable with that experienced in a high-fidelity setting. The results indicate that low-fidelity training results in a workload not distinguishable from high-fidelity training and has practical implications for increased use of low-fidelity training as part of (naval) command team training programmes.

这项研究通过使用包括工作量、战斗准备和态势感知在内的间接衡量标准,对瑞典海军指挥与控制小组培训的两种模拟环境进行了比较。文献解释说,模拟训练为练习相关场景提供了一个安全的途径。保真度(模拟的逼真程度)和工作量(需求与分配任务之间的平衡)是本研究中低保真和高保真海军模拟的关键因素。本研究旨在更好地了解各种训练方法的效果。本研究采用了重复测量的实验设计,分为三个连续递增的部分。采用主观、多维评估工具 NASA 任务负荷指数来评定感知工作量。舰艇战备状态和心理需求产生了显著的结果。在舰艇战斗准备方面,低保真和高保真设置之间存在差异,情景的第一部分 p = 0.037,第二部分 p = 0.028。心理需求在低保真环境中更高,p = 0.036。值得注意的是,在低保真环境下对舰载指挥团队进行的模拟内部战斗训练所产生的压力水平与高保真环境下的压力水平相当。结果表明,低保真训练所产生的工作量与高保真训练没有区别,这对更多地使用低保真训练作为(海军)指挥小组训练计划的一部分具有实际意义。
{"title":"Decision-making during training of a Swedish navy command and control team: a quantitative study of workload effects.","authors":"Marie Hindorf, Denise Bäckström, Carl-Oscar Jonson, Anders Jonsson, Peter Berggren","doi":"10.1007/s10339-024-01242-9","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1007/s10339-024-01242-9","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The study compared two simulation environments for training of Swedish naval Command and Control teams by using indirect measures, including workload, combat readiness, and situation awareness. The literature explains simulation-based training as providing a safe avenue to practice relevant scenarios. Fidelity, the degree of realism in the simulation, and workload, the equilibrium between demands and assigned tasks, are crucial factors examined in this study of low- and high-fidelity naval simulations. This study was conducted to better understand the effects of various training methods. An experimental design with repeated measures was used with three consecutive escalating parts. The subjective, multidimensional assessment tool, NASA-Task Load Index was used to rate perceived workload. Combat readiness of the ship and mental demand yielded significant results. For combat readiness of the ship, there was a difference between the low and the high-fidelity setting, for the initial part of the scenario p = 0.037 and for the second part p = 0.028. Mental demand was experienced as higher in the low-fidelity setting, p = 0.036. Notably, the simulated internal battle training for onboard command teams in a low-fidelity setting was found to induce a level of stress comparable with that experienced in a high-fidelity setting. The results indicate that low-fidelity training results in a workload not distinguishable from high-fidelity training and has practical implications for increased use of low-fidelity training as part of (naval) command team training programmes.</p>","PeriodicalId":47638,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Processing","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":1.7,"publicationDate":"2024-11-13","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142630304","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Navigating space: how fine and gross motor expertise influence spatial abilities at different scales. 空间导航:精细和粗大运动专长如何影响不同尺度的空间能力。
IF 1.7 4区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-11-12 DOI: 10.1007/s10339-024-01237-6
Narges Shakerian, Saeideh Monjezi, Mostafa Abdollahi Sarvi, Saeed Hesam, Mohammad Mehravar

Spatial ability, essential for navigating and interacting with the environment, comprises small-scale (e.g., mental rotation) and large-scale (e.g., spatial navigation) skills. Previous research underscores the influence of motor expertise on these abilities, yet comparative studies among different types of movement experts are limited, especially regarding the impact of gross motor skills on large-scale spatial abilities. This case-control study compared small-scale and large-scale spatial abilities among fine movement experts, gross movement experts, and non-movement experts. Ninety participants (30 per group) were assessed through computer-based spatial ability tests, including the Revised Purdue Spatial Visualization Test (PSVT: R), Mental Rotation Test, a navigation task developed in Unity 3D, and Triangle Completion Test (TCT). Fine movement experts excelled in small-scale spatial tasks compared to non-movement experts. Gross movement experts demonstrated superior large-scale spatial abilities, evidenced by lower errors in TCT and higher navigation scores, distinguishing their performance in spatial navigation and orientation from both fine movement experts and non-movement experts. The study highlights the distinct impacts of fine and gross motor expertise on spatial abilities, with gross motor skills particularly benefiting large-scale spatial navigation. These findings suggest potential clinical applications of gross motor training for improving spatial abilities in neurological populations, advocating for further research in immersive virtual environments and exploring lateral dominance effects on spatial performance.

空间能力对导航和与环境互动至关重要,它包括小范围(如心理旋转)和大范围(如空间导航)技能。以往的研究强调了运动专长对这些能力的影响,但不同类型运动专家之间的比较研究却很有限,尤其是粗大运动技能对大规模空间能力的影响。这项病例对照研究比较了精细运动专家、粗大运动专家和非运动专家的小范围和大范围空间能力。90名参与者(每组30人)接受了基于计算机的空间能力测试评估,包括修订版普渡空间视觉测试(PSVT:R)、心理旋转测试、Unity 3D开发的导航任务以及三角形完成测试(TCT)。与非运动专家相比,精细运动专家在小范围空间任务中表现出色。粗大动作专家在大尺度空间能力方面表现出色,这体现在他们在 TCT 中的错误率较低,导航得分较高,他们在空间导航和定向方面的表现有别于精细动作专家和非动作专家。研究强调了精细和粗大运动专长对空间能力的不同影响,粗大运动技能尤其有利于大尺度空间导航。这些研究结果表明,粗大运动训练在提高神经系统人群空间能力方面具有潜在的临床应用价值,并提倡在沉浸式虚拟环境中开展进一步研究,探索横向优势对空间表现的影响。
{"title":"Navigating space: how fine and gross motor expertise influence spatial abilities at different scales.","authors":"Narges Shakerian, Saeideh Monjezi, Mostafa Abdollahi Sarvi, Saeed Hesam, Mohammad Mehravar","doi":"10.1007/s10339-024-01237-6","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1007/s10339-024-01237-6","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Spatial ability, essential for navigating and interacting with the environment, comprises small-scale (e.g., mental rotation) and large-scale (e.g., spatial navigation) skills. Previous research underscores the influence of motor expertise on these abilities, yet comparative studies among different types of movement experts are limited, especially regarding the impact of gross motor skills on large-scale spatial abilities. This case-control study compared small-scale and large-scale spatial abilities among fine movement experts, gross movement experts, and non-movement experts. Ninety participants (30 per group) were assessed through computer-based spatial ability tests, including the Revised Purdue Spatial Visualization Test (PSVT: R), Mental Rotation Test, a navigation task developed in Unity 3D, and Triangle Completion Test (TCT). Fine movement experts excelled in small-scale spatial tasks compared to non-movement experts. Gross movement experts demonstrated superior large-scale spatial abilities, evidenced by lower errors in TCT and higher navigation scores, distinguishing their performance in spatial navigation and orientation from both fine movement experts and non-movement experts. The study highlights the distinct impacts of fine and gross motor expertise on spatial abilities, with gross motor skills particularly benefiting large-scale spatial navigation. These findings suggest potential clinical applications of gross motor training for improving spatial abilities in neurological populations, advocating for further research in immersive virtual environments and exploring lateral dominance effects on spatial performance.</p>","PeriodicalId":47638,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Processing","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":1.7,"publicationDate":"2024-11-12","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142630318","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Specificity ratings for English data. 英语数据的特异性评级。
IF 1.7 4区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-11-08 DOI: 10.1007/s10339-024-01239-4
Andrea Amelio Ravelli, Marianna Marcella Bolognesi, Tommaso Caselli

A dataset of specificity ratings for English words is hereby presented, analyzed and discussed in relation with other collections of speaker-generated ratings, including concreteness. Both, specificity and concreteness are analyzed in their ability to explain decision latencies in lexical and semantic tasks, showing important individual contributions. Specificity ratings are collected through best-worst scaling method on the words included in the ANEW dataset (Bradley and Lang in Affective norms for English words (ANEW): instruction manual and affective ratings (Tech. Rep.). Technical report C-1, the center for research in psychophysiology, 1999), chosen for its compatibility with many other collections of rating resources, and for its comparability with Italian specificity data (Bolognesi and Caselli in Behav Res Methods 55(7):3531-3548, 2023), allowing for cross-linguistic comparisons. Results suggest that specificity plays an important role in word processing and the importance of taking specificity into consideration when investigating concreteness effects.

本文介绍了一个英语单词特异性评级数据集,并结合其他由说话者生成的评级集合(包括具体性)对该数据集进行了分析和讨论。分析了具体性和具体性在解释词汇和语义任务中的决策延迟方面的能力,显示了各自的重要贡献。具体性评级是通过最佳-最差缩放法对 ANEW 数据集中的单词进行收集的(Bradley 和 Lang 在《英语单词情感规范(ANEW):指导手册和情感评级》(Tech.)技术报告 C-1,心理生理学研究中心,1999 年),之所以选择它是因为它与许多其他评分资源库兼容,而且它与意大利的特异性数据(Bolognesi 和 Caselli 在 Behav Res Methods 55(7):3531-3548, 2023)具有可比性,可以进行跨语言比较。研究结果表明,特异性在文字处理中起着重要作用,在研究具体性效应时必须考虑到特异性。
{"title":"Specificity ratings for English data.","authors":"Andrea Amelio Ravelli, Marianna Marcella Bolognesi, Tommaso Caselli","doi":"10.1007/s10339-024-01239-4","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1007/s10339-024-01239-4","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>A dataset of specificity ratings for English words is hereby presented, analyzed and discussed in relation with other collections of speaker-generated ratings, including concreteness. Both, specificity and concreteness are analyzed in their ability to explain decision latencies in lexical and semantic tasks, showing important individual contributions. Specificity ratings are collected through best-worst scaling method on the words included in the ANEW dataset (Bradley and Lang in Affective norms for English words (ANEW): instruction manual and affective ratings (Tech. Rep.). Technical report C-1, the center for research in psychophysiology, 1999), chosen for its compatibility with many other collections of rating resources, and for its comparability with Italian specificity data (Bolognesi and Caselli in Behav Res Methods 55(7):3531-3548, 2023), allowing for cross-linguistic comparisons. Results suggest that specificity plays an important role in word processing and the importance of taking specificity into consideration when investigating concreteness effects.</p>","PeriodicalId":47638,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Processing","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":1.7,"publicationDate":"2024-11-08","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142606731","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Recalling more each time: context change effects in hypermnesia. 每次回忆更多内容:超健忘症的语境变化效应。
IF 1.7 4区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-11-08 DOI: 10.1007/s10339-024-01240-x
Phillip N Goernert, Hannah T Corenblum, Barry S Corenblum

Hypermnesia, the increase in recall over trials without relearning or re-exposure to the studied items, has intrigued researchers since Ballard first reported the effect in 1913. In the typical hypermnesia study, additional retrieval trials are unexpected, and when announced, may induce context changes that re-focuses attention and effort on retrieving unrecalled items. The present studies examined the effects of context change on retrieval by telling some participants prior to study (trials-known condition) that three trials will be given to recall line drawings (Experiment 1) or words (Experiment 2) whereas others were not so informed (hypermnesia condition). Results of Experiment 1 revealed hypermnesia but no between-group differences on the sub-processes of item gains, losses, or intrusions. In Experiment 2, hypermnesia and between-group differences were found for item gains and intrusions, results that were marginally significant when data were aggregated across both experiments. Results are discussed in terms of the change in cue set hypothesis (Raaijmakers & Shiffrin, 1980) and the effects of internal and external context changes on hypermnesia. Suggestions for future studies are also presented.

自巴拉德于 1913 年首次报告超忆效应以来,研究人员一直对超忆效应(即在没有重新学习或重新接触所学项目的情况下,通过试验增加回忆)感到好奇。在典型的超忆研究中,额外的检索试验是意料之外的,一旦宣布,可能会引起情境变化,从而将注意力和精力重新集中到检索未被检索的项目上。本研究考察了情境变化对检索的影响,方法是在研究前告诉一些参与者(已知试验条件),将给他们三次试验来回忆线图(实验 1)或单词(实验 2),而其他参与者则不被告知(过度遗忘条件)。实验 1 的结果显示了过度遗忘,但在项目获得、丢失或侵入等子过程上没有组间差异。在实验 2 中,在项目收益和闯入方面发现了超健忘症和组间差异,将两个实验的数据汇总后,结果略有显著性。本文从线索集变化假说(Raaijmakers 和 Shiffrin,1980 年)以及内部和外部情境变化对过度记忆的影响两个方面对实验结果进行了讨论。此外,还对未来的研究提出了建议。
{"title":"Recalling more each time: context change effects in hypermnesia.","authors":"Phillip N Goernert, Hannah T Corenblum, Barry S Corenblum","doi":"10.1007/s10339-024-01240-x","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1007/s10339-024-01240-x","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Hypermnesia, the increase in recall over trials without relearning or re-exposure to the studied items, has intrigued researchers since Ballard first reported the effect in 1913. In the typical hypermnesia study, additional retrieval trials are unexpected, and when announced, may induce context changes that re-focuses attention and effort on retrieving unrecalled items. The present studies examined the effects of context change on retrieval by telling some participants prior to study (trials-known condition) that three trials will be given to recall line drawings (Experiment 1) or words (Experiment 2) whereas others were not so informed (hypermnesia condition). Results of Experiment 1 revealed hypermnesia but no between-group differences on the sub-processes of item gains, losses, or intrusions. In Experiment 2, hypermnesia and between-group differences were found for item gains and intrusions, results that were marginally significant when data were aggregated across both experiments. Results are discussed in terms of the change in cue set hypothesis (Raaijmakers & Shiffrin, 1980) and the effects of internal and external context changes on hypermnesia. Suggestions for future studies are also presented.</p>","PeriodicalId":47638,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Processing","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":1.7,"publicationDate":"2024-11-08","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142606593","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Impaired emotional multimodal integration in inhibition of return in children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. 注意缺陷/多动障碍儿童在抑制返回过程中的情绪多模式整合能力受损。
IF 1.7 4区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-11-05 DOI: 10.1007/s10339-024-01241-w
Jiaqi Wang, Yan Chen, Yue'e Zhang, Shizhong Cai, Aijun Wang, Ming Zhang

Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) is a common neurodevelopmental disorder in children. Previous studies have shown that children with ADHD have impaired processing of emotional stimuli, but it is unclear whether their ability to integrate multimodal emotional stimuli is impaired and at which processing pathway this impairment exists. The present study investigated the ability of children with ADHD to integrate emotional audiovisual stimuli under different emotional conditions, and the effect of audiovisual integration on IOR to reveal the impaired processing pathway of their emotional audiovisual integration. Fifty-eight school-age children (29 with ADHD and 29 matched typically developing (TD) children) performed an emotional valence discrimination task with a cue-target paradigm. The results showed that children with ADHD did not exhibit audiovisual integration of emotional stimuli in all experimental conditions. In addition, the IOR effect was significantly smaller for audiovisual targets than for visual targets under the negative but not the neutral emotion condition in children with ADHD, whereas this effect was present in all emotion conditions in TD children. These results indicate that the ability to integrate emotional audiovisual information is impaired in children with ADHD and this impairment exists in both bottom-up and top-down pathways. Additionally, although presenting emotional auditory stimuli at the same time as emotional faces reduced IOR both in children with ADHD and TD, the manner of reduction differed. These findings provide new evidence of emotional processing deficits and multimodal integration deficits in children with ADHD, and help provide support for children in educational settings.

注意力缺陷/多动症(ADHD)是一种常见的儿童神经发育障碍。以往的研究表明,多动症儿童对情绪刺激的处理能力受损,但他们整合多模态情绪刺激的能力是否受损,以及这种受损存在于哪种处理途径,目前尚不清楚。本研究调查了多动症儿童在不同情绪条件下整合情绪视听刺激的能力,以及视听整合对IOR的影响,以揭示其情绪视听整合受损的处理途径。58 名学龄儿童(29 名多动症儿童和 29 名与之匹配的典型发育(TD)儿童)在线索-目标范式下完成了情绪价位辨别任务。结果显示,在所有实验条件下,多动症儿童都没有表现出对情绪刺激的视听整合。此外,在负面情绪条件下,ADHD 儿童对视听目标的 IOR 效应明显小于对视觉目标的 IOR 效应,而在中性情绪条件下,TD 儿童对视听目标的 IOR 效应明显小于对视觉目标的 IOR 效应。这些结果表明,ADHD 儿童整合情绪视听信息的能力受到了损害,而且这种损害同时存在于自下而上和自上而下的通路中。此外,虽然与情绪面孔同时呈现情绪听觉刺激会降低多动症儿童和TD儿童的IOR,但降低的方式有所不同。这些发现为多动症儿童的情绪处理缺陷和多模态整合缺陷提供了新的证据,有助于为教育环境中的儿童提供支持。
{"title":"Impaired emotional multimodal integration in inhibition of return in children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder.","authors":"Jiaqi Wang, Yan Chen, Yue'e Zhang, Shizhong Cai, Aijun Wang, Ming Zhang","doi":"10.1007/s10339-024-01241-w","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1007/s10339-024-01241-w","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) is a common neurodevelopmental disorder in children. Previous studies have shown that children with ADHD have impaired processing of emotional stimuli, but it is unclear whether their ability to integrate multimodal emotional stimuli is impaired and at which processing pathway this impairment exists. The present study investigated the ability of children with ADHD to integrate emotional audiovisual stimuli under different emotional conditions, and the effect of audiovisual integration on IOR to reveal the impaired processing pathway of their emotional audiovisual integration. Fifty-eight school-age children (29 with ADHD and 29 matched typically developing (TD) children) performed an emotional valence discrimination task with a cue-target paradigm. The results showed that children with ADHD did not exhibit audiovisual integration of emotional stimuli in all experimental conditions. In addition, the IOR effect was significantly smaller for audiovisual targets than for visual targets under the negative but not the neutral emotion condition in children with ADHD, whereas this effect was present in all emotion conditions in TD children. These results indicate that the ability to integrate emotional audiovisual information is impaired in children with ADHD and this impairment exists in both bottom-up and top-down pathways. Additionally, although presenting emotional auditory stimuli at the same time as emotional faces reduced IOR both in children with ADHD and TD, the manner of reduction differed. These findings provide new evidence of emotional processing deficits and multimodal integration deficits in children with ADHD, and help provide support for children in educational settings.</p>","PeriodicalId":47638,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Processing","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":1.7,"publicationDate":"2024-11-05","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142584675","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The relationship between mindfulness and cognitive reappraisal: the mediating role of emotional and interoceptive awareness. 正念与认知再评价之间的关系:情绪和感知间意识的中介作用。
IF 1.7 4区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-11-05 DOI: 10.1007/s10339-024-01246-5
Yuan Pang, Barry Tse, Wen Liu, Qian Yang

Mindfulness and cognitive reappraisal have been recognized as two useful ways to regulate emotions. The former tends to cultivate an attitude of being open and accepting of emotional events; whereas the latter involves a top-down process of re-interpreting emotional events. However, it is unclear how mindfulness influences cognitive reappraisal. Hence, the current study mainly addressed this research issue by exploring the mediating role of emotional and interoceptive awareness. 372 participants were asked to report dispositional mindfulness, cognitive reappraisal, emotional awareness, and interoceptive awareness by means of corresponding questionnaires. First, we performed the Pearson Correlations among the four factors, then assessed the mediating role of emotional awareness and interoceptive awareness in the link between mindfulness and cognitive reappraisal in separated models. Last, we used structural equation modeling (SEM) to investigate the link when both emotional and interoceptive awareness acted as the mediating variables. Results showed that mindfulness was negatively correlated with cognitive reappraisal, emotional awareness, and interoceptive awareness; whereas emotional awareness, interoceptive awareness, and cognitive reappraisal were positively correlated with each other. Moreover, increased mindfulness had a significantly negative effect on cognitive reappraisal, by reducing emotional and interoceptive awareness separately or successively. Except for the No-worrying factor, the remaining seven factors of interoceptive awareness were significantly loaded onto the latent variable. The result reveals the negative relationship between mindfulness and cognitive reappraisal. This link is mediated by one's emotional awareness and interoceptive awareness independently or successively, which may reflect the intensity of externally-emotional reactivity that signify the need to regulate emotions by means of cognitive reappraisal.

正念和认知再评价被认为是调节情绪的两种有效方法。前者倾向于培养一种开放和接受情绪事件的态度;而后者则涉及一个自上而下重新解释情绪事件的过程。然而,正念如何影响认知再评价尚不清楚。因此,本研究主要通过探讨情绪意识和内感知意识的中介作用来解决这一研究问题。通过相应的问卷调查,372 名参与者被要求报告正念、认知再评价、情绪意识和感知间意识的倾向性。首先,我们对这四个因素进行了皮尔逊相关性分析,然后在分离模型中评估了情绪意识和感知间意识在正念和认知重评之间的中介作用。最后,我们使用结构方程模型(SEM)研究了情绪意识和感知间意识作为中介变量时的联系。结果表明,正念与认知再评价、情绪意识和感知间意识呈负相关;而情绪意识、感知间意识和认知再评价则呈正相关。此外,正念的增加对认知再评价有显著的负面影响,因为正念的增加会分别或先后降低情绪意识和感知间意识。除 "无忧 "因子外,其余七个感知间意识因子都被显著加载到潜变量上。结果显示,正念与认知再评价之间存在负相关。这种联系是由一个人的情绪意识和感知间意识单独或先后起中介作用的,这可能反映了外部情绪反应的强度,表明需要通过认知重评来调节情绪。
{"title":"The relationship between mindfulness and cognitive reappraisal: the mediating role of emotional and interoceptive awareness.","authors":"Yuan Pang, Barry Tse, Wen Liu, Qian Yang","doi":"10.1007/s10339-024-01246-5","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1007/s10339-024-01246-5","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Mindfulness and cognitive reappraisal have been recognized as two useful ways to regulate emotions. The former tends to cultivate an attitude of being open and accepting of emotional events; whereas the latter involves a top-down process of re-interpreting emotional events. However, it is unclear how mindfulness influences cognitive reappraisal. Hence, the current study mainly addressed this research issue by exploring the mediating role of emotional and interoceptive awareness. 372 participants were asked to report dispositional mindfulness, cognitive reappraisal, emotional awareness, and interoceptive awareness by means of corresponding questionnaires. First, we performed the Pearson Correlations among the four factors, then assessed the mediating role of emotional awareness and interoceptive awareness in the link between mindfulness and cognitive reappraisal in separated models. Last, we used structural equation modeling (SEM) to investigate the link when both emotional and interoceptive awareness acted as the mediating variables. Results showed that mindfulness was negatively correlated with cognitive reappraisal, emotional awareness, and interoceptive awareness; whereas emotional awareness, interoceptive awareness, and cognitive reappraisal were positively correlated with each other. Moreover, increased mindfulness had a significantly negative effect on cognitive reappraisal, by reducing emotional and interoceptive awareness separately or successively. Except for the No-worrying factor, the remaining seven factors of interoceptive awareness were significantly loaded onto the latent variable. The result reveals the negative relationship between mindfulness and cognitive reappraisal. This link is mediated by one's emotional awareness and interoceptive awareness independently or successively, which may reflect the intensity of externally-emotional reactivity that signify the need to regulate emotions by means of cognitive reappraisal.</p>","PeriodicalId":47638,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Processing","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":1.7,"publicationDate":"2024-11-05","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142577104","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The impact of fear of pain on the temporal processing of emotional faces: modulation of attentional resources. 对疼痛的恐惧对情绪面孔时间处理的影响:注意力资源的调节。
IF 1.7 4区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-11-05 DOI: 10.1007/s10339-024-01243-8
Panpan Zheng, Zhenyong Lyu

Recent research has focused on the spatial attention bias toward pain-related cues in individuals with fear of pain (FOP), but our understanding of its temporal attention features, particularly regarding emotional faces, is limited. To address this gap, 39 individuals with high fear of pain (H-FOP) and 37 with low fear of pain (L-FOP) completed a rapid serial visual presentation dual task. Participants viewed a series of rapidly presented pictures, first identified neutral building images as the first target (T1) and then emotional faces (painful, neutral, and happy) as the second target (T2). T1 and T2 presented at time intervals of 232 ms (lag 2) and 696 ms (lag 6), respectively. Results indicate that H-FOP group members were less accurate in recognizing faces at lag 2 shortly after correctly identifying T1 compared to those L-FOP group members. Notably, all participants were more accurate in recognizing painful faces at lag 2 than in identifying neutral and happy faces, suggesting a negative bias toward pain stimuli when attentional resources were limited. In contrast, at lag 6, when attentional resources were more abundant, participants showed greater accuracy in recognizing neutral faces than emotional faces. These findings underscore the influence of fear of pain and available cognitive resources on the attentional processing of emotional faces over time, providing insights into how fear of pain affects the temporal dynamics of emotional face recognition and the role of attentional resources.

最近的研究主要关注怕痛症(FOP)患者对疼痛相关线索的空间注意偏向,但我们对其时间注意特征,尤其是对情绪面孔的时间注意特征的了解还很有限。为了弥补这一不足,39 名高度疼痛恐惧患者(H-FOP)和 37 名低度疼痛恐惧患者(L-FOP)完成了一项快速连续视觉呈现双重任务。受试者观看一系列快速呈现的图片,首先识别出中性建筑图像作为第一个目标(T1),然后识别出情绪面孔(痛苦、中性和快乐)作为第二个目标(T2)。T1 和 T2 的呈现时间间隔分别为 232 毫秒(滞后 2 秒)和 696 毫秒(滞后 6 秒)。结果表明,与 L-FOP 组的参与者相比,H-FOP 组的参与者在正确识别 T1 后不久,在滞后期 2 识别人脸的准确性较低。值得注意的是,与识别中性和快乐的人脸相比,所有参与者在滞后 2 期识别痛苦人脸的准确性都更高,这表明在注意资源有限的情况下,人们对痛苦刺激产生了负面偏向。相反,在注意资源更丰富的第 6 个滞后期,参与者识别中性面孔的准确性高于识别情绪面孔。这些发现强调了对疼痛的恐惧和可用认知资源对情绪化面孔的注意加工随时间变化的影响,为了解对疼痛的恐惧如何影响情绪化面孔识别的时间动态以及注意资源的作用提供了见解。
{"title":"The impact of fear of pain on the temporal processing of emotional faces: modulation of attentional resources.","authors":"Panpan Zheng, Zhenyong Lyu","doi":"10.1007/s10339-024-01243-8","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1007/s10339-024-01243-8","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Recent research has focused on the spatial attention bias toward pain-related cues in individuals with fear of pain (FOP), but our understanding of its temporal attention features, particularly regarding emotional faces, is limited. To address this gap, 39 individuals with high fear of pain (H-FOP) and 37 with low fear of pain (L-FOP) completed a rapid serial visual presentation dual task. Participants viewed a series of rapidly presented pictures, first identified neutral building images as the first target (T1) and then emotional faces (painful, neutral, and happy) as the second target (T2). T1 and T2 presented at time intervals of 232 ms (lag 2) and 696 ms (lag 6), respectively. Results indicate that H-FOP group members were less accurate in recognizing faces at lag 2 shortly after correctly identifying T1 compared to those L-FOP group members. Notably, all participants were more accurate in recognizing painful faces at lag 2 than in identifying neutral and happy faces, suggesting a negative bias toward pain stimuli when attentional resources were limited. In contrast, at lag 6, when attentional resources were more abundant, participants showed greater accuracy in recognizing neutral faces than emotional faces. These findings underscore the influence of fear of pain and available cognitive resources on the attentional processing of emotional faces over time, providing insights into how fear of pain affects the temporal dynamics of emotional face recognition and the role of attentional resources.</p>","PeriodicalId":47638,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Processing","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":1.7,"publicationDate":"2024-11-05","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142584678","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The effects of cognitive training on driving performance. 认知训练对驾驶性能的影响。
IF 1.7 4区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-11-04 DOI: 10.1007/s10339-024-01245-6
Jose L Tapia, David Sánchez-Borda, Jon Andoni Duñabeitia

Driving is a complex task necessitating an intricate interplay of sensory, motor, and cognitive abilities. Extensive research has underscored the role of neurocognitive functions, including attention, memory, executive functions, and visuospatial skills, in driving safety and performance. Despite evidence suggesting cognitive training's potential in enhancing driving abilities, comprehensive cognitive training's impact on driving performance in young adult drivers remains unexplored. Our study aimed to fill this gap by implementing an intensive, 8-week, multidomain computerized cognitive training program and assessing its transfer effects on the driving performance of young adult drivers, using a high-fidelity simulator. The study employed a randomized controlled trial design, with passive control group. The mixed-design analysis of variance (ANOVA) revealed a notable interaction between the time of testing and the respective participant groups concerning driving performance. Post hoc analyses showed that, compared to the control group, participants undergoing cognitive training demonstrated significantly fewer traffic infractions in the post-training evaluation. These findings suggest that cognitive training could be a useful tool for enhancing driving safety and performance in young adult drivers. Further research should aim to address the limitations posed by the absence of an active control group.

驾驶是一项复杂的任务,需要感官、运动和认知能力的密切配合。大量研究强调了神经认知功能在驾驶安全和驾驶表现中的作用,包括注意力、记忆力、执行功能和视觉空间技能。尽管有证据表明认知训练在提高驾驶能力方面具有潜力,但全面认知训练对年轻成年驾驶员驾驶表现的影响仍有待探索。我们的研究旨在填补这一空白,通过使用高保真模拟器,实施为期 8 周的强化多领域计算机化认知训练计划,并评估其对年轻成年驾驶员驾驶表现的转移效应。研究采用了随机对照试验设计,设有被动对照组。混合设计方差分析(ANOVA)显示,测试时间与各参与组的驾驶表现之间存在明显的交互作用。事后分析表明,与对照组相比,接受认知训练的学员在训练后评估中的交通违规行为明显减少。这些研究结果表明,认知训练是提高年轻成年驾驶员驾驶安全和驾驶表现的有效工具。进一步的研究应着眼于解决缺乏积极对照组所带来的局限性。
{"title":"The effects of cognitive training on driving performance.","authors":"Jose L Tapia, David Sánchez-Borda, Jon Andoni Duñabeitia","doi":"10.1007/s10339-024-01245-6","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1007/s10339-024-01245-6","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Driving is a complex task necessitating an intricate interplay of sensory, motor, and cognitive abilities. Extensive research has underscored the role of neurocognitive functions, including attention, memory, executive functions, and visuospatial skills, in driving safety and performance. Despite evidence suggesting cognitive training's potential in enhancing driving abilities, comprehensive cognitive training's impact on driving performance in young adult drivers remains unexplored. Our study aimed to fill this gap by implementing an intensive, 8-week, multidomain computerized cognitive training program and assessing its transfer effects on the driving performance of young adult drivers, using a high-fidelity simulator. The study employed a randomized controlled trial design, with passive control group. The mixed-design analysis of variance (ANOVA) revealed a notable interaction between the time of testing and the respective participant groups concerning driving performance. Post hoc analyses showed that, compared to the control group, participants undergoing cognitive training demonstrated significantly fewer traffic infractions in the post-training evaluation. These findings suggest that cognitive training could be a useful tool for enhancing driving safety and performance in young adult drivers. Further research should aim to address the limitations posed by the absence of an active control group.</p>","PeriodicalId":47638,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Processing","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":1.7,"publicationDate":"2024-11-04","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142570056","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Cognitive Processing
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1