首页 > 最新文献

Cognitive Processing最新文献

英文 中文
Mirror game as a tool to influence interpersonal spontaneous behavior after performance. 镜像游戏是一种影响表演后人际自发行为的工具。
IF 1.7 4区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-07-08 DOI: 10.1007/s10339-024-01205-0
Alexandre Coutté, Nicolas Margas, Loïc P Heurley

Mirror game (MG) is an exercise in which participants imitate each other. Our study explored its spontaneous behavioral consequences after performance. In a baseline (BL) phase, two participants performed a joint Simon task. Then, they performed a lure task during which we measured the interpersonal distance they spontaneously adopted. The BL phase was followed by two phases (in counterbalanced order). The MG phase started with a MG, before a procedure like the BL phase. The individual movement (IM) phase started with movements performed alone before a procedure like the BL phase. Interpersonal distance analysis suggested that MG enhanced spontaneous approach toward the partner, whereas IM induced spontaneous avoidance. Moreover, the joint Simon effect (JSE) tended to be smaller after IM, suggesting a decreasing inclination to integrate the partner's response in one's own action plan. Furthermore, in IM phase, JSE decreased as interpersonal distance increased.

镜像游戏(MG)是一种参与者相互模仿的练习。我们的研究探讨了它在表演后产生的自发行为后果。在基线(BL)阶段,两名参与者共同完成一项西蒙任务。然后,他们进行了一次引诱任务,在此期间,我们测量了他们自发采取的人际距离。基线阶段之后是两个阶段(按平衡顺序)。MG 阶段以 MG 开始,然后是类似 BL 阶段的程序。个人动作(IM)阶段则是在类似 BL 阶段的程序之前,先单独做动作。人际距离分析表明,MG 增强了对同伴的自发接近,而 IM 则诱发了自发回避。此外,联合西蒙效应(JSE)在 IM 阶段后趋于减弱,这表明将同伴的反应整合到自己的行动计划中的倾向在减弱。此外,在即时通讯阶段,随着人际距离的增加,联合西蒙效应也会降低。
{"title":"Mirror game as a tool to influence interpersonal spontaneous behavior after performance.","authors":"Alexandre Coutté, Nicolas Margas, Loïc P Heurley","doi":"10.1007/s10339-024-01205-0","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1007/s10339-024-01205-0","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Mirror game (MG) is an exercise in which participants imitate each other. Our study explored its spontaneous behavioral consequences after performance. In a baseline (BL) phase, two participants performed a joint Simon task. Then, they performed a lure task during which we measured the interpersonal distance they spontaneously adopted. The BL phase was followed by two phases (in counterbalanced order). The MG phase started with a MG, before a procedure like the BL phase. The individual movement (IM) phase started with movements performed alone before a procedure like the BL phase. Interpersonal distance analysis suggested that MG enhanced spontaneous approach toward the partner, whereas IM induced spontaneous avoidance. Moreover, the joint Simon effect (JSE) tended to be smaller after IM, suggesting a decreasing inclination to integrate the partner's response in one's own action plan. Furthermore, in IM phase, JSE decreased as interpersonal distance increased.</p>","PeriodicalId":47638,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Processing","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.7,"publicationDate":"2024-07-08","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141555719","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The concept of cognitive readiness: potential solution and opportunity for the Malaysian army. 认知准备概念:马来西亚军队的潜在解决方案和机遇。
IF 1.7 4区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-07-06 DOI: 10.1007/s10339-024-01203-2
Hasmady Alim, Amelia Yuliana Abd Wahab, Ananthan Subramaniam, Norazman Mohamad Nor

This paper focuses on exploring the potential solution and opportunity in the development of the Malaysian Army Transformation Plan by using the concept of Cognitive Readiness (CR). Here, the concept of CR equipped the military personnel to be cognitively ready to perform their role in military operations. The main aim of the paper is to highlight the fundamental discourse of 'what is cognitive readiness' in discovering the potential solution and opportunity in the development of the Malaysian Army Transformation Plan. The paper suggests that the strategy for transformation may start at the tactical level by focusing on enhancing the military personnel's CR. The study proposed that the Malaysian Army Organization prepare the military personnel with Knowledge, Skills and Abilities (KSA). KSA are important to boost the military personnel to have a distinctive character such as thinking critically, problem-solving and decision-making to perform effectively during military operations. In this preliminary study, the paper proposed a Framework for Tactical Cognitive Readiness (TCR) as a potential solution and opportunity for the Malaysian Army.

本文重点探讨马来西亚军队转型计划发展过程中使用认知准备(CR)概念的潜在解决方案和机遇。在这里,"认知准备 "的概念使军事人员在认知上做好准备,以便在军事行动中发挥作用。本文的主要目的是强调 "什么是认知准备 "这一基本论述,以发现马来西亚军队转型计划发展中的潜在解决方案和机遇。本文建议,转型战略可从战术层面入手,重点提高军事人员的 "客户关系 "能力。研究建议马来西亚陆军组织为军事人员准备知识、技能和能力(KSA)。知识、技能和能力对于提高军事人员在军事行动中有效执行任务所需的批判性思维、解决问题和决策等方面的独特能力非常重要。在这项初步研究中,本文提出了战术认知准备(TCR)框架,作为马来西亚军队的潜在解决方案和机遇。
{"title":"The concept of cognitive readiness: potential solution and opportunity for the Malaysian army.","authors":"Hasmady Alim, Amelia Yuliana Abd Wahab, Ananthan Subramaniam, Norazman Mohamad Nor","doi":"10.1007/s10339-024-01203-2","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1007/s10339-024-01203-2","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>This paper focuses on exploring the potential solution and opportunity in the development of the Malaysian Army Transformation Plan by using the concept of Cognitive Readiness (CR). Here, the concept of CR equipped the military personnel to be cognitively ready to perform their role in military operations. The main aim of the paper is to highlight the fundamental discourse of 'what is cognitive readiness' in discovering the potential solution and opportunity in the development of the Malaysian Army Transformation Plan. The paper suggests that the strategy for transformation may start at the tactical level by focusing on enhancing the military personnel's CR. The study proposed that the Malaysian Army Organization prepare the military personnel with Knowledge, Skills and Abilities (KSA). KSA are important to boost the military personnel to have a distinctive character such as thinking critically, problem-solving and decision-making to perform effectively during military operations. In this preliminary study, the paper proposed a Framework for Tactical Cognitive Readiness (TCR) as a potential solution and opportunity for the Malaysian Army.</p>","PeriodicalId":47638,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Processing","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.7,"publicationDate":"2024-07-06","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141545395","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
On the relationship between subjective decision criteria and paranoid ideations. 主观判断标准与妄想症之间的关系。
IF 1.7 4区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-06-26 DOI: 10.1007/s10339-024-01204-1
Svetoslav Bliznashki

Following the conjecture made by (Bliznashki and Hristova in Appetite 167:105645, 2021), we test the hypothesis that liberal subjective decision criteria exhibited during a task involving discrimination between random and systematically correlated patterns should be associated with elevated levels of paranoid ideations. Study 1 establishes the proposed association in the presence of several control measures while also demonstrating that the relationship in question is significantly moderated by subjects' working memory spans and tendencies to be overconfident in their judgments. Study 2 provides further evidence that these effects are indeed specific to tasks involving discrimination between random and systematic patterns and that the observed results are not due to some form of (anti) acquiescence bias or other general trends. Certain specifics of the correlation matrices involving cognitive measures significantly related to the paranoia continuum suggest that our results are consistent with the Entropic Brain Hypothesis. Finally, a simulation study employing a Neural Network demonstrates that increased entropy and liberal decision criteria might be connected to each other with said connection being amenable to an interpretation within the Bayesian paradigm.

根据(Bliznashki 和 Hristova,载于《食欲》167:105645,2021 年)的猜想,我们检验了这样一个假设:在一项涉及随机模式和系统相关模式之间判别的任务中,受试者表现出的宽松主观决策标准应与偏执想法水平的升高有关。研究 1 在几项控制测量中证实了所提出的关联,同时也证明了受试者的工作记忆时间跨度和过度自信的判断倾向对这种关系有显著的调节作用。研究 2 提供了进一步的证据,证明这些效应确实是特定于涉及随机和系统模式辨别的任务,而且观察到的结果并不是由于某种形式的(反)默许偏差或其他一般趋势造成的。涉及与妄想症连续性显著相关的认知测量的相关矩阵的某些细节表明,我们的结果与熵脑假说是一致的。最后,一项利用神经网络进行的模拟研究表明,熵增加与自由决策标准之间可能存在联系,这种联系可以在贝叶斯范式中得到解释。
{"title":"On the relationship between subjective decision criteria and paranoid ideations.","authors":"Svetoslav Bliznashki","doi":"10.1007/s10339-024-01204-1","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1007/s10339-024-01204-1","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Following the conjecture made by (Bliznashki and Hristova in Appetite 167:105645, 2021), we test the hypothesis that liberal subjective decision criteria exhibited during a task involving discrimination between random and systematically correlated patterns should be associated with elevated levels of paranoid ideations. Study 1 establishes the proposed association in the presence of several control measures while also demonstrating that the relationship in question is significantly moderated by subjects' working memory spans and tendencies to be overconfident in their judgments. Study 2 provides further evidence that these effects are indeed specific to tasks involving discrimination between random and systematic patterns and that the observed results are not due to some form of (anti) acquiescence bias or other general trends. Certain specifics of the correlation matrices involving cognitive measures significantly related to the paranoia continuum suggest that our results are consistent with the Entropic Brain Hypothesis. Finally, a simulation study employing a Neural Network demonstrates that increased entropy and liberal decision criteria might be connected to each other with said connection being amenable to an interpretation within the Bayesian paradigm.</p>","PeriodicalId":47638,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Processing","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.7,"publicationDate":"2024-06-26","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141451899","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
APOE genotype, hippocampal volume, and cognitive reserve predict improvement by cognitive training in older adults without dementia: a randomized controlled trial. APOE基因型、海马体积和认知储备可预测认知训练对无痴呆症老年人的改善:一项随机对照试验。
IF 1.7 4区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-06-19 DOI: 10.1007/s10339-024-01202-3
Pedro Montejo Carrasco, Mercedes Montenegro-Peña, David Prada Crespo, Inmaculada Rodríguez Rojo, Ana Barabash Bustelo, Borja Montejo Rubio, Alberto Marcos Dolado, Fernando Maestú Unturbe, María Luisa Delgado Losada

Cognitive training (CT) programs aim to improve cognitive performance and impede its decline. Thus, defining the characteristics of individuals who can benefit from these interventions is essential. Our objectives were to assess if the cognitive reserve (CR), APOE genotype (e4 carriers/non-carriers) and/or hippocampal volume might predict the effectiveness of a CT program. Participants were older adults without dementia (n = 226), randomized into parallel experimental and control groups. The assessment consisted of a neuropsychological protocol and additional data regarding total intracranial, gray matter, left/right hippocampus volume; APOE genotype; and Cognitive Reserve (CR). The intervention involved multifactorial CT (30 sessions, 90 min each), with an evaluation pre- and post-training (at six months); the control group simply following the center's routine activities. The primary outcome measures were the change in cognitive performance and the predictors of change. The results show that APOE-e4 non-carriers (79.1%) with a larger left hippocampal volume achieved better gains in semantic verbal fluency (R2 = .19). Subjects with a larger CR and a greater gray matter volume better improved their processing speed (R2 = .18). Age was correlated with the improvement in executive functions, such that older age predicts less improvement (R2 = .07). Subjects with a larger left hippocampal volume achieved more significant gains in general cognitive performance (R2 = .087). In conclusion, besides the program itself, the effectiveness of CT depends on age, biological factors like genotype and brain volume, and CR. Thus, to achieve better results through a CT, it is essential to consider the different characteristics of the participants, including genetic factors.Trial registration: Trial retrospectively registered on January 29th, 2020-(ClinicalTrials.gov -NCT04245579).

认知训练(CT)计划旨在提高认知能力并阻止其下降。因此,确定能从这些干预措施中受益的个体特征至关重要。我们的目标是评估认知储备(CR)、APOE基因型(e4携带者/非携带者)和/或海马体积是否能预测CT项目的效果。参与者为无痴呆症的老年人(n = 226),随机分为实验组和对照组。评估包括神经心理学方案和有关颅内总体积、灰质、左/右海马体积、APOE基因型和认知储备(CR)的其他数据。干预措施包括多因素 CT(30 次,每次 90 分钟),并在培训前后(6 个月时)进行评估;对照组只需参加中心的常规活动。主要结果指标是认知能力的变化和变化的预测因素。结果显示,左侧海马体积较大的非 APOE-e4 携带者(79.1%)在语义语言流畅性方面取得了更好的进步(R2 = 0.19)。CR较大且灰质体积较大的受试者在处理速度方面有更好的提高(R2 = 0.18)。年龄与执行功能的改善相关,年龄越大,改善越小(R2 = 0.07)。左侧海马体积较大的受试者在一般认知能力方面的提高更为显著(R2 = 0.087)。总之,除了程序本身,CT 的有效性还取决于年龄、基因型和脑容量等生物因素以及 CR。因此,要想通过 CT 取得更好的效果,必须考虑参与者的不同特点,包括遗传因素:试验于 2020 年 1 月 29 日进行了回顾性注册(ClinicalTrials.gov -NCT04245579)。
{"title":"APOE genotype, hippocampal volume, and cognitive reserve predict improvement by cognitive training in older adults without dementia: a randomized controlled trial.","authors":"Pedro Montejo Carrasco, Mercedes Montenegro-Peña, David Prada Crespo, Inmaculada Rodríguez Rojo, Ana Barabash Bustelo, Borja Montejo Rubio, Alberto Marcos Dolado, Fernando Maestú Unturbe, María Luisa Delgado Losada","doi":"10.1007/s10339-024-01202-3","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1007/s10339-024-01202-3","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Cognitive training (CT) programs aim to improve cognitive performance and impede its decline. Thus, defining the characteristics of individuals who can benefit from these interventions is essential. Our objectives were to assess if the cognitive reserve (CR), APOE genotype (e4 carriers/non-carriers) and/or hippocampal volume might predict the effectiveness of a CT program. Participants were older adults without dementia (n = 226), randomized into parallel experimental and control groups. The assessment consisted of a neuropsychological protocol and additional data regarding total intracranial, gray matter, left/right hippocampus volume; APOE genotype; and Cognitive Reserve (CR). The intervention involved multifactorial CT (30 sessions, 90 min each), with an evaluation pre- and post-training (at six months); the control group simply following the center's routine activities. The primary outcome measures were the change in cognitive performance and the predictors of change. The results show that APOE-e4 non-carriers (79.1%) with a larger left hippocampal volume achieved better gains in semantic verbal fluency (R<sup>2</sup> = .19). Subjects with a larger CR and a greater gray matter volume better improved their processing speed (R<sup>2</sup> = .18). Age was correlated with the improvement in executive functions, such that older age predicts less improvement (R<sup>2</sup> = .07). Subjects with a larger left hippocampal volume achieved more significant gains in general cognitive performance (R<sup>2</sup> = .087). In conclusion, besides the program itself, the effectiveness of CT depends on age, biological factors like genotype and brain volume, and CR. Thus, to achieve better results through a CT, it is essential to consider the different characteristics of the participants, including genetic factors.Trial registration: Trial retrospectively registered on January 29th, 2020-(ClinicalTrials.gov -NCT04245579).</p>","PeriodicalId":47638,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Processing","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.7,"publicationDate":"2024-06-19","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141421405","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Mental simulation of colour properties during language comprehension: influence of context and comprehension stages. 语言理解过程中对颜色属性的心理模拟:语境和理解阶段的影响。
IF 1.7 4区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-06-08 DOI: 10.1007/s10339-024-01201-4
Donglin Liu, Lijuan Wang, Ying Han

Many studies have shown that mental simulation may occur during language comprehension. Supporting evidence is derived from the matching effects in the sentence-picture verification (SPV) task often used to assess mental simulations of object properties, such as size, orientation, and shape. However, mixed results have been obtained regarding object colour, with researchers reporting matching or mismatching effects. This study investigated the impact of colour information clarity within sentences on the process of mental simulation during language comprehension. Employing the SPV task and using novel objects, we examined whether there is a mental simulation of colour after excluding typical/atypical colour bias and how varying levels of colour information clarity in sentences influence the emergence of matching effects at different stages of comprehension. To address these issues, we conducted two experiments. In Experiment 1, the participants read normal sentences and subsequently engaged in picture verification with a novel object after a 500 ms delay. In Experiment 2, the participants encountered sentences containing both clear and unclear colour information and, after either a 0 ms or 1500 ms interval, completed picture verification tasks with a novel object. Null effects were found in the 500 ms condition for normal sentences and the 0 ms condition for unclear colour information sentences. A mismatching effect appeared in the 0 ms condition after clear colour information sentences, and a matching effect appeared in the 1500 ms condition for all sentences. The results indicated that after excluding colour bias, the participants still formed mental simulations of colour during language comprehension. Our results also indicated that ongoing colour simulation with time pressure impacted the participant responses. The participants ignored unclear colour information under time pressure, but without time pressure, they constructed simulations that were as detailed as possible, regardless of whether the implicit colour information in the sentence was clear.

许多研究表明,心理模拟可能发生在语言理解过程中。句子-图片验证(SPV)任务中的匹配效应就是佐证,该任务通常用于评估对物体属性(如大小、方向和形状)的心理模拟。然而,关于物体颜色的研究结果却不尽相同,研究人员报告了匹配或不匹配效应。本研究调查了句子中颜色信息的清晰度对语言理解过程中心理模拟的影响。我们采用 SPV 任务并使用新颖的对象,考察了在排除典型/典型颜色偏差后是否存在对颜色的心理模拟,以及句子中不同程度的颜色信息清晰度如何影响不同理解阶段匹配效应的出现。为了解决这些问题,我们进行了两项实验。在实验 1 中,被试阅读正常句子,然后在延迟 500 毫秒后与一个新物体进行图片验证。在实验 2 中,被试阅读了包含清晰和不清晰颜色信息的句子,在 0 毫秒或 1500 毫秒的间隔后,完成了与新物体的图片验证任务。在 500 毫秒条件下,正常句子出现无效效应;在 0 毫秒条件下,颜色信息不清晰的句子出现无效效应。颜色信息清晰的句子在 0 毫秒条件下出现了不匹配效应,而所有句子在 1500 毫秒条件下都出现了匹配效应。结果表明,在排除了颜色偏差之后,被试在语言理解过程中仍然形成了对颜色的心理模拟。我们的结果还表明,时间压力下的持续颜色模拟影响了被试的反应。在时间压力下,被试忽略了不明确的颜色信息,但在没有时间压力的情况下,无论句子中的隐含颜色信息是否明确,被试都会构建尽可能详细的模拟。
{"title":"Mental simulation of colour properties during language comprehension: influence of context and comprehension stages.","authors":"Donglin Liu, Lijuan Wang, Ying Han","doi":"10.1007/s10339-024-01201-4","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1007/s10339-024-01201-4","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Many studies have shown that mental simulation may occur during language comprehension. Supporting evidence is derived from the matching effects in the sentence-picture verification (SPV) task often used to assess mental simulations of object properties, such as size, orientation, and shape. However, mixed results have been obtained regarding object colour, with researchers reporting matching or mismatching effects. This study investigated the impact of colour information clarity within sentences on the process of mental simulation during language comprehension. Employing the SPV task and using novel objects, we examined whether there is a mental simulation of colour after excluding typical/atypical colour bias and how varying levels of colour information clarity in sentences influence the emergence of matching effects at different stages of comprehension. To address these issues, we conducted two experiments. In Experiment 1, the participants read normal sentences and subsequently engaged in picture verification with a novel object after a 500 ms delay. In Experiment 2, the participants encountered sentences containing both clear and unclear colour information and, after either a 0 ms or 1500 ms interval, completed picture verification tasks with a novel object. Null effects were found in the 500 ms condition for normal sentences and the 0 ms condition for unclear colour information sentences. A mismatching effect appeared in the 0 ms condition after clear colour information sentences, and a matching effect appeared in the 1500 ms condition for all sentences. The results indicated that after excluding colour bias, the participants still formed mental simulations of colour during language comprehension. Our results also indicated that ongoing colour simulation with time pressure impacted the participant responses. The participants ignored unclear colour information under time pressure, but without time pressure, they constructed simulations that were as detailed as possible, regardless of whether the implicit colour information in the sentence was clear.</p>","PeriodicalId":47638,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Processing","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.7,"publicationDate":"2024-06-08","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141293839","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Cognitive entailments among "the true, the good, the beautiful": a mainland Chinese sample. 真善美 "之间的认知蕴涵:一个中国大陆样本。
IF 1.7 4区 心理学 Q3 Computer Science Pub Date : 2024-05-29 DOI: 10.1007/s10339-024-01200-5
Xinchi Yu, Ernst Pöppel, Weidong Zhan, Yan Bao

Philosophers and cognitive scientists have long debated about the entailments among "the true, the good, the beautiful" (TGB hereafter). In the current article, we directly probed mainland Chinese subjects' cognitive entailment among TGB. Using 1-7 (Experiment 1) and 1-6 (Experiment 2) Likert scales, we convergently observed that mainland Chinese subjects tend to think that the beautiful is not the true, and that the good is the beautiful. Additionally, Experiment 1 also revealed that mainland Chinese subjects tend to think that the true is not the beautiful. Some of these results may reflect anthropological universals, and some others may reflect cultural specifics. Experiment 3 revealed that the most popular translation of TGB in Chinese into English is rather "the true, the kind, the beautiful", suggesting that the three concepts mapped to TGB in Chinese is not one-to-one mapped to the three concepts mapped to TGB in English. Therefore, caution should be exercised when making cross-linguistic or cross-cultural comparisons about TGB in the future.

关于 "真善美"(TGB)之间的蕴涵,哲学家和认知科学家们争论已久。本文直接探究了中国大陆被试对 "真、善、美 "的认知蕴涵。通过1-7(实验1)和1-6(实验2)的李克特量表,我们发现中国大陆被试倾向于认为 "美 "不是 "真","善 "就是 "美"。此外,实验 1 还显示,中国大陆受试者倾向于认为 "真 "不是 "美"。这些结果有些可能反映了人类学的普遍性,有些可能反映了文化的特殊性。实验 3 显示,TGB 在汉语中最流行的英译是 "the true, the kind, the beautiful",这表明汉语中映射到 TGB 的三个概念与英语中映射到 TGB 的三个概念并不是一一对应的。因此,今后在对 TGB 进行跨语言或跨文化比较时应谨慎行事。
{"title":"Cognitive entailments among \"the true, the good, the beautiful\": a mainland Chinese sample.","authors":"Xinchi Yu, Ernst Pöppel, Weidong Zhan, Yan Bao","doi":"10.1007/s10339-024-01200-5","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1007/s10339-024-01200-5","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Philosophers and cognitive scientists have long debated about the entailments among \"the true, the good, the beautiful\" (TGB hereafter). In the current article, we directly probed mainland Chinese subjects' cognitive entailment among TGB. Using 1-7 (Experiment 1) and 1-6 (Experiment 2) Likert scales, we convergently observed that mainland Chinese subjects tend to think that the beautiful is not the true, and that the good is the beautiful. Additionally, Experiment 1 also revealed that mainland Chinese subjects tend to think that the true is not the beautiful. Some of these results may reflect anthropological universals, and some others may reflect cultural specifics. Experiment 3 revealed that the most popular translation of TGB in Chinese into English is rather \"the true, the kind, the beautiful\", suggesting that the three concepts mapped to TGB in Chinese is not one-to-one mapped to the three concepts mapped to TGB in English. Therefore, caution should be exercised when making cross-linguistic or cross-cultural comparisons about TGB in the future.</p>","PeriodicalId":47638,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Processing","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.7,"publicationDate":"2024-05-29","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141176655","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Reading abilities in preterm children: a systematic review and meta-analysis. 早产儿的阅读能力:系统回顾和荟萃分析。
IF 1.7 4区 心理学 Q3 Computer Science Pub Date : 2024-05-16 DOI: 10.1007/s10339-024-01192-2
Mónica Gutiérrez-Ortega, Aitor Álvarez-Bardón, Esperanza Vergara-Moragues, Javier Tubío, Alejandro González-Andrade

It is well recognized that children born preterm have specific learning difficulties. The objective of this study is to carry out a systematic review and meta-analysis on preterm children's reading ability performance. Of the 486 studies identified, 53 met the inclusion criteria, resulting in 143 effect sizes. 33,500 children between 5 and 18 years were included of whom 13,765 were preterm infants and 19,735 were full-term infants. Among preterm-born children without neurodevelopmental disorders significant differences and larger effect sizes were found in the reading subcategories, letter-word knowledge, reading comprehension, and spelling, whereas no significant differences were found in phonological awareness and rapid automatized naming. Extremely preterm children showed larger effect size. The present meta-analysis, which includes a much larger number of studies, shows that preterm children have lower performance than full-term children in reading and spelling. This fact underlines the need to design, develop and implement neuroeducational programs that take into account the characteristics of preterm born students.

早产儿有特殊的学习困难,这一点已得到公认。本研究旨在对早产儿的阅读能力表现进行系统回顾和荟萃分析。在已确定的 486 项研究中,有 53 项符合纳入标准,产生了 143 个效应大小。共纳入 33500 名 5 至 18 岁的儿童,其中早产儿 13765 名,足月儿 19735 名。在没有神经发育障碍的早产儿中,在阅读子类别、字母-单词知识、阅读理解和拼写方面发现了显著差异和更大的效应大小,而在语音意识和快速自动命名方面则没有发现显著差异。极早产儿的效应大小更大。本荟萃分析包括了更多的研究,表明早产儿在阅读和拼写方面的表现低于足月儿。这一事实强调了设计、开发和实施考虑到早产儿特点的神经教育计划的必要性。
{"title":"Reading abilities in preterm children: a systematic review and meta-analysis.","authors":"Mónica Gutiérrez-Ortega, Aitor Álvarez-Bardón, Esperanza Vergara-Moragues, Javier Tubío, Alejandro González-Andrade","doi":"10.1007/s10339-024-01192-2","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1007/s10339-024-01192-2","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>It is well recognized that children born preterm have specific learning difficulties. The objective of this study is to carry out a systematic review and meta-analysis on preterm children's reading ability performance. Of the 486 studies identified, 53 met the inclusion criteria, resulting in 143 effect sizes. 33,500 children between 5 and 18 years were included of whom 13,765 were preterm infants and 19,735 were full-term infants. Among preterm-born children without neurodevelopmental disorders significant differences and larger effect sizes were found in the reading subcategories, letter-word knowledge, reading comprehension, and spelling, whereas no significant differences were found in phonological awareness and rapid automatized naming. Extremely preterm children showed larger effect size. The present meta-analysis, which includes a much larger number of studies, shows that preterm children have lower performance than full-term children in reading and spelling. This fact underlines the need to design, develop and implement neuroeducational programs that take into account the characteristics of preterm born students.</p>","PeriodicalId":47638,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Processing","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.7,"publicationDate":"2024-05-16","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140946216","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Sex/gender differences in general cognitive abilities: an investigation using the Leiter-3. 一般认知能力的性别差异:使用 Leiter-3 进行的调查。
IF 1.7 4区 心理学 Q3 Computer Science Pub Date : 2024-05-15 DOI: 10.1007/s10339-024-01199-9
David Giofrè, Enrico Toffalini, Lorenzo Esposito, Cesare Cornoldi

Research has shown that differences between males and females are not in general intelligence, but only in some specific factors and tasks. We used the Italian standardization of the Leiter-3, which is a completely nonverbal cognitive battery, to investigate the nature of sex/gender (we used sex/gender to reflect the awareness that the effects of biological 'sex' and socially constructed 'gender' cannot be easily separated and that most individuals' identities are informed by both sex and gender) differences. In doing so we used a multigroup confirmatory factor analysis approach. Results confirmed that males and females perform similarly in general intelligence, but present with some specificities. Males perform better on some, but not all, tasks requiring the spatial manipulation of the stimuli, but females consistently outperform males in tasks such as the nonverbal Stroop, requiring inhibition and attention control to a larger extent. The clinical and practical implications of our findings are considerable. The identification of specific cognitive strengths and weaknesses in males and females underscores the importance of tailored approaches in clinical assessments and interventions.

研究表明,男性和女性之间的差异不在于一般智力,而在于某些特定因素和任务。我们利用意大利的莱特-3(Leiter-3)标准化测验(这是一个完全非言语的认知测验)来研究性/性别差异的本质(我们使用性/性别来反映生物 "性 "和社会构建的 "性别 "的影响不容易分开,大多数人的身份都同时受到性和性别的影响)。为此,我们采用了多组确认性因素分析方法。结果证实,男性和女性在一般智力方面的表现相似,但存在一些特殊性。男性在一些(但不是所有)需要对刺激物进行空间操作的任务中表现更好,但女性在非言语 Stroop 等任务中的表现一直优于男性,这些任务对抑制和注意力控制的要求更高。我们的研究结果具有重要的临床和实践意义。我们发现了男性和女性在认知方面的具体强项和弱项,这凸显了在临床评估和干预中采取有针对性的方法的重要性。
{"title":"Sex/gender differences in general cognitive abilities: an investigation using the Leiter-3.","authors":"David Giofrè, Enrico Toffalini, Lorenzo Esposito, Cesare Cornoldi","doi":"10.1007/s10339-024-01199-9","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1007/s10339-024-01199-9","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Research has shown that differences between males and females are not in general intelligence, but only in some specific factors and tasks. We used the Italian standardization of the Leiter-3, which is a completely nonverbal cognitive battery, to investigate the nature of sex/gender (we used sex/gender to reflect the awareness that the effects of biological 'sex' and socially constructed 'gender' cannot be easily separated and that most individuals' identities are informed by both sex and gender) differences. In doing so we used a multigroup confirmatory factor analysis approach. Results confirmed that males and females perform similarly in general intelligence, but present with some specificities. Males perform better on some, but not all, tasks requiring the spatial manipulation of the stimuli, but females consistently outperform males in tasks such as the nonverbal Stroop, requiring inhibition and attention control to a larger extent. The clinical and practical implications of our findings are considerable. The identification of specific cognitive strengths and weaknesses in males and females underscores the importance of tailored approaches in clinical assessments and interventions.</p>","PeriodicalId":47638,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Processing","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.7,"publicationDate":"2024-05-15","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140923186","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Practice-induced SNARC: evidence from a null-SNARC sample. 实践引起的 SNARC:来自无效 SNARC 样本的证据。
IF 1.7 4区 心理学 Q3 Computer Science Pub Date : 2024-05-13 DOI: 10.1007/s10339-024-01198-w
Ezgi Palaz, Hakan Çetinkaya, Zeynep Tuncali, Bengi Kamar, Seda Dural

The mental representation of numbers inherently involves a spatial organization, often positioning smaller numbers to the left and larger numbers to the right. The SNARC effect, characterized by faster responses to small numbers using the left hand and vice versa for large numbers, is typically attributed to this left-to-right oriented mental number line (MNL). However, the direction of the SNARC effect seems to rely on reading direction, with most research exploring these mechanisms conducted within left-to-right reading cultures where the SNARC effect is prevalent. This study takes advantage of a sample from a left-to-right reading culture that does not exhibit the SNARC effect, allowing us to isolate and elucidate the stand-alone effects of recent experiences on SNARC. Therefore, the current study aims to investigate how MNL-compatible and MNL-incompatible practices induce an effect within a sample lacking the SNARC effect. To accomplish this, we reinvited the individuals from the sample which had previously shown no SNARC, and retested those who agreed to take part in the current study after an MNL-compatible or MNL-incompatible practice manipulation. The findings revealed an absence of the SNARC effect with MNL-compatible practices. Conversely, MNL-incompatible practices yielded a reverse SNARC effect. These results prompt a discussion on SNARC mechanisms within the framework of practice effects.

数字的心理表征本质上涉及一种空间组织,通常将较小的数字放在左边,较大的数字放在右边。SNARC效应的特点是用左手对小数字反应较快,反之则对大数字反应较慢,这种效应通常归因于这种从左到右的心理数字线(MNL)。然而,SNARC 效应的方向似乎取决于阅读方向,大多数探索这些机制的研究都是在 SNARC 效应盛行的从左到右阅读文化中进行的。本研究利用了从左到右阅读文化中的样本,这种文化并不表现出 SNARC 效应,这使我们能够分离并阐明近期经验对 SNARC 的独立影响。因此,本研究旨在调查与 MNL 兼容和与 MNL 不兼容的练习如何在缺乏 SNARC 效应的样本中产生效应。为了达到这一目的,我们重新邀请了之前没有表现出SNARC的样本中的个体,并在进行了MNL兼容或MNL不兼容练习操作后,对那些同意参加当前研究的个体进行了重新测试。结果显示,与 MNL 兼容的练习没有 SNARC 效应。相反,与 MNL 不兼容的练习则产生了相反的 SNARC 效应。这些结果促使人们在练习效应的框架内讨论 SNARC 机制。
{"title":"Practice-induced SNARC: evidence from a null-SNARC sample.","authors":"Ezgi Palaz, Hakan Çetinkaya, Zeynep Tuncali, Bengi Kamar, Seda Dural","doi":"10.1007/s10339-024-01198-w","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1007/s10339-024-01198-w","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The mental representation of numbers inherently involves a spatial organization, often positioning smaller numbers to the left and larger numbers to the right. The SNARC effect, characterized by faster responses to small numbers using the left hand and vice versa for large numbers, is typically attributed to this left-to-right oriented mental number line (MNL). However, the direction of the SNARC effect seems to rely on reading direction, with most research exploring these mechanisms conducted within left-to-right reading cultures where the SNARC effect is prevalent. This study takes advantage of a sample from a left-to-right reading culture that does not exhibit the SNARC effect, allowing us to isolate and elucidate the stand-alone effects of recent experiences on SNARC. Therefore, the current study aims to investigate how MNL-compatible and MNL-incompatible practices induce an effect within a sample lacking the SNARC effect. To accomplish this, we reinvited the individuals from the sample which had previously shown no SNARC, and retested those who agreed to take part in the current study after an MNL-compatible or MNL-incompatible practice manipulation. The findings revealed an absence of the SNARC effect with MNL-compatible practices. Conversely, MNL-incompatible practices yielded a reverse SNARC effect. These results prompt a discussion on SNARC mechanisms within the framework of practice effects.</p>","PeriodicalId":47638,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Processing","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.7,"publicationDate":"2024-05-13","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140910894","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The influence of face mask color on perceptions of African American and white men. 面罩颜色对非裔美国人和白人男性看法的影响。
IF 1.7 4区 心理学 Q3 Computer Science Pub Date : 2024-05-10 DOI: 10.1007/s10339-024-01196-y
Lauren A Morris, Doris G Bazzini, Christopher J Holden, Savannah J Lee

Despite their widespread use during the COVID-19 pandemic, face masks hinder abilities to interpret facial expressions. Yet, they can also reduce the appearance of characteristics that are used to categorize individuals into racial groups, such as Afrocentric features. The color of a face mask might also promote associations with certain types of behavior and professional occupations (e.g., blue surgical mask connoting physician stereotypes; black masks potentially being associated with criminality). This study assessed whether the presence and color of a face mask impacted perceptions of a target male of varying race. White participants (N = 250) were presented with an African American or White male adult face from the Chicago Face Database (of equal age and attractiveness) wearing a blue or black surgical mask, or no mask (Photoshopped onto the face) and rated the man on emotions (happy, sad, angry) as well as how trustworthy, threatening, and attractive the target appeared. Targets wearing a blue surgical mask were judged as more trustworthy and attractive than those wearing no mask (perhaps due to association with medical professions), but these judgements were not qualified by race, despite the African American target's selection based on Afrocentric features. The color black on a face mask did not exacerbate negative perceptions of targets, perhaps suggesting a decline effect in previously demonstrated associations between this color and criminal actions. Unlike previous research performed at the beginning of the Covid-19 Pandemic on cloth masks shown to potentially exacerbate racial biases, surgical masks (pleated and made of polymeric materials), appear to lessen potential stereotyping of Black relative to White men.

尽管在 COVID-19 大流行期间口罩被广泛使用,但口罩妨碍了解读面部表情的能力。然而,它们也会减少用于将个人划分为种族群体的特征的出现,如非洲裔特征。口罩的颜色还可能会让人联想到某些类型的行为和专业职业(例如,蓝色外科口罩意味着对医生的刻板印象;黑色口罩可能与犯罪有关)。本研究评估了口罩的存在和颜色是否会影响人们对不同种族男性目标人物的看法。白人参与者(N = 250)会看到一张来自芝加哥脸部数据库的非裔美国人或白人成年男性面孔(年龄和吸引力相同),面孔上戴着蓝色或黑色外科口罩,或没有戴口罩(Photoshop 合成的面孔),参与者会对该男子的情绪(快乐、悲伤、愤怒)以及目标人物的可信度、威胁性和吸引力进行评分。戴蓝色外科口罩的目标比不戴口罩的目标更值得信赖和更有吸引力(可能是由于与医学专业有关),但这些判断不受种族影响,尽管非裔美国人目标的选择是基于非洲裔特征。面罩上的黑色并没有加剧人们对目标的负面印象,这或许表明,之前证明的这种颜色与犯罪行为之间的关联性出现了下降效应。与之前在 19 世纪科维德大流行初期进行的关于布制口罩的研究不同,外科口罩(褶皱和聚合材料制成)可能会加剧种族偏见,而相对于白人男性,黑人口罩似乎减少了对黑人的潜在刻板印象。
{"title":"The influence of face mask color on perceptions of African American and white men.","authors":"Lauren A Morris, Doris G Bazzini, Christopher J Holden, Savannah J Lee","doi":"10.1007/s10339-024-01196-y","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1007/s10339-024-01196-y","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Despite their widespread use during the COVID-19 pandemic, face masks hinder abilities to interpret facial expressions. Yet, they can also reduce the appearance of characteristics that are used to categorize individuals into racial groups, such as Afrocentric features. The color of a face mask might also promote associations with certain types of behavior and professional occupations (e.g., blue surgical mask connoting physician stereotypes; black masks potentially being associated with criminality). This study assessed whether the presence and color of a face mask impacted perceptions of a target male of varying race. White participants (N = 250) were presented with an African American or White male adult face from the Chicago Face Database (of equal age and attractiveness) wearing a blue or black surgical mask, or no mask (Photoshopped onto the face) and rated the man on emotions (happy, sad, angry) as well as how trustworthy, threatening, and attractive the target appeared. Targets wearing a blue surgical mask were judged as more trustworthy and attractive than those wearing no mask (perhaps due to association with medical professions), but these judgements were not qualified by race, despite the African American target's selection based on Afrocentric features. The color black on a face mask did not exacerbate negative perceptions of targets, perhaps suggesting a decline effect in previously demonstrated associations between this color and criminal actions. Unlike previous research performed at the beginning of the Covid-19 Pandemic on cloth masks shown to potentially exacerbate racial biases, surgical masks (pleated and made of polymeric materials), appear to lessen potential stereotyping of Black relative to White men.</p>","PeriodicalId":47638,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Processing","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.7,"publicationDate":"2024-05-10","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140897324","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Cognitive Processing
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1